1
|
Basnak J, Ota M. Learnability Advantage of Segmental Repetitions in Word Learning. LANGUAGE AND SPEECH 2024; 67:1093-1120. [PMID: 38312096 PMCID: PMC11583519 DOI: 10.1177/00238309231223909] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/06/2024]
Abstract
To date, research on wordform learning biases has mostly focused on language-dependent factors, such as the phonotactics and neighborhood density of the language(s) known by the learner. Domain-general biases, by contrast, have received little attention. In this study, we focus on one such bias-an advantage for string-internal repetitions-and examine its effects on wordform learning. Importantly, we consider whether any type of segmental repetition is equally beneficial for word recall, or whether learning is favored more or only by repeated consonants, in line with previous research indicating that consonants play a larger role than vowels in lexical processing. In Experiment 1, adult English speakers learned artificial consonant-vowel-consonant-vowel words containing either a repeated consonant (e.g., /sesu/, "c-rep"), a repeated vowel (e.g., /sepe/, "v-rep"), or dissimilar consonants and vowels (e.g., /sepu/, "no-rep"). Recall results showed no advantage for v-reps but higher accuracy for c-reps compared with no-reps. In Experiment 2, participants performed a label preference task with the same stimuli. The results showed dispreference for both c-reps and v-reps relative to no-reps, indicating that the results of Experiment 1 are independent of wordlikeness effects. These outcomes reveal that there is a form-learning bias for words with identical consonants but not for words with identical vowels, suggesting that a domain-general advantage for repetitions within strings is modulated by a language-specific processing bias for consonants.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Johanna Basnak
- School of Philosophy, Psychology & Language Sciences, The University of Edinburgh, UK
| | - Mitsuhiko Ota
- School of Philosophy, Psychology & Language Sciences, The University of Edinburgh, UK
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Bettoni R, Riva V, Molteni M, Macchi Cassia V, Bulf H, Cantiani C. Rules generalization in children with dyslexia. RESEARCH IN DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES 2024; 146:104673. [PMID: 38280272 DOI: 10.1016/j.ridd.2024.104673] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/11/2023] [Revised: 12/29/2023] [Accepted: 01/08/2024] [Indexed: 01/29/2024]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Rule learning (RL) is the ability to extract and generalize higher-order repetition-based structures. Children with Developmental Dyslexia (DD) often report difficulties in learning complex regularities in sequential stimuli, which might be due to the complexity of the rule to be learned. Learning high-order repetition-based rules represents a building block for the development of language skills. AIMS This study investigates the ability to extract and generalize simple, repetition-based visual rules (e.g., ABA) in 8-11-year-old children without (TD) and with a diagnosis of Development Dyslexia (DD) and its relationship with language and reading skills. METHOD Using a forced-choice paradigm, children were first exposed to a visual sequence containing a repetition-based rule (e.g., ABA) and were then asked to recognize familiar and novel rules generated by new visual elements. Standardized language and reading tests were also administered to both groups. RESULTS The accuracy in recognizing rules was above chance for both groups, even though DD children were less accurate than TD children, suggesting a less efficient RL mechanism in the DD group. Moreover, visual RL was positively correlated with both language and reading skills. CONCLUSION These results further confirm the crucial role of RL in the acquisition of linguistic skills and mastering reading abilities.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Roberta Bettoni
- Department of Psychology, University of Milano-Bicocca, Milan, Italy.
| | - Valentina Riva
- Child Psychopathology Unit, Scientific Institute, IRCCS E. Medea, Bosisio Parini, Lecco, Italy
| | - Massimo Molteni
- Child Psychopathology Unit, Scientific Institute, IRCCS E. Medea, Bosisio Parini, Lecco, Italy
| | | | - Hermann Bulf
- Department of Psychology, University of Milano-Bicocca, Milan, Italy
| | - Chiara Cantiani
- Child Psychopathology Unit, Scientific Institute, IRCCS E. Medea, Bosisio Parini, Lecco, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Avcu E, Gow D. Exploring Abstract Pattern Representation in The Brain and Non-symbolic Neural Networks. BIORXIV : THE PREPRINT SERVER FOR BIOLOGY 2023:2023.11.27.568877. [PMID: 38076846 PMCID: PMC10705297 DOI: 10.1101/2023.11.27.568877] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/24/2023]
Abstract
Human cognitive and linguistic generativity depends on the ability to identify abstract relationships between perceptually dissimilar items. Marcus et al. (1999) found that human infants can rapidly discover and generalize patterns of syllable repetition (reduplication) that depend on the abstract property of identity, but simple recurrent neural networks (SRNs) could not. They interpreted these results as evidence that purely associative neural network models provide an inadequate framework for characterizing the fundamental generativity of human cognition. Here, we present a series of deep long short-term memory (LSTM) models that identify abstract syllable repetition patterns and words based on training with cochleagrams that represent auditory stimuli. We demonstrate that models trained to identify individual syllable trigram words and models trained to identify reduplication patterns discover representations that support classification of abstract repetition patterns. Simulations examined the effects of training categories (words vs. patterns) and pretraining to identify syllables, on the development of hidden node representations that support repetition pattern discrimination. Representational similarity analyses (RSA) comparing patterns of regional brain activity based on MRI-constrained MEG/EEG data to patterns of hidden node activation elicited by the same stimuli showed a significant correlation between brain activity localized in primarily posterior temporal regions and representations discovered by the models. These results suggest that associative mechanisms operating over discoverable representations that capture abstract stimulus properties account for a critical example of human cognitive generativity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Enes Avcu
- Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Cambridge, MA 02170
| | - David Gow
- Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Cambridge, MA 02170
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Gow DW, Avcu E, Schoenhaut A, Sorensen DO, Ahlfors SP. Abstract representations in temporal cortex support generative linguistic processing. LANGUAGE, COGNITION AND NEUROSCIENCE 2022; 38:765-778. [PMID: 37332658 PMCID: PMC10270390 DOI: 10.1080/23273798.2022.2157029] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/08/2022] [Accepted: 11/21/2022] [Indexed: 06/20/2023]
Abstract
Generativity, the ability to create and evaluate novel constructions, is a fundamental property of human language and cognition. The productivity of generative processes is determined by the scope of the representations they engage. Here we examine the neural representation of reduplication, a productive phonological process that can create novel forms through patterned syllable copying (e.g. ba-mih → ba-ba-mih, ba-mih-mih, or ba-mih-ba). Using MRI-constrained source estimates of combined MEG/EEG data collected during an auditory artificial grammar task, we identified localized cortical activity associated with syllable reduplication pattern contrasts in novel trisyllabic nonwords. Neural decoding analyses identified a set of predominantly right hemisphere temporal lobe regions whose activity reliably discriminated reduplication patterns evoked by untrained, novel stimuli. Effective connectivity analyses suggested that sensitivity to abstracted reduplication patterns was propagated between these temporal regions. These results suggest that localized temporal lobe activity patterns function as abstract representations that support linguistic generativity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- David W. Gow
- Department of Neurology Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School; Boston, MA, 02114
- Department of Psychology, Salem State University; Salem, MA, 01970
- Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital; Charlestown, MA, 02129
- Program in Speech and Hearing Bioscience and Technology, Division of Medical Sciences, Harvard Medical School; Boston, MA 02115
| | - Enes Avcu
- Department of Neurology Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School; Boston, MA, 02114
| | - Adriana Schoenhaut
- Department of Neurology Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School; Boston, MA, 02114
| | - David O. Sorensen
- Program in Speech and Hearing Bioscience and Technology, Division of Medical Sciences, Harvard Medical School; Boston, MA 02115
| | - Seppo P. Ahlfors
- Program in Speech and Hearing Bioscience and Technology, Division of Medical Sciences, Harvard Medical School; Boston, MA 02115
- Department of Radiology, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School; Boston, MA, 02114
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Abstract
Vision and learning have long been considered to be two areas of research linked only distantly. However, recent developments in vision research have changed the conceptual definition of vision from a signal-evaluating process to a goal-oriented interpreting process, and this shift binds learning, together with the resulting internal representations, intimately to vision. In this review, we consider various types of learning (perceptual, statistical, and rule/abstract) associated with vision in the past decades and argue that they represent differently specialized versions of the fundamental learning process, which must be captured in its entirety when applied to complex visual processes. We show why the generalized version of statistical learning can provide the appropriate setup for such a unified treatment of learning in vision, what computational framework best accommodates this kind of statistical learning, and what plausible neural scheme could feasibly implement this framework. Finally, we list the challenges that the field of statistical learning faces in fulfilling the promise of being the right vehicle for advancing our understanding of vision in its entirety. Expected final online publication date for the Annual Review of Vision Science, Volume 8 is September 2022. Please see http://www.annualreviews.org/page/journal/pubdates for revised estimates.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- József Fiser
- Department of Cognitive Science, Center for Cognitive Computation, Central European University, Vienna 1100, Austria;
| | - Gábor Lengyel
- Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, University of Rochester, Rochester, New York 14627, USA
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Musical instrument familiarity affects statistical learning of tone sequences. Cognition 2021; 218:104949. [PMID: 34768123 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104949] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/25/2020] [Revised: 06/22/2021] [Accepted: 10/28/2021] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
Abstract
Most listeners have an implicit understanding of the rules that govern how music unfolds over time. This knowledge is acquired in part through statistical learning, a robust learning mechanism that allows individuals to extract regularities from the environment. However, it is presently unclear how this prior musical knowledge might facilitate or interfere with the learning of novel tone sequences that do not conform to familiar musical rules. In the present experiment, participants listened to novel, statistically structured tone sequences composed of pitch intervals not typically found in Western music. Between participants, the tone sequences either had the timbre of artificial, computerized instruments or familiar instruments (piano or violin). Knowledge of the statistical regularities was measured as by a two-alternative forced choice recognition task, requiring discrimination between novel sequences that followed versus violated the statistical structure, assessed at three time points (immediately post-training, as well as one day and one week post-training). Compared to artificial instruments, training on familiar instruments resulted in reduced accuracy. Moreover, sequences from familiar instruments - but not artificial instruments - were more likely to be judged as grammatical when they contained intervals that approximated those commonly used in Western music, even though this cue was non-informative. Overall, these results demonstrate that instrument familiarity can interfere with the learning of novel statistical regularities, presumably through biasing memory representations to be aligned with Western musical structures. These results demonstrate that real-world experience influences statistical learning in a non-linguistic domain, supporting the view that statistical learning involves the continuous updating of existing representations, rather than the establishment of entirely novel ones.
Collapse
|
7
|
Prosody facilitates learning the word order in a new language. Cognition 2021; 213:104686. [PMID: 33863550 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104686] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/14/2020] [Revised: 03/15/2021] [Accepted: 03/16/2021] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
One of the prominent ideas developed by Jacques Mehler and his colleagues was that perceptual tuning, present from birth on, enables infants, and language learners in general, to extract regularities from speech input. Here we discuss language learners'' ability to extract basic word order (VO or OV) structure from prosodic regularities in a language. The two are closely related: in phonological phrases of VO languages, the most prominent word is the rightmost one, and in OV languages, it is the leftmost one. In speech, this prominence is realized as extended duration, or as elevated pitch, sometimes combined with changes in intensity. When learning the first (L1) or the second language (L2), exposure to relevant rhythmic structure elicits implicit learning about syntactic structure, including the basic word order. However, it remains unclear whether triggering the learning process requires a certain level of familiarity with the relevant rhythm. It is moreover unknown whether prosodic information can help L2 learners to extract and learn the vocabulary of a new language. We tested Spanish- and Italian-speaking adults' ability to learn words from an artificial language with either non-native OV or native VO word order. The results show that learners used prosodic information to identify the most prominent words in short utterances when the artificial language was similar to the native language, with duration-based prominence in prosody and a VO word order. In contrast, when the artificial language had a non-native prominence marked by pitch alternations and an OV word order, prominent words were learned only after a three-day exposure to the relevant rhythmic structure. Thus, for adult L2 learners, only repeated exposure to the relevant prosody elicited learning new words from an unknown language with non-native prosodic marking, indicating that, with familiarity, prosodic cues can facilitate learning in L2.
Collapse
|
8
|
de la Cruz-Pavía I, Gervain J. Infants’ perception of repetition-based regularities in speech: a look from the perspective of the same/different distinction. Curr Opin Behav Sci 2021. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2020.11.014] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
|
9
|
|
10
|
Planton S, van Kerkoerle T, Abbih L, Maheu M, Meyniel F, Sigman M, Wang L, Figueira S, Romano S, Dehaene S. A theory of memory for binary sequences: Evidence for a mental compression algorithm in humans. PLoS Comput Biol 2021; 17:e1008598. [PMID: 33465081 PMCID: PMC7845997 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1008598] [Citation(s) in RCA: 28] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/09/2020] [Revised: 01/29/2021] [Accepted: 12/01/2020] [Indexed: 01/29/2023] Open
Abstract
Working memory capacity can be improved by recoding the memorized information in a condensed form. Here, we tested the theory that human adults encode binary sequences of stimuli in memory using an abstract internal language and a recursive compression algorithm. The theory predicts that the psychological complexity of a given sequence should be proportional to the length of its shortest description in the proposed language, which can capture any nested pattern of repetitions and alternations using a limited number of instructions. Five experiments examine the capacity of the theory to predict human adults' memory for a variety of auditory and visual sequences. We probed memory using a sequence violation paradigm in which participants attempted to detect occasional violations in an otherwise fixed sequence. Both subjective complexity ratings and objective violation detection performance were well predicted by our theoretical measure of complexity, which simply reflects a weighted sum of the number of elementary instructions and digits in the shortest formula that captures the sequence in our language. While a simpler transition probability model, when tested as a single predictor in the statistical analyses, accounted for significant variance in the data, the goodness-of-fit with the data significantly improved when the language-based complexity measure was included in the statistical model, while the variance explained by the transition probability model largely decreased. Model comparison also showed that shortest description length in a recursive language provides a better fit than six alternative previously proposed models of sequence encoding. The data support the hypothesis that, beyond the extraction of statistical knowledge, human sequence coding relies on an internal compression using language-like nested structures.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Samuel Planton
- Cognitive Neuroimaging Unit, CEA, INSERM, Université Paris-Sud, Université Paris-Saclay, NeuroSpin center, Gif/Yvette, France
| | - Timo van Kerkoerle
- Cognitive Neuroimaging Unit, CEA, INSERM, Université Paris-Sud, Université Paris-Saclay, NeuroSpin center, Gif/Yvette, France
| | - Leïla Abbih
- Cognitive Neuroimaging Unit, CEA, INSERM, Université Paris-Sud, Université Paris-Saclay, NeuroSpin center, Gif/Yvette, France
| | - Maxime Maheu
- Cognitive Neuroimaging Unit, CEA, INSERM, Université Paris-Sud, Université Paris-Saclay, NeuroSpin center, Gif/Yvette, France
- Université de Paris, Paris, France
| | - Florent Meyniel
- Cognitive Neuroimaging Unit, CEA, INSERM, Université Paris-Sud, Université Paris-Saclay, NeuroSpin center, Gif/Yvette, France
| | - Mariano Sigman
- Laboratorio de Neurociencia, Universidad Torcuato Di Tella, Buenos Aires, Argentina
- CONICET (Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Tecnicas), Buenos Aires, Argentina
- Facultad de Lenguas y Educacion, Universidad Nebrija, Madrid, Spain
| | - Liping Wang
- Institute of Neuroscience, Key Laboratory of Primate Neurobiology, CAS Center for Excellence in Brain Science and Intelligence Technology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Shanghai, China
| | - Santiago Figueira
- CONICET (Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Tecnicas), Buenos Aires, Argentina
- Universidad de Buenos Aires. Facultad de Ciencias Exactas y Naturales, Departamento de Computacion, Buenos Aires, Argentina
| | - Sergio Romano
- CONICET (Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Tecnicas), Buenos Aires, Argentina
- Universidad de Buenos Aires. Facultad de Ciencias Exactas y Naturales, Departamento de Computacion, Buenos Aires, Argentina
| | - Stanislas Dehaene
- Cognitive Neuroimaging Unit, CEA, INSERM, Université Paris-Sud, Université Paris-Saclay, NeuroSpin center, Gif/Yvette, France
- Collège de France, Paris, France
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Endress AD. A Simple, Biologically Plausible Feature Detector for Language Acquisition. J Cogn Neurosci 2020; 32:435-445. [DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_01494] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/04/2022]
Abstract
Abstract
Language has a complex grammatical system we still have to understand computationally and biologically. However, some evolutionarily ancient mechanisms have been repurposed for grammar so that we can use insight from other taxa into possible circuit-level mechanisms of grammar. Drawing upon recent evidence for the importance of disinhibitory circuits across taxa and brain regions, I suggest a simple circuit that explains the acquisition of core grammatical rules used in 85% of the world's languages: grammatical rules based on sameness/difference relations. This circuit acts as a sameness detector. “Different” items are suppressed through inhibition, but presenting two “identical” items leads to inhibition of inhibition. The items are thus propagated for further processing. This sameness detector thus acts as a feature detector for a grammatical rule. I suggest that having a set of feature detectors for elementary grammatical rules might make language acquisition feasible based on relatively simple computational mechanisms.
Collapse
|
12
|
Alhama RG, Zuidema W. A review of computational models of basic rule learning: The neural-symbolic debate and beyond. Psychon Bull Rev 2019; 26:1174-1194. [PMID: 31140126 PMCID: PMC6710217 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-019-01602-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/02/2022]
Abstract
We present a critical review of computational models of generalization of simple grammar-like rules, such as ABA and ABB. In particular, we focus on models attempting to account for the empirical results of Marcus et al. (Science, 283(5398), 77-80 1999). In that study, evidence is reported of generalization behavior by 7-month-old infants, using an Artificial Language Learning paradigm. The authors fail to replicate this behavior in neural network simulations, and claim that this failure reveals inherent limitations of a whole class of neural networks: those that do not incorporate symbolic operations. A great number of computational models were proposed in follow-up studies, fuelling a heated debate about what is required for a model to generalize. Twenty years later, this debate is still not settled. In this paper, we review a large number of the proposed models. We present a critical analysis of those models, in terms of how they contribute to answer the most relevant questions raised by the experiment. After identifying which aspects require further research, we propose a list of desiderata for advancing our understanding on generalization.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Raquel G. Alhama
- Language Development Department, Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Wundtlaan 1, 6525 XD Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | - Willem Zuidema
- Institute for Logic, Language and Computation, University of Amsterdam, Science Park 107, 1098 XG Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Tseng CH, Chow HM, Ma YK, Ding J. Preverbal infants utilize cross-modal semantic congruency in artificial grammar acquisition. Sci Rep 2018; 8:12707. [PMID: 30139964 PMCID: PMC6107625 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-018-30927-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/05/2017] [Accepted: 07/30/2018] [Indexed: 11/09/2022] Open
Abstract
Learning in a multisensory world is challenging as the information from different sensory dimensions may be inconsistent and confusing. By adulthood, learners optimally integrate bimodal (e.g. audio-visual, AV) stimulation by both low-level (e.g. temporal synchrony) and high-level (e.g. semantic congruency) properties of the stimuli to boost learning outcomes. However, it is unclear how this capacity emerges and develops. To approach this question, we examined whether preverbal infants were capable of utilizing high-level properties with grammar-like rule acquisition. In three experiments, we habituated pre-linguistic infants with an audio-visual (AV) temporal sequence that resembled a grammar-like rule (A-A-B). We varied the cross-modal semantic congruence of the AV stimuli (Exp 1: congruent syllables/faces; Exp 2: incongruent syllables/shapes; Exp 3: incongruent beeps/faces) while all the other low-level properties (e.g. temporal synchrony, sensory energy) were constant. Eight- to ten-month-old infants only learned the grammar-like rule from AV congruent stimuli pairs (Exp 1), not from incongruent AV pairs (Exp 2, 3). Our results show that similar to adults, preverbal infants' learning is influenced by a high-level multisensory integration gating system, pointing to a perceptual origin of bimodal learning advantage that was not previously acknowledged.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chia-Huei Tseng
- Research Institute of Electrical Communication, Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan.
| | - Hiu Mei Chow
- Department of Psychology, University of Massachusetts Boston, Boston, USA
| | - Yuen Ki Ma
- Department of Psychology, The University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong, SAR, China
| | - Jie Ding
- Department of Psychology, The University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong, SAR, China
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Guevara-Rukoz A, Cristia A, Ludusan B, Thiollière R, Martin A, Mazuka R, Dupoux E. Are Words Easier to Learn From Infant- Than Adult-Directed Speech? A Quantitative Corpus-Based Investigation. Cogn Sci 2018; 42:1586-1617. [PMID: 29851142 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.12616] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/15/2016] [Revised: 12/04/2017] [Accepted: 02/26/2018] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Abstract
We investigate whether infant-directed speech (IDS) could facilitate word form learning when compared to adult-directed speech (ADS). To study this, we examine the distribution of word forms at two levels, acoustic and phonological, using a large database of spontaneous speech in Japanese. At the acoustic level we show that, as has been documented before for phonemes, the realizations of words are more variable and less discriminable in IDS than in ADS. At the phonological level, we find an effect in the opposite direction: The IDS lexicon contains more distinctive words (such as onomatopoeias) than the ADS counterpart. Combining the acoustic and phonological metrics together in a global discriminability score reveals that the bigger separation of lexical categories in the phonological space does not compensate for the opposite effect observed at the acoustic level. As a result, IDS word forms are still globally less discriminable than ADS word forms, even though the effect is numerically small. We discuss the implication of these findings for the view that the functional role of IDS is to improve language learnability.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Alejandrina Cristia
- Laboratoire de Sciences Cognitives et Psycholinguistique, ENS/EHESS/CNRS/PSL
| | - Bogdan Ludusan
- Laboratoire de Sciences Cognitives et Psycholinguistique, ENS/EHESS/CNRS/PSL
- Laboratory for Language Development, RIKEN Brain Science Institute
| | - Roland Thiollière
- Laboratoire de Sciences Cognitives et Psycholinguistique, ENS/EHESS/CNRS/PSL
| | - Andrew Martin
- Faculty of Letters, Department of English Literature and Language, Konan University
| | - Reiko Mazuka
- Laboratory for Language Development, RIKEN Brain Science Institute
- Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Duke University
| | - Emmanuel Dupoux
- Laboratoire de Sciences Cognitives et Psycholinguistique, ENS/EHESS/CNRS/PSL
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Visual artificial grammar learning by rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta): exploring the role of grammar complexity and sequence length. Anim Cogn 2018; 21:267-284. [DOI: 10.1007/s10071-018-1164-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/12/2017] [Revised: 01/20/2018] [Accepted: 01/28/2018] [Indexed: 01/04/2023]
|
16
|
Ota M, Skarabela B. Reduplication facilitates early word segmentation. JOURNAL OF CHILD LANGUAGE 2018; 45:204-218. [PMID: 28162111 DOI: 10.1017/s0305000916000660] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/06/2023]
Abstract
This study explores the possibility that early word segmentation is aided by infants' tendency to segment words with repeated syllables ('reduplication'). Twenty-four nine-month-olds were familiarized with passages containing one novel reduplicated word and one novel non-reduplicated word. Their central fixation times in response to these as well as new reduplicated and non-reduplicated words introduced at test showed that familiarized reduplicated words were segmented better than familiarized non-reduplicated words. These results demonstrate that infants are predisposed to segment words with repeated phonological elements, and suggest that register-specific words in infant-directed speech may have evolved in response to this learning bias.
Collapse
|
17
|
Witt A, Vinter A. Perceptual and Positional Saliencies Influence Children's Sequence Learning Differently with Age and Instructions at Test. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 2017; 70:2219-2233. [DOI: 10.1080/17470218.2016.1230141] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
Abstract
There is growing evidence that, faced with a complex environment, participants subdivide the incoming information into small perceptual units, called chunks. Although statistical properties have been identified as playing a key role in chunking, we wanted to determine whether perceptual (repetitions) and positional (initial units) features might provide immediate guidance for the parsing of information into chunks. Children aged 5 and 8 years were exposed to sequences of 3, 4, or 5 colours. Sequence learning was assessed either through an explicit generation test (Experiment 1) or through a recognition test (Experiment 2). Experiment 1 showed that perceptual and positional saliencies benefited learning and that sensitivity to repetitions was age dependent and permitted the formation of longer chunks (trigrams) in the oldest children. Experiment 2 suggested that children became sensitive to perceptual and positional saliencies regardless of age and that the both types of saliencies supported the formation of longer chunks in the oldest children. The discussion focuses on the multiple factors intervening in sequence learning and their differential effects as a function of the instructions used at test to assess sequence learning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Arnaud Witt
- LEAD-CNRS, University of Bourgogne Franche-Comté, Dijon, France
| | - Annie Vinter
- LEAD-CNRS, University of Bourgogne Franche-Comté, Dijon, France
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Liu H, Xu C, Liang J. Dependency distance: A new perspective on syntactic patterns in natural languages. Phys Life Rev 2017. [DOI: 10.1016/j.plrev.2017.03.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 101] [Impact Index Per Article: 12.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
|
19
|
Abstract
Language learners encounter numerous opportunities to learn regularities, but need to decide which of these regularities to learn, because some are not productive in their native language. Here, we present an account of rule learning based on perceptual and memory primitives (Endress, Dehaene-Lambertz, & Mehler, Cognition, 105(3), 577–614, 2007; Endress, Nespor, & Mehler, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(8), 348–353, 2009), suggesting that learners preferentially learn regularities that are more salient to them, and that the pattern of salience reflects the frequency of language features across languages. We contrast this view with previous artificial grammar learning research, which suggests that infants “choose” the regularities they learn based on rational, Bayesian criteria (Frank & Tenenbaum, Cognition, 120(3), 360–371, 2013; Gerken, Cognition, 98(3)B67–B74, 2006, Cognition, 115(2), 362–366, 2010). In our experiments, adult participants listened to syllable strings starting with a syllable reduplication and always ending with the same “affix” syllable, or to syllable strings starting with this “affix” syllable and ending with the “reduplication”. Both affixation and reduplication are frequently used for morphological marking across languages. We find three crucial results. First, participants learned both regularities simultaneously. Second, affixation regularities seemed easier to learn than reduplication regularities. Third, regularities in sequence offsets were easier to learn than regularities at sequence onsets. We show that these results are inconsistent with previous Bayesian rule learning models, but mesh well with the perceptual or memory primitives view. Further, we show that the pattern of salience revealed in our experiments reflects the distribution of regularities across languages. Ease of acquisition might thus be one determinant of the frequency of regularities across languages.
Collapse
|
20
|
Artificial grammar learning in zebra finches and human adults: XYX versus XXY. Anim Cogn 2016; 18:151-64. [PMID: 25015135 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-014-0786-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 34] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/28/2014] [Revised: 06/05/2014] [Accepted: 07/01/2014] [Indexed: 12/26/2022]
Abstract
Abstracting syntactic rules is critical to human language learning. It is debated whether this ability, already present in young infants, is human- and language specific or can also be found in non-human animals, indicating it may arise from more general cognitive mechanisms. Current studies are often ambiguous and few have directly compared rule learning by humans and non-human animals. In a series of discrimination experiments, we presented zebra finches and human adults with comparable training and tests with the same artificial stimuli consisting of XYX and XXY structures, in which X and Y were zebra finch song elements. Zebra finches readily discriminated the training stimuli. Some birds also discriminated novel stimuli when these were composed of familiar element types, but none of the birds generalized the discrimination to novel element types. We conclude that zebra finches show evidence of simple rule abstraction related to positional learning, suggesting stimulus-bound generalization, but found no evidence for a more abstract rule generalization. This differed from the human adults, who categorized novel stimuli consisting of novel element types into different groups according to their structure. The limited abilities for rule abstraction in zebra finches may indicate what the precursors of more complex abstraction as found in humans may have been like.
Collapse
|
21
|
Santolin C, Rosa-Salva O, Regolin L, Vallortigara G. Generalization of visual regularities in newly hatched chicks (Gallus gallus). Anim Cogn 2016; 19:1007-17. [PMID: 27287627 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-016-1005-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/14/2016] [Revised: 05/24/2016] [Accepted: 05/30/2016] [Indexed: 12/27/2022]
Abstract
Evidence of learning and generalization of visual regularities in a newborn organism is provided in the present research. Domestic chicks have been trained to discriminate visual triplets of simultaneously presented shapes, implementing AAB versus ABA (Experiment 1), AAB versus ABB and AAB versus BAA (Experiment 2). Chicks distinguished pattern-following and pattern-violating novel test triplets in all comparisons, showing no preference for repetition-based patterns. The animals generalized to novel instances even when the patterns compared were not discriminable by the presence or absence of reduplicated elements or by symmetry (e.g., AAB vs. ABB). These findings represent the first evidence of learning and generalization of regularities at the onset of life in an animal model, revealing intriguing differences with respect to human newborns and infants. Extensive prior experience seems to be unnecessary to drive the process, suggesting that chicks are predisposed to detect patterns characterizing the visual world.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chiara Santolin
- Department of General Psychology, University of Padova, Padova, Italy. .,Waisman Center, University of Wisconsin-Madison, 1500, Highland Avenue, Madison, WI, 53705, USA. .,Department of Psychology and Cognitive Science, University of Trento, Rovereto, Italy.
| | - Orsola Rosa-Salva
- CIMeC, Center for Mind-Brain Sciences, University of Trento, Rovereto, Italy
| | - Lucia Regolin
- Department of General Psychology, University of Padova, Padova, Italy
| | | |
Collapse
|
22
|
Endress AD, Bonatti LL. Words, rules, and mechanisms of language acquisition. WILEY INTERDISCIPLINARY REVIEWS. COGNITIVE SCIENCE 2015; 7:19-35. [PMID: 26683248 DOI: 10.1002/wcs.1376] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/23/2015] [Revised: 09/21/2015] [Accepted: 11/17/2015] [Indexed: 11/10/2022]
Abstract
We review recent artificial language learning studies, especially those following Endress and Bonatti (Endress AD, Bonatti LL. Rapid learning of syllable classes from a perceptually continuous speech stream. Cognition 2007, 105:247-299), suggesting that humans can deploy a variety of learning mechanisms to acquire artificial languages. Several experiments provide evidence for multiple learning mechanisms that can be deployed in fluent speech: one mechanism encodes the positions of syllables within words and can be used to extract generalization, while the other registers co-occurrence statistics of syllables and can be used to break a continuum into its components. We review dissociations between these mechanisms and their potential role in language acquisition. We then turn to recent criticisms of the multiple mechanisms hypothesis and show that they are inconsistent with the available data. Our results suggest that artificial and natural language learning is best understood by dissecting the underlying specialized learning abilities, and that these data provide a rare opportunity to link important language phenomena to basic psychological mechanisms. For further resources related to this article, please visit the WIREs website.
Collapse
|
23
|
ten Cate C. On the phonetic and syntactic processing abilities of birds: from songs to speech and artificial grammars. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2014; 28:157-64. [PMID: 25078891 DOI: 10.1016/j.conb.2014.07.019] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/02/2014] [Revised: 07/10/2014] [Accepted: 07/11/2014] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
Like speech and language, the songs of many songbirds consist of learned, rapidly produced, structured sequences of distinct vocal units, originating from an interplay between experience and learning biases. Songs are species specific, but also show considerable within species variation in elements or element sequencing. This variation implies that birds possess mechanisms to identify, categorize and combine sounds. I review the abilities for speech sound perception and categorization, as well as for grammatical rule learning by birds. Speech sound perception in birds is in many ways comparable to human speech perception. Birds can also detect and generalize patterns underlying artificially arranged strings of vocal elements. However, there is a need for more comparative studies to examine the limits of their rule learning abilities and how they relate to those of humans.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Carel ten Cate
- Leiden Institute of Biology and Leiden Institute for Brain and Cognition, Leiden University, PO Box 9505, 2300 RA Leiden, The Netherlands.
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Affiliation(s)
| | - Ansgar D. Endress
- Department of Technology; Universitat Pompeu Fabra
- Department of Psychology; City University London
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
Basirat A, Dehaene S, Dehaene-Lambertz G. A hierarchy of cortical responses to sequence violations in three-month-old infants. Cognition 2014; 132:137-50. [PMID: 24809742 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.03.013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 68] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/03/2012] [Revised: 01/30/2014] [Accepted: 03/28/2014] [Indexed: 12/26/2022]
Abstract
The adult human brain quickly adapts to regular temporal sequences, and emits a sequence of novelty responses when these regularities are violated. These novelty responses have been interpreted as error signals that reflect the difference between the incoming signal and predictions generated at multiple cortical levels. Do infants already possess such a hierarchy of violation-detection mechanisms? Using high-density recordings of event-related potentials during an auditory local-global violation paradigm, we show that three-month-old infants process novelty in temporal sequences at two distinct levels. Violations of local expectancies, such as perceiving a deviant vowel "a" after repeated presentation of another vowel i-i-i, elicited an early auditory mismatch response. Conversely, violations of global expectancies, such as hearing the rare sequence a-a-a-a instead of the frequent sequence a-a-a-i, modulated this early mismatch response and led to a late frontal negative slow wave, whose cortical sources included the left inferior frontal region. These results suggest that the infant brain already possesses two dissociable systems for temporal sequence learning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anahita Basirat
- INSERM, U992, Cognitive Neuroimaging Unit, F-91191 Gif/Yvette, France; CEA, DSV/I2BM, NeuroSpin Center, F-91191 Gif/Yvette, France; University Paris-Sud, Cognitive Neuroimaging Unit, F-91191 Gif/Yvette, France.
| | - Stanislas Dehaene
- INSERM, U992, Cognitive Neuroimaging Unit, F-91191 Gif/Yvette, France; CEA, DSV/I2BM, NeuroSpin Center, F-91191 Gif/Yvette, France; University Paris-Sud, Cognitive Neuroimaging Unit, F-91191 Gif/Yvette, France; Collège de France, F-75005 Paris, France
| | - Ghislaine Dehaene-Lambertz
- INSERM, U992, Cognitive Neuroimaging Unit, F-91191 Gif/Yvette, France; CEA, DSV/I2BM, NeuroSpin Center, F-91191 Gif/Yvette, France; University Paris-Sud, Cognitive Neuroimaging Unit, F-91191 Gif/Yvette, France
| |
Collapse
|
26
|
Landy D, Allen C, Zednik C. A perceptual account of symbolic reasoning. Front Psychol 2014; 5:275. [PMID: 24795662 PMCID: PMC4001060 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00275] [Citation(s) in RCA: 37] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/11/2013] [Accepted: 03/14/2014] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
People can be taught to manipulate symbols according to formal mathematical and logical rules. Cognitive scientists have traditionally viewed this capacity-the capacity for symbolic reasoning-as grounded in the ability to internally represent numbers, logical relationships, and mathematical rules in an abstract, amodal fashion. We present an alternative view, portraying symbolic reasoning as a special kind of embodied reasoning in which arithmetic and logical formulae, externally represented as notations, serve as targets for powerful perceptual and sensorimotor systems. Although symbolic reasoning often conforms to abstract mathematical principles, it is typically implemented by perceptual and sensorimotor engagement with concrete environmental structures.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- David Landy
- Psychological and Brain Science/Cognitive Science, Indiana University Bloomington, IN, USA
| | - Colin Allen
- History and Philosophy of Science/Cognitive Science, Indiana University Bloomington, IN, USA
| | - Carlos Zednik
- Institute of Cognitive Science, University of Osnabrück Osnabrück, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
27
|
Endress AD. How are Bayesian models really used? Reply to Frank (2013). Cognition 2014; 130:81-4. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.09.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/07/2013] [Revised: 09/13/2013] [Accepted: 09/18/2013] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
|
28
|
Gervain J, Werker JF. Learning non-adjacent regularities at age 0 ; 7. JOURNAL OF CHILD LANGUAGE 2013; 40:860-872. [PMID: 22863363 DOI: 10.1017/s0305000912000256] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/01/2023]
Abstract
One important mechanism suggested to underlie the acquisition of grammar is rule learning. Indeed, infants aged 0 ; 7 are able to learn rules based on simple identity relations (adjacent repetitions, ABB: "wo fe fe" and non-adjacent repetitions, ABA: "wo fe wo", respectively; Marcus et al., 1999). One unexplored issue is whether young infants are able to process both adjacent and non-adjacent repetitions. As the previous studies always compared the two types of repetition structures directly, the ability to learn only one of them was sufficient for successful discrimination in these tasks. The present study reports two experiments, in which we test the ability of infants aged 0 ; 7 to discriminate adjacent and non-adjacent repetition structures against random controls (ABB vs. ABC and ABA vs. ABC). We show that, contrary to some previous proposals, infants aged 0 ; 7 successfully discriminate both repetition types from random controls, but show no spontaneous preference for either of them.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Judit Gervain
- CNRS and Université Paris Descartes, Sorbonne Paris Cité, Paris, France.
| | | |
Collapse
|
29
|
Frank MC. Throwing out the Bayesian baby with the optimal bathwater: Response to Endress (2013). Cognition 2013; 128:417-23. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.04.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/16/2013] [Revised: 04/29/2013] [Accepted: 04/30/2013] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
|
30
|
Li F, Jiang S, Guo X, Yang Z, Dienes Z. The nature of the memory buffer in implicit learning: learning Chinese tonal symmetries. Conscious Cogn 2013; 22:920-30. [PMID: 23863131 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2013.06.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/11/2012] [Revised: 06/07/2013] [Accepted: 06/09/2013] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
Abstract
Previous research has established that people can implicitly learn chunks, which (in terms of formal language theory) do not require a memory buffer to process. The present study explores the implicit learning of nonlocal dependencies generated by higher than finite-state grammars, specifically, Chinese tonal retrogrades (i.e. centre embeddings generated from a context-free grammar) and inversions (i.e. cross-serial dependencies generated from a mildly context-sensitive grammar), which do require buffers (for example, last in-first out and first in-first out, respectively). People were asked to listen to and memorize artificial poetry instantiating one of the two grammars; after this training phase, people were informed of the existence of rules and asked to classify new poems, while providing attributions of the basis of their judgments. People acquired unconscious structural knowledge of both tonal retrogrades and inversions. Moreover, inversions were implicitly learnt more easily than retrogrades constraining the nature of the memory buffer in computational models of implicit learning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Feifei Li
- School of Psychology and Cognitive Science, East China Normal University, Shanghai, China
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
31
|
Endress AD. Bayesian learning and the psychology of rule induction. Cognition 2013; 127:159-76. [PMID: 23454791 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.11.014] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/25/2011] [Revised: 11/29/2012] [Accepted: 11/30/2012] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
Abstract
In recent years, Bayesian learning models have been applied to an increasing variety of domains. While such models have been criticized on theoretical grounds, the underlying assumptions and predictions are rarely made concrete and tested experimentally. Here, I use Frank and Tenenbaum's (2011) Bayesian model of rule-learning as a case study to spell out the underlying assumptions, and to confront them with the empirical results Frank and Tenenbaum (2011) propose to simulate, as well as with novel experiments. While rule-learning is arguably well suited to rational Bayesian approaches, I show that their models are neither psychologically plausible nor ideal observer models. Further, I show that their central assumption is unfounded: humans do not always preferentially learn more specific rules, but, at least in some situations, those rules that happen to be more salient. Even when granting the unsupported assumptions, I show that all of the experiments modeled by Frank and Tenenbaum (2011) either contradict their models, or have a large number of more plausible interpretations. I provide an alternative account of the experimental data based on simple psychological mechanisms, and show that this account both describes the data better, and is easier to falsify. I conclude that, despite the recent surge in Bayesian models of cognitive phenomena, psychological phenomena are best understood by developing and testing psychological theories rather than models that can be fit to virtually any data.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ansgar D Endress
- Universitat Pompeu Fabra, Center of Brain and Cognition, C. Roc Boronat, 138, Edifici Tanger, 55.106, 08018 Barcelona, Spain.
| |
Collapse
|
32
|
Nastase S, Iacovella V, Hasson U. Uncertainty in visual and auditory series is coded by modality-general and modality-specific neural systems. Hum Brain Mapp 2013; 35:1111-28. [PMID: 23408389 DOI: 10.1002/hbm.22238] [Citation(s) in RCA: 49] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/30/2012] [Revised: 09/11/2012] [Accepted: 11/12/2012] [Indexed: 11/06/2022] Open
Abstract
Coding for the degree of disorder in a temporally unfolding sensory input allows for optimized encoding of these inputs via information compression and predictive processing. Prior neuroimaging work has examined sensitivity to statistical regularities within single sensory modalities and has associated this function with the hippocampus, anterior cingulate, and lateral temporal cortex. Here we investigated to what extent sensitivity to input disorder, quantified by Markov entropy, is subserved by modality-general or modality-specific neural systems when participants are not required to monitor the input. Participants were presented with rapid (3.3 Hz) auditory and visual series varying over four levels of entropy, while monitoring an infrequently changing fixation cross. For visual series, sensitivity to the magnitude of disorder was found in early visual cortex, the anterior cingulate, and the intraparietal sulcus. For auditory series, sensitivity was found in inferior frontal, lateral temporal, and supplementary motor regions implicated in speech perception and sequencing. Ventral premotor and central cingulate cortices were identified as possible candidates for modality-general uncertainty processing, exhibiting marginal sensitivity to disorder in both modalities. The right temporal pole differentiated the highest and lowest levels of disorder in both modalities, but did not show general sensitivity to the parametric manipulation of disorder. Our results indicate that neural sensitivity to input disorder relies largely on modality-specific systems embedded in extended sensory cortices, though uncertainty-related processing in frontal regions may be driven by both input modalities.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Samuel Nastase
- Center for Mind/Brain Sciences (CIMeC), The University of Trento, Rovereto (TN), Italy
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
33
|
Rule learning by zebra finches in an artificial grammar learning task: which rule? Anim Cogn 2012; 16:165-75. [PMID: 22971840 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-012-0559-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 47] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/24/2011] [Revised: 08/24/2012] [Accepted: 08/28/2012] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
Abstract
A hallmark of the human language faculty is the use of syntactic rules. The natural vocalizations of animals are syntactically simple, but several studies indicate that animals can detect and discriminate more complex structures in acoustic stimuli. However, how they discriminate such structures is often not clear. Using an artificial grammar learning paradigm, zebra finches were tested in a Go/No-go experiment for their ability to distinguish structurally different three-element sound sequences. In Experiment 1, zebra finches learned to discriminate ABA and BAB from ABB, AAB, BBA, and ABB sequences. Tests with probe sounds consisting of four elements suggested that the discrimination was based on attending to the presence or absence of repeated A- and B-elements. One bird generalized the discrimination to a new element type. In Experiment 2, we continued the training by adding four-element songs following a 'first and last identical versus different' rule that could not be solved by attending to repetitions. Only two out of five birds learned the overall discrimination. Testing with novel probes demonstrated that discrimination was not based on using the 'first and last identical' rule, but on attending to the presence or absence of the individual training stimuli. The two birds differed in the strategies used. Our results thus demonstrate only a limited degree of abstract rule learning but highlight the need for extensive and critical probe testing to examine the rules that animals (and humans) use to solve artificial grammar learning tasks. They also underline that rule learning strategies may differ between individuals.
Collapse
|
34
|
Infant rule learning: advantage language, or advantage speech? PLoS One 2012; 7:e40517. [PMID: 22815756 PMCID: PMC3399874 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0040517] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/01/2012] [Accepted: 06/11/2012] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Infants appear to learn abstract rule-like regularities (e.g., la la da follows an AAB pattern) more easily from speech than from a variety of other auditory and visual stimuli (Marcus et al., 2007). We test if that facilitation reflects a specialization to learn from speech alone, or from modality-independent communicative stimuli more generally, by measuring 7.5-month-old infants’ ability to learn abstract rules from sign language-like gestures. Whereas infants appear to easily learn many different rules from speech, we found that with sign-like stimuli, and under circumstances comparable to those of Marcus et al. (1999), hearing infants were able to learn an ABB rule, but not an AAB rule. This is consistent with results of studies that demonstrate lower levels of infant rule learning from a variety of other non-speech stimuli, and we discuss implications for accounts of speech-facilitation.
Collapse
|
35
|
Abstract
The mismatch negativity (MMN) is thought to index the activation of specialized neural networks for active prediction and deviance detection. However, a detailed neuronal model of the neurobiological mechanisms underlying the MMN is still lacking, and its computational foundations remain debated. We propose here a detailed neuronal model of auditory cortex, based on predictive coding, that accounts for the critical features of MMN. The model is entirely composed of spiking excitatory and inhibitory neurons interconnected in a layered cortical architecture with distinct input, predictive, and prediction error units. A spike-timing dependent learning rule, relying upon NMDA receptor synaptic transmission, allows the network to adjust its internal predictions and use a memory of the recent past inputs to anticipate on future stimuli based on transition statistics. We demonstrate that this simple architecture can account for the major empirical properties of the MMN. These include a frequency-dependent response to rare deviants, a response to unexpected repeats in alternating sequences (ABABAA…), a lack of consideration of the global sequence context, a response to sound omission, and a sensitivity of the MMN to NMDA receptor antagonists. Novel predictions are presented, and a new magnetoencephalography experiment in healthy human subjects is presented that validates our key hypothesis: the MMN results from active cortical prediction rather than passive synaptic habituation.
Collapse
|
36
|
Hochmann JR, Benavides-Varela S, Nespor M, Mehler J. Consonants and vowels: different roles in early language acquisition. Dev Sci 2011; 14:1445-58. [PMID: 22010902 DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01089.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 61] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
Abstract
Language acquisition involves both acquiring a set of words (i.e. the lexicon) and learning the rules that combine them to form sentences (i.e. syntax). Here, we show that consonants are mainly involved in word processing, whereas vowels are favored for extracting and generalizing structural relations. We demonstrate that such a division of labor between consonants and vowels plays a role in language acquisition. In two very similar experimental paradigms, we show that 12-month-old infants rely more on the consonantal tier when identifying words (Experiment 1), but are better at extracting and generalizing repetition-based srtuctures over the vocalic tier (Experiment 2). These results indicate that infants are able to exploit the functional differences between consonants and vowels at an age when they start acquiring the lexicon, and suggest that basic speech categories are assigned to different learning mechanisms that sustain early language acquisition.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jean-Rémy Hochmann
- Cognitive Neuroscience Department, SISSA, International School for Advanced Studies, Trieste, Italy.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
37
|
Three ideal observer models for rule learning in simple languages. Cognition 2011; 120:360-71. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.10.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 28] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/10/2010] [Revised: 09/08/2010] [Accepted: 10/11/2010] [Indexed: 11/13/2022]
|
38
|
Abstract
The literature on repetition processing reveals an intriguing paradox between the particular salience of repetitions, which makes them easy to learn, and a tendency to avoid them when generating sequences. The aim of this experiment was to study the extent to which children can learn to produce these avoided behaviours by means of an artificial grammar paradigm using generation tests with implicit or explicit instructions. The analysis of the control group's performance confirmed the presence of a spontaneous tendency to avoid generating repetitions. A comparison with chance revealed that the children learned to produce repetitions in the explicit test but not in the implicit test. However, a comparison with the control group showed that learning nonetheless occurred in the experimental group with the implicit test. The discussion focused on this antirepetition behavioural bias and how it interacted with the type of information processes elicited by the tests selected for assessing implicit learning effects.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Arnaud Witt
- LEAD-CNRS, Université de Bourgogne, Dijon, France
| | - Annie Vinter
- LEAD-CNRS, Université de Bourgogne, Dijon, France
| |
Collapse
|
39
|
Witt A, Vinter A. Artificial grammar learning in children: abstraction of rules or sensitivity to perceptual features? PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2011; 76:97-110. [DOI: 10.1007/s00426-011-0328-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/30/2010] [Accepted: 02/24/2011] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
|
40
|
Abstract
All theories of language development suggest that learning is constrained. However, theories differ on whether these constraints arise from language-specific processes or have domain-general origins such as the characteristics of human perception and information processing. The current experiments explored constraints on statistical learning of patterns, such as the phonotactic patterns of an infants' native language. Infants in these experiments were presented with a visual analog of a phonotactic learning task used by J. R. Saffran and E. D. Thiessen (2003). Saffran and Thiessen found that infants' phonotactic learning was constrained such that some patterns were learned more easily than other patterns. The current results indicate that infants' learning of visual patterns shows the same constraints as infants' learning of phonotactic patterns. This is consistent with theories suggesting that constraints arise from domain-general sources and, as such, should operate over many kinds of stimuli in addition to linguistic stimuli.
Collapse
|
41
|
Bion RAH, Benavides-Varela S, Nespor M. Acoustic markers of prominence influence infants' and adults' segmentation of speech sequences. LANGUAGE AND SPEECH 2011; 54:123-140. [PMID: 21524015 DOI: 10.1177/0023830910388018] [Citation(s) in RCA: 40] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/30/2023]
Abstract
Two experiments investigated the way acoustic markers of prominence influence the grouping of speech sequences by adults and 7-month-old infants. In the first experiment, adults were familiarized with and asked to memorize sequences of adjacent syllables that alternated in either pitch or duration. During the test phase, participants heard pairs of syllables with constant pitch and duration and were asked whether the syllables had appeared adjacently during familiarization. Adults were better at remembering pairs of syllables that during familiarization had short syllables preceding long syllables, or high-pitched syllables preceding low-pitched syllables. In the second experiment, infants were familiarized and tested with similar stimuli as in the first experiment, and their preference for pairs of syllables was accessed using the head-turn preference paradigm.When familiarized with syllables alternating in pitch, infants showed a preference to listen to pairs of syllables that had high pitch in the first syllable. However, no preference was found when the familiarization stream alternated in duration. It is proposed that these perceptual biases help infants and adults find linguistic units in the continuous speech stream.While the bias for grouping based on pitch appears early in development, biases for durational grouping might rely on more extensive linguistic experience.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ricardo A H Bion
- Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Jordan Hall, Building 420, Stanford, CA 94305, USA.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
42
|
Gervain J, Mehler J. Speech Perception and Language Acquisition in the First Year of Life. Annu Rev Psychol 2010; 61:191-218. [PMID: 19575623 DOI: 10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100408] [Citation(s) in RCA: 138] [Impact Index Per Article: 9.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Judit Gervain
- Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver British Columbia, V6T 1Z4, Canada
| | - Jacques Mehler
- Neuroscience Sector, Scuola Internazionale Superiore di Studi Avanzati, Trieste 31014, Italy;
| |
Collapse
|
43
|
|
44
|
Perceptual and memory constraints on language acquisition. Trends Cogn Sci 2009; 13:348-53. [PMID: 19647474 DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.05.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 99] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/13/2007] [Revised: 05/05/2009] [Accepted: 05/11/2009] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
A wide variety of organisms employ specialized mechanisms to cope with the demands of their environment. We suggest that the same is true for humans when acquiring artificial grammars, and at least some basic properties of natural grammars. We show that two basic mechanisms can explain many results in artificial grammar learning experiments, and different linguistic regularities ranging from stress assignment to interfaces between different components of grammar. One mechanism is sensitive to identity relations, whereas the other uses sequence edges as anchor points for extracting positional regularities. This piecemeal approach to mental computations helps to explain otherwise perplexing data, and offers a working hypothesis on how statistical and symbolic accounts of cognitive processes could be bridged.
Collapse
|
45
|
Abstract
Repetition is a pervasive feature of children's environments, and may be an important contributor to learning such complex sequential structures as language. Endress, Dehaene-Lambertz, and Mehler (2007) found that repeated tone sequences were learned more easily than sequences containing ordinal relations, but there have been no direct comparisons of repeating sequences versus sequences that contain similar, but not identical, stimuli. In Experiment 1, we compared learning from repeating tone sequences to learning from tones that varied in similarity, and confirmed that repetition is a special case for learning. In Experiment 2 we showed that the learning distinction between repeated and similar elements is not affected by whether similarity is variable. We conclude by indicating that repetition provides an important constraint on learning, and we discuss the extent to which such constraints are consistent with general-purpose statistical learning mechanisms.
Collapse
|
46
|
Abstract
What are the origins of the efficient language learning abilities that allow humans to acquire their mother tongue in just a few years very early in life? Although previous studies have identified different mechanisms underlying the acquisition of auditory and speech patterns in older infants and adults, the earliest sensitivities remain unexplored. To address this issue, we investigated the ability of newborns to learn simple repetition-based structures in two optical brain-imaging experiments. In the first experiment, 22 neonates listened to syllable sequences containing immediate repetitions (ABB; e.g., "mubaba," "penana"), intermixed with random control sequences (ABC; e.g., "mubage," "penaku"). We found increased responses to the repetition sequences in the temporal and left frontal areas, indicating that the newborn brain differentiated the two patterns. The repetition sequences evoked greater activation than the random sequences during the first few trials, suggesting the presence of an automatic perceptual mechanism to detect repetitions. In addition, over the subsequent trials, activation increased further in response to the repetition sequences but not in response to the random sequences, indicating that recognition of the ABB pattern was enhanced by repeated exposure. In the second experiment, in which nonadjacent repetitions (ABA; e.g., "bamuba," "napena") were contrasted with the same random controls, no discrimination was observed. These findings suggest that newborns are sensitive to certain input configurations in the auditory domain, a perceptual ability that might facilitate later language development.
Collapse
|