1
|
Abadi D, Willem van Prooijen J, Krouwel A, Fischer AH. Anti-establishment sentiments: realistic and symbolic threat appraisals predict populist attitudes and conspiracy mentality. Cogn Emot 2024; 38:1246-1260. [PMID: 38863199 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2024.2360584] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/05/2023] [Revised: 03/30/2024] [Accepted: 05/21/2024] [Indexed: 06/13/2024]
Abstract
Previous research has found that populist attitudes and conspiracy mentality - here summarised as anti-establishment attitudes - increase when people feel threatened. Two types of intergroup threat have been distinguished, namely realistic threats (pertaining to socio-economic resources, climate, or health), and symbolic threats (pertaining to cultural values). However, there is no agreement on which types of threat and corresponding appraisals would be most important in predicting anti-establishment attitudes. We hypothesise that it is the threat itself, irrespective of its cause, that predicts anti-establishment attitudes. In the current paper, we conducted new (multilevel) regression analyses on previously collected data from four high-powered studies with multiple time points (Study 1) or collected in multiple nations (Studies 2-4). All studies included a populist attitudes scale, a conspiracy mentality scale, and different types of threat and emotion measures, reflecting both realistic and symbolic threats. Across studies, both realistic and symbolic threats positively predicted anti-establishment attitudes. The results support an emotional appraisal approach to anti-establishment attitudes, which highlights the importance of anxiety and feeling threatened regardless of what type of event elicits the threat.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- David Abadi
- Department of Psychology, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, Netherlands
| | - Jan Willem van Prooijen
- Department of Experimental and Applied Psychology, VU Amsterdam, Amsterdam, Netherlands
- Netherlands Institute for the Study of Crime and Law Enforcement (NSCR), HV Amsterdam, Netherlands
- Department of Criminal Law and Criminology, Maastricht University, Maastricht, Netherlands
| | - André Krouwel
- Department of Communication Science, VU Amsterdam, Amsterdam, Netherlands
| | - Agneta H Fischer
- Department of Psychology, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Neerdaels J, Tröster C, Van Quaquebeke N. It's (a) Shame: Why Poverty Leads to Support for Authoritarianism. PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN 2024; 50:942-956. [PMID: 36575968 DOI: 10.1177/01461672221141509] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/29/2022]
Abstract
The literature has widely discussed and supported the relationship between poverty and support for authoritarian leaders and regimes. However, there are different claims about the mediating mechanism and a lack of empirical tests. We hypothesize that the effect of poverty on support for authoritarianism is mediated by shame: People living in poverty frequently experience social exclusion and devaluation, which is reflected in feelings of shame. Such shame, in turn, is likely to increase support for authoritarianism, mainly due to the promise of social re-inclusion. We support our hypothesis in two controlled experiments and a large-scale field study while empirically ruling out the two main alternative explanations offered in the literature: stress and anxiety. Finally, we discuss how the present findings can support policymakers in efficiently addressing the negative political consequences of poverty.
Collapse
|
3
|
Dworakowski O, Meier T, Mehl MR, Pennebaker JW, Boyd RL, Horn AB. Comparing the language style of heads of state in the US, UK, Germany and Switzerland during COVID-19. Sci Rep 2024; 14:1708. [PMID: 38242954 PMCID: PMC10799077 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-024-51362-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/24/2023] [Accepted: 01/04/2024] [Indexed: 01/21/2024] Open
Abstract
The COVID-19 pandemic posed a global threat to nearly every society around the world. Individuals turned to their political leaders to safely guide them through this crisis. The most direct way political leaders communicated with their citizens was through official speeches and press conferences. In this report, we compare psychological language markers of four different heads of state during the early stage of the pandemic. Specifically, we collected all pandemic-related speeches and press conferences delivered by political leaders in the USA (Trump), UK (Johnson), Germany (Merkel), and Switzerland (Swiss Federal Council) between February 27th and August 31st, 2020. We used natural language analysis to examine language markers of expressed positive and negative emotions, references to the community (we-talk), analytical thinking, and authenticity and compare these language markers across the four nations. Level differences in the language markers between the leaders can be detected: Trump's language was characterized by a high expression of positive emotion, Merkel's by a strong communal focus, and Johnson's and the Swiss Federal Council by a high level of analytical thinking. Overall, these findings mirror different strategies used by political leaders to deal with the COVID-19 pandemic.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Olenka Dworakowski
- URPP "Dynamics of Healthy Aging", University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland.
- Department of Psychology - Gerontopsychology, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland.
| | - Tabea Meier
- URPP "Dynamics of Healthy Aging", University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
- Department of Psychology - Gerontopsychology, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
| | - Matthias R Mehl
- Department of Psychology, University of Arizona, Tucson, USA
| | - James W Pennebaker
- Department of Psychology, The University of Texas at Austin, Austin, USA
| | - Ryan L Boyd
- Department of Computer Science, Stony Brook University, New York, USA
| | - Andrea B Horn
- URPP "Dynamics of Healthy Aging", University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
- Department of Psychology - Gerontopsychology, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
- Competence Center Gerontology, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Bor A, Jørgensen F, Petersen MB. The COVID-19 pandemic eroded system support but not social solidarity. PLoS One 2023; 18:e0288644. [PMID: 37590308 PMCID: PMC10434936 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0288644] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/18/2022] [Accepted: 06/30/2023] [Indexed: 08/19/2023] Open
Abstract
While the World was busy mitigating the disastrous health and economic effects of the novel coronavirus, a less direct, but not less concerning peril has largely remained unexplored: the COVID-19 crisis may have disrupted some of the most fundamental social and political relationships in democratic societies. We interviewed samples resembling the national population of Denmark, Hungary, Italy and the US three times: in April, June and December of 2020 (14K observations). We show that multiple (but not all) measures of support for the political system decreased between April and December. Exploiting the panel setup, we demonstrate that within-respondent increases in indicators of pandemic fatigue (specifically, the perceived subjective burden of the pandemic and feelings of anomie) correspond to decreases in system support and increases in extreme anti-systemic attitudes. At the same time, we find no systematic trends in feelings of social solidarity, which are largely unaffected by changes in pandemic burden.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alexander Bor
- Democracy Institute, Central European University, Budapest, Hungary
- Department of Political Science, Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
5
|
Re-engaging cultural differences: Culture, morality, trauma and the integration of non-Western migrants. Curr Opin Psychol 2022; 48:101454. [PMID: 36099678 DOI: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2022.101454] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/08/2022] [Revised: 07/22/2022] [Accepted: 07/27/2022] [Indexed: 01/28/2023]
Abstract
We locate our review of recent social scientific literature on non-Western migrants in Western liberal democracies within two opposing master narratives: a subtractive and an additive view of migration. Within this framework, we bring to light the contemporary conceptualizations of non-Western migrants in psychology by focusing on trauma. We then examine the cultural and moral clashes that sometimes arise from trans-global migration and the psychology of integration. We end by highlighting the importance of further research on cultural pluralism and omniculturalism to help foster more peaceful and diverse societies.
Collapse
|
6
|
Uysal MS, Jurstakova K, Uluşahin Y. An Integrative Social Identity Model of Populist Leadership. SOCIAL AND PERSONALITY PSYCHOLOGY COMPASS 2022. [DOI: 10.1111/spc3.12713] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Mete Sefa Uysal
- Department of Social Psychology Friedrich Schiller University Jena Jena Germany
- School of Psychology University of Sussex Brighton United Kingdom
| | - Klara Jurstakova
- School of Psychology and Life Sciences Canterbury Christ Church University Canterbury UK
| | - Yasemin Uluşahin
- School of Psychology and Neuroscience University of St. Andrews St Andrews UK
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Montiel CJ, Dela Paz ES, Medriano JS. Narrative expansion and "terrorist" labeling: Discursive conflict escalation by state media. JOURNAL OF SOCIAL AND POLITICAL PSYCHOLOGY 2022. [DOI: 10.5964/jspp.5577] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/20/2022] Open
Abstract
How does state rhetoric change as conflict intensifies against intrastate enemies? We forward the concept of narrative expansion and labeling, to analyze the escalatory transformation of conflict discourse by the Philippine state media. The data set includes 4,098 articles from the state’s official news agency, covering early attempts at reconciliation and the eventual failure of peace negotiations between the Philippine Government and the National Democratic Front (NDF). Analysis involves a mixed methods approach, combining computational network analytics of word networks with a qualitative interpretation of emergent themes. Results reveal a discursive shift emanating from the state’s mouthpiece, alongside the political deterioration of peace talks with the NDF. The state narrative initially expands to include not only conciliatory but also confrontational talk. Eventually combative talks dominate, including a shift in labeling the enemy as terrorist rather than rebel. Narrative expansion likewise refers to state news discursively increasing the number of social actors involved in the conflict as either enemy or ally. Our findings contribute to understanding how discursive shifts may move from conciliatory to hostile discourse in a protracted intrastate conflict.
Collapse
|
8
|
Cena L, Roccato M, Russo S. Relative deprivation, national
GDP
and right‐wing populism: A multilevel, multinational study. JOURNAL OF COMMUNITY & APPLIED SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 2022. [DOI: 10.1002/casp.2636] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Lorenzo Cena
- Department of Psychology University of Torino Torino Italy
| | | | - Silvia Russo
- Department of Psychology University of Torino Torino Italy
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Affiliation(s)
- Thomas Gries
- Department of Economics, University of Paderborn, Paderborn, Germany
| | - Veronika Müller
- School of Advanced International Studies, Johns Hopkins University, Washington, D.C
| | - John T. Jost
- Departments of Psychology, Politics, & Data Science, New York University, New York, New York
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Çakal H, Altınışık Y, Gökcekuş Ö, Eraslan EG. Why people vote for thin-centred ideology parties? A multi-level multi-country test of individual and aggregate level predictors. PLoS One 2022; 17:e0264421. [PMID: 35239674 PMCID: PMC8893635 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0264421] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/22/2021] [Accepted: 02/10/2022] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
The present research investigates the individual and aggregate level determinants of support for thin-centred ideology parties across 23 European countries. Employing a multilevel modelling approach, we analysed European Social Survey data round 7 2014 (N = 44000). Our findings show that stronger identification with one’s country and confidence in one’s ability to influence the politics positively but perceiving the system as satisfactory and responsive; trusting the institutions and people, and having positive attitudes toward minorities, i.e., immigrants and refugees, negatively predict support for populist and single issue parties. The level of human development and perceptions of corruption at the country level moderate these effects. Thus, we provide the first evidence that the populist surge is triggered by populist actors’ capacity to simultaneously invoke vertical, “ordinary” people against “the elites”, and horizontal, “us” against “threatening aliens”, categories of people as well as the sovereignty of majority over minorities. These categories and underlying social psychological processes of confidence, trust, and threats are moderated by the general level of human development and corruption perceptions in a country. It is, therefore, likely that voting for populist parties will increase as the liberally democratic countries continue to prosper and offer better opportunities for human development. Stronger emphasis on safeguarding the integrity of the economic and democratic institutions, as our findings imply, and preserving their ethical and honest, i.e., un-corrupt, nature can keep this surge under check.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hüseyin Çakal
- School of Psychology, Keele University, Keele, United Kingdom
| | | | - Ömer Gökcekuş
- Seton Hall University, South Orange, New Jersey, United States of America
| | | |
Collapse
|
11
|
Lüders A, Urbanska K, Wollast R, Nugier A, Guimond S. Bottom-up populism: How relative deprivation and populist attitudes mobilize leaderless anti-government protest. JOURNAL OF SOCIAL AND POLITICAL PSYCHOLOGY 2021. [DOI: 10.5964/jspp.7349] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/20/2022] Open
Abstract
The present research focuses on populism as a bottom-up phenomenon that emerges from shared perceptions of relative deprivation. We predict that by serving as a shared ideological basis, populist attitudes can mobilize leaderless anti-government protest across ideological boundaries. We test this prediction in the context of the French Yellow Vests movement. Using a sample of French citizens (N = 562), we compare the effects of different indicators of relative deprivation on Yellow Vest protest participation and the extent to which populist attitudes account for these relationships. Results indicate that protests were fuelled by indicators of relative deprivation at the individual and group levels. Populist attitudes were best predicted by vertical comparisons between “the people” and “the elite” and fully accounted for the relationship between this type of group relative deprivation and protesting. Conversely, populist attitudes only partially accounted for the relationships between protesting and traditional measures of relative deprivation that either contrast natives with immigrants or individuals with fellow citizens. The findings strengthen the understanding of populism as a “thin centred” belief set that can unite and mobilize those who feel unfairly disadvantaged compared to a socio-political elite.
Collapse
|
12
|
Montiel CJ, Uyheng J, Dela Paz E. The Language of Pandemic Leaderships: Mapping Political Rhetoric During the COVID-19 Outbreak. POLITICAL PSYCHOLOGY 2021; 42:747-766. [PMID: 34230725 PMCID: PMC8250800 DOI: 10.1111/pops.12753] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/28/2020] [Revised: 03/17/2021] [Accepted: 03/19/2021] [Indexed: 05/14/2023]
Abstract
This article maps political rhetoric by national leaders during the COVID-19 pandemic. We identify and characterize global variations in major rhetorical storylines invoked in publicly available speeches (N = 1201) across a sample of 26 countries. Employing a text analytics or corpus linguistics approach, we show that state heads rhetorically lead their nations by: enforcing systemic interventions, upholding global unity, encouraging communal cooperation, stoking national fervor, and assuring responsive governance. Principal component analysis further shows that country-level rhetoric is organized along emergent dimensions of cultural cognition: an agency-structure axis to define the loci of pandemic interventions and a hierarchy-egalitarianism axis which distinguishes top-down enforcement from bottom-up calls for cooperation. Furthermore, we detect a striking contrast between countries featuring populist versus cosmopolitan rhetoric, which diverged in terms of their collective meaning making around leading over versus leading with, as well as their experienced pandemic severity. We conclude with implications for understanding global pandemic leadership in an unequal world and the contributions of mixed-methods approaches to a generative political psychology in times of crisis.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Joshua Uyheng
- Ateneo de Manila University
- Carnegie Mellon University
| | | |
Collapse
|
13
|
Womick J, Eckelkamp J, Luzzo S, Ward SJ, Baker SG, Salamun A, King LA. Exposure to authoritarian values leads to lower positive affect, higher negative affect, and higher meaning in life. PLoS One 2021; 16:e0256759. [PMID: 34525099 PMCID: PMC8443031 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0256759] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/04/2021] [Accepted: 08/13/2021] [Indexed: 12/04/2022] Open
Abstract
Five studies tested the effect of exposure to authoritarian values on positive affect (PA), negative affect (NA), and meaning in life (MIL). Study 1 (N = 1,053) showed that simply completing a measure of right-wing authoritarianism (vs. not) prior to rating MIL led to higher MIL. Preregistered Study 2 (N = 1,904) showed that reading speeches by real-world authoritarians (e.g., Adolf Hitler) led to lower PA, higher NA, and higher MIL than a control passage. In preregistered Studies 3 (N = 1,573) and 4 (N = 1,512), Americans read authoritarian, egalitarian, or control messages and rated mood, MIL, and evaluated the passages. Both studies showed that egalitarian messages led to better mood and authoritarian messages led to higher MIL. Study 5 (N = 148) directly replicated these results with Canadians. Aggregating across studies (N = 3,401), moderational analyses showed that meaning in life, post manipulation, was associated with more favorable evaluations of the authoritarian passage. In addition, PA was a stronger predictor of MIL in the egalitarian and control conditions than in the authoritarian condition. Further results showed no evidence that negative mood (or disagreement) spurred the boost in MIL. Implications and future directions are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jake Womick
- University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, North Carolina, United States of America
| | - John Eckelkamp
- University of Missouri, Columbia, New York, United States of America
| | - Sam Luzzo
- University of Missouri, Columbia, New York, United States of America
| | - Sarah J. Ward
- University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign, Illinois, United States of America
| | - S. Glenn Baker
- Reed College, Portland, Oregon, United States of America
| | - Alison Salamun
- University of Missouri, Columbia, New York, United States of America
| | - Laura A. King
- University of Missouri, Columbia, New York, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Modern Forms of Populism and Social Policies: Personal Values, Populist Attitudes, and Ingroup Definitions in Support of Left-Wing and Right-Wing Welfare Policies in Italy. GENEALOGY 2021. [DOI: 10.3390/genealogy5030060] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
We analyzed the relationship between modern forms of populism and citizen support for exclusive welfare policies and proposals, and we focused on support for left-wing- and right-wing-oriented welfare policies enacted or proposed during the Lega Nord (LN)–Five Star Movement (FSM) government in Italy (2018–2019). In light of the theoretical perspective of political ideology as motivated by social cognition, we examined citizens’ support for the two policies considering adherence to populist attitudes, agreement on the criteria useful to define ingroup membership, and personal values. We also took into account the role of cognitive sophistication in populism avoidance. A total of 785 Italian adults (F = 56.6; mean age = 35.8) completed an online survey in the summer of 2019 based on the following: support for populist policies and proposals, political ideologies and positioning, personal values, and ingroup boundaries. We used correlation and regression analyses. The results highlight the relationships between populism and political conservatism. Populism was related to the vertical and horizontal borders defining the “people”; cognitive sophistication was not a relevant driver. We identified some facilitating factors that could promote adherence to and support for public policies inspired by the values of the right or of the left, without a true ideological connotation.
Collapse
|
15
|
Stathi S, Guerra R. Unpacking the social psychology of populism: A brief introductory note. JOURNAL OF THEORETICAL SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 2021. [DOI: 10.1002/jts5.98] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Sofia Stathi
- School of Human Sciences, Institute for Lifecourse Development University of Greenwich London UK
| | - Rita Guerra
- CIS‐IUL ISCTE‐Instituto Universitário de Lisboa Lisboa Portugal
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Mahendran K, English A, Nieland S. No Obvious Home: the Public’s Dialogical Creation of Home During the Third Wave of Decolonization. HUMAN ARENAS 2021. [PMCID: PMC7881309 DOI: 10.1007/s42087-020-00176-w] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
The global crises we currently face, ecological, refugee-related and dealing with austerity arising out of the Covid-19 pandemic share a common feature. Together they have the capacity to call into question shared understandings of what constitutes the physical, political and psychological boundaries of home. Consensual understanding (social representations) of home, if unexamined, risks retaining primordial, stable, bounded and historically continuous dimensions. The focus of this article, to this end, is the public’s understanding of home. The “un-homing” techniques used by populist leaders are brought into dialogue with how citizens, as dialogical selves, talk about home. The contours of common-sense on belonging are being informed; it is proposed by a third wave of decolonization. Stimulus-led interviews (N = 76) were conducted in England, Germany, Ireland, Scotland and Sweden. Dialogical analysis shows the public use two social representations relating to (i) freedom of movement and (ii) how the world is organized. These social representations decolonize home beyond national/transnational boundaries towards the transglobal. Citizens, irrespective of degree of migration, navigate future (in) securities using intergenerational dialogue. This serves to anchor transglobal migration-mobility to intergenerational continuity and the possibilities of travelling together through life. Public dialogue, when diffracted into a spectrum of positions on home, has the capacity to counter black/white, us/them, xenophobic protectionism within nationalist populism. In conclusion, scientific studies which reveal the depths of public capacity may become centrally important to post-pandemic recovery.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kesi Mahendran
- School of Psychology, The Open University, Milton Keynes, UK
| | - Anthony English
- School of Psychology, The Open University, Milton Keynes, UK
| | - Sue Nieland
- School of Psychology, The Open University, Milton Keynes, UK
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Affiliation(s)
- Séamus A Power
- Department of Psychology, University of Copenhagen, Øster Farimagsgade 2A, 1353 København K, Denmark.
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Power SA, Madsen T, Morton TA. Relative deprivation and revolt: current and future directions. Curr Opin Psychol 2020; 35:119-124. [PMID: 32674060 DOI: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2020.06.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/21/2020] [Revised: 06/03/2020] [Accepted: 06/14/2020] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
Abstract
We review research applying relative deprivation theory to comprehend social, economic, and political phenomena relating to social change. We highlight areas illuminated by relative deprivation and limitations of this contemporary research. Next, we outline four theoretical elaborations of relative deprivation theory to advance understanding of complex socio-economic and political processes of underlying rallies, riots, and revolutions. We end by suggesting methodological approaches and research agendas to understand psychological processes of social change.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Séamus A Power
- Department of Psychology, University of Copenhagen, Øster Farimagsgade 2A, 1353 København K, Denmark.
| | - Thomas Madsen
- Department of Psychology, University of Copenhagen, Øster Farimagsgade 2A, 1353 København K, Denmark
| | - Thomas A Morton
- Department of Psychology, University of Copenhagen, Øster Farimagsgade 2A, 1353 København K, Denmark
| |
Collapse
|