251
|
Affiliation(s)
- Takeshi Hamamura
- School of Psychology; Curtin University; Perth Western Australia Australia
| | | |
Collapse
|
252
|
Li X, Qian X, Lǚ X, Wang X, Ji N, Zhang M, Ren M. Upregulated structural and regulatory genes involved in anthocyanin biosynthesis for coloration of purple grains during the middle and late grain-filling stages. PLANT PHYSIOLOGY AND BIOCHEMISTRY : PPB 2018; 130:235-247. [PMID: 30014927 DOI: 10.1016/j.plaphy.2018.07.011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/14/2018] [Revised: 07/07/2018] [Accepted: 07/09/2018] [Indexed: 06/08/2023]
Abstract
Anthocyanin biosynthesis is controlled by structural and regulatory genes. Purple wheat grains accumulate anthocyanin during developmental processes. However, anthocyanin cannot accumulate at the beginning of grain formation. To understand the reason for this phenomenon, we performed observations and analyses of pigments, developmental stages, and the transcriptome of caryopsis in Triticum aestivum L. cv. Guizi 1 (GZ1). In the early grain-filling stage (10 dpa to 20 dpa), anthocyanin accumulated from nearly 0 mg·kg-1 (10 dpa) to 15.39 mg·kg-1 (20 dpa), and the expression levels of structural genes (except GzDFR) and main regulatory genes GzMYB-7D1 and GzMYC-2A1 were low. When the grains developed to the middle (20 dpa to 30 dpa) and late (30 dpa to 40 dpa) grain-filling stages, the anthocyanin content peaked at 197.31 mg·kg-1, and the expression levels of structural and regulatory genes at 25 dpa and 35 dpa were higher than that at 10 dpa. In particular, the expression levels of GzANS, Gz3GT, GzMYB-7D1, and GzMYC-2A1 were upregulated 45.74˜28.54, 765.00˜384.00, 419.00˜574.00, and 5.34˜29.05 times, respectively. Grains were also colored from green to purple. Anthocyanin accumulates in the pericarp and testa and is stored in vacuoles of epidermal and transverse cells. The major compositions are cyanidin and peonidin. These results revealed that the upregulated structural and regulatory genes in the middle and late grain-filling stages may result in the anthocyanin biosynthesis and coloration of grains, which provides new insights into anthocyanin biosynthesis and regulation mechanisms.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Xiaolan Li
- School of Life Sciences, State Engineering Technology Institute for Karst Desertification Control, Key Laboratory of Plant Resources Conservation and Germplasm Innovation in Mountainous Region (Ministry of Education), Guizhou University, Guiyang, 550025, China.
| | - Xiaokang Qian
- School of Agriculture, Guizhou Sub-Center of National Wheat Improvement Center, Guizhou University, Guiyang, 550025, China
| | - Xiang Lǚ
- School of Life Sciences, State Engineering Technology Institute for Karst Desertification Control, Key Laboratory of Plant Resources Conservation and Germplasm Innovation in Mountainous Region (Ministry of Education), Guizhou University, Guiyang, 550025, China
| | - Xiaohong Wang
- School of Life Sciences, State Engineering Technology Institute for Karst Desertification Control, Key Laboratory of Plant Resources Conservation and Germplasm Innovation in Mountainous Region (Ministry of Education), Guizhou University, Guiyang, 550025, China
| | - Ning Ji
- School of Life Sciences, State Engineering Technology Institute for Karst Desertification Control, Key Laboratory of Plant Resources Conservation and Germplasm Innovation in Mountainous Region (Ministry of Education), Guizhou University, Guiyang, 550025, China
| | - Mingsheng Zhang
- School of Life Sciences, State Engineering Technology Institute for Karst Desertification Control, Key Laboratory of Plant Resources Conservation and Germplasm Innovation in Mountainous Region (Ministry of Education), Guizhou University, Guiyang, 550025, China.
| | - Mingjian Ren
- School of Agriculture, Guizhou Sub-Center of National Wheat Improvement Center, Guizhou University, Guiyang, 550025, China.
| |
Collapse
|
253
|
Molleman L, Gächter S. Societal background influences social learning in cooperative decision making. EVOL HUM BEHAV 2018. [DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2018.05.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/16/2022]
|
254
|
Muthukrishna M, Henrich J, Toyokawa W, Hamamura T, Kameda T, Heine SJ. Overconfidence is universal? Elicitation of Genuine Overconfidence (EGO) procedure reveals systematic differences across domain, task knowledge, and incentives in four populations. PLoS One 2018; 13:e0202288. [PMID: 30161140 PMCID: PMC6116975 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0202288] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/05/2018] [Accepted: 07/31/2018] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Overconfidence is sometimes assumed to be a human universal, but there remains a dearth of data systematically measuring overconfidence across populations and contexts. Moreover, cross-cultural experiments often fail to distinguish between placement and precision and worse still, often compare population-mean placement estimates rather than individual performance subtracted from placement. Here we introduce a procedure for concurrently capturing both placement and precision at an individual level based on individual performance: The Elicitation of Genuine Overconfidence (EGO) procedure. We conducted experiments using the EGO procedure, manipulating domain, task knowledge, and incentives across four populations-Japanese, Hong Kong Chinese, Euro Canadians, and East Asian Canadians. We find that previous measures of population-level overconfidence may have been misleading; rather than universal, overconfidence is highly context dependent. Our results reveal cross-cultural differences in sensitivity to incentives and differences in overconfidence strategies, with underconfidence, accuracy, and overconfidence. Comparing sexes, we find inconsistent results for overplacement, but that males are consistently more confident in their placement. These findings have implications for our understanding of the adaptive value of overconfidence and its role in explaining population-level and individual-level differences in economic and psychological behavior.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michael Muthukrishna
- Department of Psychological and Behavioural Science, London School of Economics and Political Science, London, United Kingdom
- * E-mail:
| | - Joseph Henrich
- Department of Human Evolutionary Biology, Harvard University, Cambridge, Massachusetts, United States of America
- Canadian Institute for Advanced Research, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
| | - Wataru Toyokawa
- School of Biology, University of St Andrews, St Andrews, United Kingdom
| | - Takeshi Hamamura
- School of Psychology and Speech Pathology, Curtin University, Perth, Australia
| | - Tatsuya Kameda
- Department of Social Psychology, The University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan
| | - Steven J. Heine
- Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
255
|
Klüppel LM, Pierce L, Snyder JA. Perspective—The Deep Historical Roots of Organization and Strategy: Traumatic Shocks, Culture, and Institutions. ORGANIZATION SCIENCE 2018. [DOI: 10.1287/orsc.2017.1173] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Affiliation(s)
| | - Lamar Pierce
- Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, Missouri 63130
| | | |
Collapse
|
256
|
Zhang Z, Warren CM, Lei Y, Xing Q, Li H. Commentary: Are groups more or less than the sum of their members? The moderating role of individual identification. Front Psychol 2018; 9:999. [PMID: 29962995 PMCID: PMC6013690 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00999] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/14/2018] [Accepted: 05/28/2018] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Affiliation(s)
- Zhonglu Zhang
- Department of Psychology, Guangzhou University, Guangzhou, China.,Research Centre for Brain Function and Psychological Science, Shenzhen University, Shenzhen, China
| | | | - Yi Lei
- Research Centre for Brain Function and Psychological Science, Shenzhen University, Shenzhen, China.,Shenzhen Institute of Neuroscience, Shenzhen, China.,Institute of Affective and Social Neuroscience, Shenzhen University, Shenzhen, China
| | - Qiang Xing
- Department of Psychology, Guangzhou University, Guangzhou, China
| | - Hong Li
- Research Centre for Brain Function and Psychological Science, Shenzhen University, Shenzhen, China.,Shenzhen Institute of Neuroscience, Shenzhen, China.,Institute of Affective and Social Neuroscience, Shenzhen University, Shenzhen, China
| |
Collapse
|
257
|
Relational mobility predicts social behaviors in 39 countries and is tied to historical farming and threat. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2018; 115:7521-7526. [PMID: 29959208 PMCID: PMC6055178 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1713191115] [Citation(s) in RCA: 116] [Impact Index Per Article: 16.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Biologists and social scientists have long tried to understand why some societies have more fluid and open interpersonal relationships and how those differences influence culture. This study measures relational mobility, a socioecological variable quantifying voluntary (high relational mobility) vs. fixed (low relational mobility) interpersonal relationships. We measure relational mobility in 39 societies and test whether it predicts social behavior. People in societies with higher relational mobility report more proactive interpersonal behaviors (e.g., self-disclosure and social support) and psychological tendencies that help them build and retain relationships (e.g., general trust, intimacy, self-esteem). Finally, we explore ecological factors that could explain relational mobility differences across societies. Relational mobility was lower in societies that practiced settled, interdependent subsistence styles, such as rice farming, and in societies that had stronger ecological and historical threats.
Collapse
|
258
|
de Oliveira S, Nisbett RE. Culture Changes How We Think About Thinking: From "Human Inference" to "Geography of Thought". PERSPECTIVES ON PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE 2018; 12:782-790. [PMID: 28972847 DOI: 10.1177/1745691617702718] [Citation(s) in RCA: 54] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
Cultural comparison has challenged people's assumptions of universality in psychology. It has also revealed that many questions and approaches in psychology are not culture-free, but reflect a distinctively Western analytic framework. In this framework, the world is assumed to operate by discernible and stable rules, contradiction is a problem to be resolved, and entities are viewed as relatively independent agents. Context and relationships between people and objects are relatively downplayed-or, when they are examined, are assumed to operate under parsimonious rules. Dialectical or holistic thinking, a framework more prevalent in East Asian societies, involves greater attention to context and relationships, assumptions of change rather than stasis, and acceptance of contradiction. Analytic thinking is useful for science and daily life. But sometimes dialectical thinking results in more accurate conclusions or pragmatically useful decisions than analytic thinking. Therefore, we propose that both dialectical and analytic thinking should be consciously adopted as tools in the "cognitive toolbox" of researchers and laypeople alike. In the present article, we review the cross-cultural work demonstrating the psychological differences that analytic versus dialectical thinking produce. We then consider the strengths of each type of thinking and how they may serve complementary functions for problem solving.
Collapse
|
259
|
Misiak M, Butovskaya M, Sorokowski P. Ecology shapes moral judgments towards food-wasting behavior: Evidence from the Yali of West Papua, the Ngorongoro Maasai, and Poles. Appetite 2018; 125:124-130. [DOI: 10.1016/j.appet.2017.12.031] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/14/2017] [Revised: 10/19/2017] [Accepted: 12/24/2017] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
|
260
|
Ito A, Gobel MS, Uchida Y. Leaders in Interdependent Contexts Suppress Nonverbal Assertiveness: A Multilevel Analysis of Japanese University Club Leaders' and Members' Rank Signaling. Front Psychol 2018; 9:723. [PMID: 29904361 PMCID: PMC5991138 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00723] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/01/2018] [Accepted: 04/25/2018] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Previous research has shown that leadership is signaled through nonverbal assertiveness. However, those studies have been mostly conducted in individualistic cultural contexts, such as in the U.S. Here, we suggest that one important strategy for goal attainment in collectivistic cultures is for leaders to self-regulate their behaviors. Thus, contrary to the previous evidence from individualistic cultural contexts, in collectivistic cultural contexts, leaders might suppress nonverbal assertiveness. To test this possibility, we assessed nonverbal behaviors (NVB) of Japanese leaders and members, and how they were evaluated by observers. We recruited Japanese leaders and members of university clubs and video-recorded them while introducing their club. Then, we coded their nonverbal rank signaling behavior. Finally, we asked a new set of naïve observers to watch these video-clips and to judge targets' suitability for being possible club leaders. Results of a multilevel analysis (level 1: individual participants, level 2: clubs) suggested that the more the club culture focused on tasks (rather than relationships), the more likely were leaders (but not members) of those clubs to suppress their nonverbal assertiveness. Naïve observers judged individuals who restrained from emitting nonverbal assertiveness as being more suitable and worthy club leaders. Thus, our findings demonstrate the cultural fit between contextual effects at the collective level (i.e., cultural orientation of a group) and the signaling and perceiving of social ranks at the individual level (i.e., suppression of nonverbal assertiveness). We discuss the importance of studying the cultural fit between the collective reality that people inhabit and people's psychology for future research in cultural psychology.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Atsuki Ito
- Graduate School of Human and Environmental Studies, Kyoto University, Kyoto, Japan
| | - Matthias S Gobel
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Sage Center for the Study of the Mind, University of California, Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, CA, United States
| | - Yukiko Uchida
- Kokoro Research Center, Kyoto University, Kyoto, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
261
|
Milfont TL, Klein RA. Replication and Reproducibility in Cross-Cultural Psychology. JOURNAL OF CROSS-CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY 2018. [DOI: 10.1177/0022022117744892] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
Replication is the scientific gold standard that enables the confirmation of research findings. Concerns related to publication bias, flexibility in data analysis, and high-profile cases of academic misconduct have led to recent calls for more replication and systematic accumulation of scientific knowledge in psychological science. This renewed emphasis on replication may pose specific challenges to cross-cultural research due to inherent practical difficulties in emulating an original study in other cultural groups. The purpose of the present article is to discuss how the core concepts of this replication debate apply to cross-cultural psychology. Distinct to replications in cross-cultural research are examinations of bias and equivalence in manipulations and procedures, and that targeted research populations may differ in meaningful ways. We identify issues in current psychological research (analytic flexibility, low power) and possible solutions (preregistration, power analysis), and discuss ways to implement best practices in cross-cultural replication attempts.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Taciano L. Milfont
- Centre for Applied Cross-Cultural Research, Victoria University of Wellington, New Zealand
| | | |
Collapse
|
262
|
Liu P, Chan D, Qiu L, Tov W, Tong VJC. Effects of Cultural Tightness-Looseness and Social Network Density on Expression of Positive and Negative Emotions: A Large-Scale Study of Impression Management by Facebook Users. PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN 2018; 44:1567-1581. [PMID: 29742997 DOI: 10.1177/0146167218770999] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
Using data from 13,789 Facebook users across U.S. states, this study examined the main effects of societal-level cultural tightness-looseness and its interaction effects with individuals' social network density on impression management (IM) in terms of online emotional expression. Results showed that individuals from culturally tight (vs. loose) states were more likely to express positive emotions and less likely to express negative emotions. Meanwhile, for positive emotional expression, there was a tightness-looseness by social network density interaction effect. In culturally tight states, individuals with dense (vs. sparse) networks were more likely to express positive emotions, while in culturally loose states this pattern was reversed. For negative emotional expression, however, no such interaction was observed. Our findings highlight the influence of cultural norms and social network structure on emotional expressions as IM strategies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Pan Liu
- 1 Singapore Management University, Singapore
| | - David Chan
- 1 Singapore Management University, Singapore
| | - Lin Qiu
- 2 Nanyang Technological University, Singapore
| | - William Tov
- 1 Singapore Management University, Singapore
| | | |
Collapse
|
263
|
Cai H, Zou X, Feng Y, Liu Y, Jing Y. Increasing Need for Uniqueness in Contemporary China: Empirical Evidence. Front Psychol 2018; 9:554. [PMID: 29867619 PMCID: PMC5952033 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00554] [Citation(s) in RCA: 31] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/08/2017] [Accepted: 04/03/2018] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Past research has documented various cultural and psychological changes in contemporary China. In two studies, we examine how Chinese people's need for uniqueness (NFU) also has changed. In Study 1, we found a significant cross-generational increase in Chinese participants' self-reported NFU. In Study 2, we sampled the names of Chinese newborn babies over the last five decades and found that parents have been increasingly likely to use unique characters to name their children. These findings suggest that the NFU has been rising in China, a historically collectivistic-oriented society. Theoretical and practical implications of our findings were discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Huajian Cai
- Key Laboratory of Behavioral Sciences, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China
| | - Xi Zou
- London Business School, London, United Kingdom
| | - Yi Feng
- Key Laboratory of Behavioral Sciences, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China
| | - Yunzhi Liu
- Key Laboratory of Behavioral Sciences, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China
| | - Yiming Jing
- Key Laboratory of Behavioral Sciences, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China
| |
Collapse
|
264
|
Culture, economic development, social-network type, and mortality: Evidence from Chinese older adults. Soc Sci Med 2018; 204:23-30. [DOI: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2018.03.021] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/26/2017] [Revised: 03/08/2018] [Accepted: 03/11/2018] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
|
265
|
Talhelm T, Zhang X, Oishi S. Moving chairs in Starbucks: Observational studies find rice-wheat cultural differences in daily life in China. SCIENCE ADVANCES 2018; 4:eaap8469. [PMID: 29707634 PMCID: PMC5916507 DOI: 10.1126/sciadv.aap8469] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/06/2017] [Accepted: 03/06/2018] [Indexed: 06/08/2023]
Abstract
Traditional paddy rice farmers had to share labor and coordinate irrigation in a way that most wheat farmers did not. We observed people in everyday life to test whether these agricultural legacies gave rice-farming southern China a more interdependent culture and wheat-farming northern China a more independent culture. In Study 1, we counted 8964 people sitting in cafes in six cities and found that people in northern China were more likely to be sitting alone. In Study 2, we moved chairs together in Starbucks across the country so that they were partially blocking the aisle (n = 678). People in northern China were more likely to move the chair out of the way, which is consistent with findings that people in individualistic cultures are more likely to try to control the environment. People in southern China were more likely to adjust the self to the environment by squeezing through the chairs. Even in China's most modern cities, rice-wheat differences live on in everyday life.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Thomas Talhelm
- University of Chicago Booth School of Business, Chicago, IL 60637, USA
| | - Xuemin Zhang
- Beijing Key Laboratory of Applied Experimental Psychology and Faculty of Psychology, State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning, Beijing Normal University, Beijing 100875, China
| | | |
Collapse
|
266
|
Pang D, Proyer RT. An Initial Cross-Cultural Comparison of Adult Playfulness in Mainland China and German-Speaking Countries. Front Psychol 2018; 9:421. [PMID: 29651265 PMCID: PMC5885041 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00421] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/15/2017] [Accepted: 03/13/2018] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Compared with playfulness in infants and children, playfulness in adults is relatively under-studied. Although there is no empirical research comparing differences in adult playfulness across cultures, one might expect variations between Western and Eastern societies such as China. While playfulness is typically seen as a positive trait in Western culture, there are hints in Chinese culture that being playful has negative connotations (e.g., associations with laziness and seeing play as the opposite of work). The aim of this study was to compare expressions of playfulness in one sample from German-speaking countries (n = 143) and two samples from China (Guangzhou: n = 176; Beijing: n = 100). Participants completed one playfulness scale developed in the West (Short Measure of Adult Playfulness, SMAP) and one from the East (Adult Playfulness Questionnaire, APQ). Additional ratings of the participants were collected to measure: (a) the level of playful behavior expressed by people in different situations (e.g., when being around family members, in public, or on social media), and (b) individuals' perceptions of society's expectations concerning the appropriateness of being playful in the given situations. Overall, the results of the comparisons were mixed. Although SMAP scores did not vary significantly across the three samples, people from German-speaking countries tended to score higher on some facets of the APQ and some situational ratings. Stronger effects were found when comparing only the German-speaking sample and the Guangzhou sample. In addition to the cross-cultural differences that we expected, we also detected Chinese regional variations (North vs. South). We conclude that societal rules and cultural factors may impact expressions of playfulness in a society.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dandan Pang
- Personality and Assessment, Department of Psychology, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
| | - René T. Proyer
- Personality and Assessment, Department of Psychology, Martin Luther University Halle-Wittenberg, Halle, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
267
|
Grace D, Lindahl J, Wanyoike F, Bett B, Randolph T, Rich KM. Poor livestock keepers: ecosystem-poverty-health interactions. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2018; 372:rstb.2016.0166. [PMID: 28584174 PMCID: PMC5468691 DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2016.0166] [Citation(s) in RCA: 42] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 10/21/2016] [Indexed: 12/23/2022] Open
Abstract
Humans have never been healthier, wealthier or more numerous. Yet, present success may be at the cost of future prosperity and in some places, especially in sub-Saharan Africa, poverty persists. Livestock keepers, especially pastoralists, are over-represented among the poor. Poverty has been mainly attributed to a lack of access, whether to goods, education or enabling institutions. More recent insights suggest ecosystems may influence poverty and the self-reinforcing mechanisms that constitute poverty traps in more subtle ways. The plausibility of zoonoses as poverty traps is strengthened by landmark studies on disease burden in recent years. While in theory, endemic zoonoses are best controlled in the animal host, in practice, communities are often left to manage disease themselves, with the focus on treatment rather than prevention. We illustrate this with results from a survey on health costs in a pastoral ecosystem. Epidemic zoonoses are more likely to elicit official responses, but these can have unintended consequences that deepen poverty traps. In this context, a systems understanding of disease control can lead to more effective and pro-poor disease management. We illustrate this with an example of how a system dynamics model can help optimize responses to Rift Valley fever outbreaks in Kenya by giving decision makers real-time access to the costs of the delay in vaccinating. In conclusion, a broader, more ecological understanding of poverty and of the appropriate responses to the diseases of poverty can contribute to improved livelihoods for livestock keepers in Africa. This article is part of the themed issue ‘One Health for a changing world: zoonoses, ecosystems and human well-being’.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Delia Grace
- International Livestock Research Institute, Box 30709, Nairobi, Kenya
| | - Johanna Lindahl
- International Livestock Research Institute, Box 30709, Nairobi, Kenya.,Department of Medical Biochemistry and Microbiology, Uppsala University, 751 05 Uppsala, Sweden
| | - Francis Wanyoike
- International Livestock Research Institute, Box 30709, Nairobi, Kenya
| | - Bernard Bett
- International Livestock Research Institute, Box 30709, Nairobi, Kenya
| | - Tom Randolph
- International Livestock Research Institute, Box 30709, Nairobi, Kenya
| | - Karl M Rich
- Lincoln University, Lincoln 7647, New Zealand
| |
Collapse
|
268
|
Climate, aggression, and violence (CLASH): a cultural-evolutionary approach. Curr Opin Psychol 2018; 19:113-118. [DOI: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.04.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/01/2017] [Accepted: 04/12/2017] [Indexed: 12/22/2022]
|
269
|
Van de Vliert E, Murray DR. Climate and Creativity: Cold and Heat Trigger Invention and Innovation in Richer Populations. CREATIVITY RESEARCH JOURNAL 2018. [DOI: 10.1080/10400419.2018.1411571] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
|
270
|
Alesina A, Giuliano P, Nunn N. Traditional agricultural practices and the sex ratio today. PLoS One 2018; 13:e0190510. [PMID: 29338023 PMCID: PMC5770021 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0190510] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/21/2017] [Accepted: 12/15/2017] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
Abstract
We study the historical origins of cross-country differences in the male-to-female sex ratio. Our analysis focuses on the use of the plough in traditional agriculture. In societies that did not use the plough, women tended to participate in agriculture as actively as men. By contrast, in societies that used the plough, men specialized in agricultural work, due to the physical strength needed to pull the plough or control the animal that pulls it. We hypothesize that this difference caused plough-using societies to value boys more than girls. Today, this belief is reflected in male-biased sex ratios, which arise due to sex-selective abortion or infanticide, or gender-differences in access to family resources, which results in higher mortality rates for girls. Testing this hypothesis, we show that descendants of societies that traditionally practiced plough agriculture today have higher average male-to-female sex ratios. We find that this effect systematically increases in magnitude and statistical significance as one looks at older cohorts. Estimates using instrumental variables confirm our findings from multivariate OLS analysis.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alberto Alesina
- Department of Economics, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, United States of America
- NBER, Cambridge, MA, United States of America
| | - Paola Giuliano
- NBER, Cambridge, MA, United States of America
- Anderson School of Management, UCLA, Los Angeles, CA, United States of America
- * E-mail:
| | - Nathan Nunn
- Department of Economics, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, United States of America
- NBER, Cambridge, MA, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
271
|
Talhelm T. Hong Kong Liberals Are WEIRD: Analytic Thought Increases Support for Liberal Policies. PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN 2018; 44:717-728. [PMID: 29338556 DOI: 10.1177/0146167217746151] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
This study tests whether liberals and conservatives within the same society think as if they were from different cultures. I tested this by measuring the cultural thought style of social liberals and conservatives in Hong Kong (Study 1). Liberals tended to think more analytically (more "WEIRD"), and conservatives tended to think more holistically (more common in East Asia). In Study 2, I trained people to think analytically or holistically before they read articles on political issues. Analytic thought caused people to form more liberal opinions, and holistic thought caused people to form more conservative opinions. The thought training affected participants' responses to a social issue, but not an economic issue or whether they identified as liberal or conservative. This study replicates a previous U.S. finding in an East Asian culture and a different political environment, suggesting that the link between politics and thought style extends beyond the United States.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Thomas Talhelm
- 1 The University of Chicago Booth School of Business, IL, USA
| |
Collapse
|
272
|
Roberts SO, Guo C, Ho AK, Gelman SA. Children’s descriptive-to-prescriptive tendency replicates (and varies) cross-culturally: Evidence from China. J Exp Child Psychol 2018; 165:148-160. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2017.03.018] [Citation(s) in RCA: 34] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/14/2016] [Revised: 03/21/2017] [Accepted: 03/24/2017] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
|
273
|
Matheson K, Bombay A, Anisman H. Culture as an ingredient of personalized medicine. J Psychiatry Neurosci 2018; 43:3-6. [PMID: 29252161 PMCID: PMC5747533 DOI: 10.1503/jpn.170234] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/14/2022] Open
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Hymie Anisman
- Correspondence to: H. Anisman, Department of Neuroscience, Carleton University, Ottawa ON K1S 5B6;
| |
Collapse
|
274
|
Wei W, Lu JG, Galinsky AD, Wu H, Gosling SD, Rentfrow PJ, Yuan W, Zhang Q, Guo Y, Zhang M, Gui W, Guo XY, Potter J, Wang J, Li B, Li X, Han YM, Lv M, Guo XQ, Choe Y, Lin W, Yu K, Bai Q, Shang Z, Han Y, Wang L. Regional ambient temperature is associated with human personality. Nat Hum Behav 2017; 1:890-895. [PMID: 31024181 DOI: 10.1038/s41562-017-0240-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 60] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/15/2017] [Accepted: 10/11/2017] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
Abstract
Human personality traits differ across geographical regions 1-5 . However, it remains unclear what generates these geographical personality differences. Because humans constantly experience and react to ambient temperature, we propose that temperature is a crucial environmental factor that is associated with individuals' habitual behavioural patterns and, therefore, with fundamental dimensions of personality. To test the relationship between ambient temperature and personality, we conducted two large-scale studies in two geographically large yet culturally distinct countries: China and the United States. Using data from 59 Chinese cities (N = 5,587), multilevel analyses and machine learning analyses revealed that compared with individuals who grew up in regions with less clement temperatures, individuals who grew up in regions with more clement temperatures (that is, closer to 22 °C) scored higher on personality factors related to socialization and stability (agreeableness, conscientiousness, and emotional stability) and personal growth and plasticity (extraversion and openness to experience). These relationships between temperature clemency and personality factors were replicated in a larger dataset of 12,499 ZIP-code level locations (the lowest geographical level feasible) in the United States (N = 1,660,638). Taken together, our findings provide a perspective on how and why personalities vary across geographical regions beyond past theories (subsistence style theory, selective migration theory and pathogen prevalence theory). As climate change continues across the world, we may also observe concomitant changes in human personality.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Wenqi Wei
- School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences and Beijing Key Laboratory for Behavior and Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing, China.,Center for Biomedical Informatics, College of Medicine, Texas A&M University Health Science Center, Bryan, TX, USA
| | - Jackson G Lu
- Columbia Business School, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA
| | - Adam D Galinsky
- Columbia Business School, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA
| | - Han Wu
- School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences and Beijing Key Laboratory for Behavior and Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing, China
| | - Samuel D Gosling
- Department of Psychology, University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX, USA.,School of Psychological Sciences, University of Melbourne, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia
| | - Peter J Rentfrow
- Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
| | - Wenjie Yuan
- School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences and Beijing Key Laboratory for Behavior and Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing, China
| | - Qi Zhang
- School of Psychology, Liaoning Normal University, Dalian, China
| | - Yongyu Guo
- School of Psychology, Nanjing Normal University, Nanjing, China
| | - Ming Zhang
- Department of Psychology, Soochow University, Suzhou, China
| | - Wenjing Gui
- School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences and Beijing Key Laboratory for Behavior and Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing, China
| | - Xiao-Yi Guo
- School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences and Beijing Key Laboratory for Behavior and Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing, China
| | | | | | - Bingtan Li
- School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences and Beijing Key Laboratory for Behavior and Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing, China
| | - Xiaojie Li
- School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences and Beijing Key Laboratory for Behavior and Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing, China
| | - Yang-Mei Han
- School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences and Beijing Key Laboratory for Behavior and Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing, China
| | - Meizhen Lv
- School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences and Beijing Key Laboratory for Behavior and Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing, China
| | - Xiang-Qing Guo
- School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences and Beijing Key Laboratory for Behavior and Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing, China
| | - Yera Choe
- School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences and Beijing Key Laboratory for Behavior and Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing, China
| | - Weipeng Lin
- Department of Human Resource Management, Business School, Nankai University, Tianjin, China
| | - Kun Yu
- School of Labor and Human Resources, Renmin University of China, Beijing, China
| | - Qiyu Bai
- School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences and Beijing Key Laboratory for Behavior and Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing, China
| | - Zhe Shang
- Guanghua School of Management, Peking University, Beijing, China
| | - Ying Han
- School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences and Beijing Key Laboratory for Behavior and Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing, China
| | - Lei Wang
- School of Psychological and Cognitive Sciences and Beijing Key Laboratory for Behavior and Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing, China.
| |
Collapse
|
275
|
Hamamura T. A cultural psychological analysis of cultural change. ASIAN JOURNAL OF SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 2017. [DOI: 10.1111/ajsp.12194] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/30/2022]
|
276
|
Apanovich VV, Bezdenezhnykh BN, Sams M, Jääskeläinen IP, Alexandrov Y. Event-related potentials during individual, cooperative, and competitive task performance differ in subjects with analytic vs. holistic thinking. Int J Psychophysiol 2017; 123:136-142. [PMID: 28986326 DOI: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2017.10.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/19/2017] [Revised: 09/06/2017] [Accepted: 10/03/2017] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
It has been presented that Western cultures (USA, Western Europe) are mostly characterized by competitive forms of social interaction, whereas Eastern cultures (Japan, China, Russia) are mostly characterized by cooperative forms. It has also been stated that thinking in Eastern countries is predominantly holistic and in Western countries analytic. Based on this, we hypothesized that subjects with analytic vs. holistic thinking styles show differences in decision making in different types of social interaction conditions. We investigated behavioural and brain-activity differences between subjects with analytic and holistic thinking during a choice reaction time (ChRT) task, wherein the subjects either cooperated, competed (in pairs), or performed the task without interaction with other participants. Healthy Russian subjects (N=78) were divided into two groups based on having analytic or holistic thinking as determined with an established questionnaire. We measured reaction times as well as event-related brain potentials. There were significant differences between the interaction conditions in task performance between subjects with analytic and holistic thinking. Both behavioral performance and physiological measures exhibited higher variance in holistic than in analytic subjects. Differences in amplitude and P300 latency suggest that decision making was easier for the holistic subjects in the cooperation condition, in contrast to analytic subjects for whom decision making based on these measures seemed to be easier in the competition condition. The P300 amplitude was higher in the individual condition as compared with the collective conditions. Overall, our results support the notion that the brains of analytic and holistic subjects work differently in different types of social interaction conditions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- V V Apanovich
- Laboratory of Neural Bases of Mind, Institute of Psychology, Russian Academy of Sciences, Yaroslavskaya str., 13, 129366 Moscow, Russia.
| | - B N Bezdenezhnykh
- Laboratory of Neural Bases of Mind, Institute of Psychology, Russian Academy of Sciences, Yaroslavskaya str., 13, 129366 Moscow, Russia
| | - M Sams
- Brain and Mind Laboratory, Department of Neuroscience and Biomedical Engineering, Aalto University School of Science, FIN-00076 Espoo, Finland
| | - I P Jääskeläinen
- Brain and Mind Laboratory, Department of Neuroscience and Biomedical Engineering, Aalto University School of Science, FIN-00076 Espoo, Finland
| | - YuI Alexandrov
- Laboratory of Neural Bases of Mind, Institute of Psychology, Russian Academy of Sciences, Yaroslavskaya str., 13, 129366 Moscow, Russia; Department of Psychology, National Research University Higher School of Economics, Russian Federation, Moscow, Russia
| |
Collapse
|
277
|
Wang Q. Why Should We All Be Cultural Psychologists? Lessons From the Study of Social Cognition. PERSPECTIVES ON PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE 2017; 11:583-596. [PMID: 27694456 DOI: 10.1177/1745691616645552] [Citation(s) in RCA: 57] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/22/2022]
Abstract
I call the attention of psychologists to the pivotal role of cultural psychology in extending and enriching research programs. I argue that it is not enough to simply acknowledge the importance of culture and urge psychologists to practice cultural psychology in their research. I deconstruct five assumptions about cultural psychology that seriously undermine its contribution to the building of a true psychological science, including that cultural psychology (a) is only about finding group differences, (b) does not appertain to group similarities, (c) concerns only group-level analysis, (d) is irrelevant to basic psychological processes, and (e) is used only to confirm the generalizability of theories. I discuss how cultural psychology can provide unique insights into psychological processes and further equip researchers with additional tools to understand human behavior. Drawing lessons from the 20 years of cultural research that my colleagues and I have done on the development of social cognition, including autobiographical memory, future thinking, self, and emotion knowledge, I demonstrate that incorporating cultural psychology into research programs is not only necessary but also feasible.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Qi Wang
- Department of Human Development, Cornell University
| |
Collapse
|
278
|
Abstract
U.S. American cultures and psyches reflect and promote independence. Devos and Banaji (2005) asked, does American equal White? This article asks, does American equal independent? The answer is that when compared to people in East Asian or South Asian contexts, people in American contexts tend to show an independent psychological signature—a sense of self as individual, separate, influencing others and the world, free from influence, and equal to, if not better than, others (Markus & Conner, 2013). Independence is a reasonable description of the selves of people in the White, middle-class American mainstream. Yet it is a less good characterization of the selves of the majority of Americans who are working-class and/or people of color. A cultural psychological approach reveals that much of North American psychology is still grounded in an independent model of the self and, as such, neglects social contexts and the psychologies of a majority of Americans. Given the prominence of independence in American ideas and institutions, the interdependent tendencies that arise from intersections of national culture with social class, race, and ethnicity go unrecognized and are often misunderstood and stigmatized. This unseen clash of independence and interdependence is a significant factor in many challenges, including those of education, employment, health, immigration, criminal justice, and political polarization.
Collapse
|
279
|
Hong YY, Cheon BK. How Does Culture Matter in the Face of Globalization? PERSPECTIVES ON PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE 2017; 12:810-823. [DOI: 10.1177/1745691617700496] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
Globalization has made exposure to multiple cultures not only possible, but often necessary and unavoidable. This article focuses on how people react and adapt to increasing globalization and multiculturalism. We posit that reactions to multiculturalism and intercultural contact are not universal and are themselves shaped by cultural experiences. That is, culture provides a frame of reference for reconciling and negotiating the inflow of foreign cultures and peoples. Although exposure to foreign cultures can widen one’s worldview, thereby enhancing creativity and reducing prejudice, intercultural contact can also bring about negative exclusionary responses such as aversion, disgust, and defensiveness. We explore how culture and individual differences, such as a belief in racial essentialism, critically shape reactions to intercultural contact. Our discussion sheds light on recent geopolitical and societal shifts that reflect an increased backlash against rising globalization and cultural diversity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Bobby K. Cheon
- Division of Psychology, Nanyang Technological University
- Singapore Institute for Clinical Sciences, Agency for Science, Technology and Research (A*STAR)
| |
Collapse
|
280
|
Yamada J, Kito M, Yuki M. Passion, Relational Mobility, and Proof of Commitment: A Comparative Socio-Ecological Analysis of an Adaptive Emotion in a Sexual Market. EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY 2017; 15:1474704917746056. [PMID: 29237298 PMCID: PMC10480844 DOI: 10.1177/1474704917746056] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/28/2017] [Accepted: 11/06/2017] [Indexed: 09/09/2023] Open
Abstract
Although monogamy, the exclusive bonding with a specific partner, is one characteristic of modern human mating, long-term romantic relationships inherently possess the commitment problem, which is the conflict between maintaining a relationship with a certain partner and seeking attractive alternatives. Frank has argued that love and passion help solve this problem because they make individuals commit voluntarily to the relationship, leading the other party to also be committed with less concern over being cheated on or rejected. Combining this idea with the comparative socio-ecological approach, we hypothesize that passion will be more pronounced in social environments in which people have greater freedom to choose and replace their partners (i.e., high relational mobility) than in societies in which relationships tend to be more stable and hard to change (i.e., low relational mobility). To test this hypothesis, we compared Americans (living in a society with high relational mobility) and Japanese (living in a society with low relational mobility). As predicted, Americans were more passionate toward their romantic partners than Japanese, and this cultural difference was partially explained by the levels of perceived relational mobility in participants' local ecology. Moreover, more intense passion was found to lead to greater commitment behaviors in both societies. The importance of taking socioecological factors into consideration for the theory of the adaptive function of interpersonal emotions is also discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Junko Yamada
- Department of Behavioral Science, Hokkaido University, Sapporo, Japan
| | - Mie Kito
- Department of Sociology, Meiji Gakuin University, Tokyo, Japan
| | - Masaki Yuki
- Department of Behavioral Science, Hokkaido University, Sapporo, Japan
- Center for Experimental Research in Social Sciences, Hokkaido University, Sapporo, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
281
|
Varnum MEW, Grossmann I. Cultural Change: The How and the Why. PERSPECTIVES ON PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE 2017; 12:956-972. [DOI: 10.1177/1745691617699971] [Citation(s) in RCA: 73] [Impact Index Per Article: 9.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/05/2023]
Abstract
More than half a century of cross-cultural research has demonstrated group-level differences in psychological and behavioral phenomena, from values to attention to neural responses. However, cultures are not static, with several specific changes documented for cultural products, practices, and values. How and why do societies change? Here we juxtapose theory and insights from cultural evolution and social ecology. Evolutionary approaches enable an understanding of the how of cultural change, suggesting transmission mechanisms by which the contents of culture may change. Ecological approaches provide insights into the why of cultural change: They identify specific environmental pressures, which evoke shifts in psychology and thereby enable greater precision in predictions of specific cultural changes based on changes in ecological conditions. Complementary insights from the ecological and cultural evolutionary approaches can jointly clarify the process by which cultures change. We end by discussing the relevance of cultural change research for the contemporary societal shifts and by highlighting several critical challenges and future directions for the emerging field of cross-temporal research on culture and psychology.
Collapse
|
282
|
de Oliveira S, Nisbett RE. Beyond East and West: Cognitive Style in Latin America. JOURNAL OF CROSS-CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY 2017. [DOI: 10.1177/0022022117730816] [Citation(s) in RCA: 40] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
|
283
|
Kitayama S, Salvador CE. Culture Embrained: Going Beyond the Nature-Nurture Dichotomy. PERSPECTIVES ON PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE 2017; 12:841-854. [PMID: 28972851 PMCID: PMC5841951 DOI: 10.1177/1745691617707317] [Citation(s) in RCA: 38] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
Over the past three decades, the cultural psychology literature has established that there is systematic cultural variation in the nature of agency in the domains of cognition, emotion, and motivation. This literature adopted both self-report and performance-based (or behavioral) indicators of these processes, which set the stage for a more recent systematic exploration of cultural influences at the neural and biological level. Moreover, previous work has largely focused on East-West differences, thereby calling for a systematic exploration of other ethnic groups. To address these issues, this article reviews recent work in cultural neuroscience, while paying close attention to Latino Americans-the single most rapidly growing minority group in the United States. We focus on research that has employed neural measures and show that culture has systematic influences on the brain. We also point out that, unlike more traditional self-report or performance-based measures, neural indicators of culture are reliably linked to theoretically relevant individual difference variables such as self-construal and acculturation. Cultural neuroscience offers the framework to go beyond the dichotomy between nature and nurture and to explore how they may dynamically interact.
Collapse
|
284
|
Tominaga H, Uchida Y, Miyamoto Y, Yamasaki T. Negative Affect during a Collective (but Not an Individual) Task Is Associated with Holistic Attention in East Asian Cultural Context. Front Psychol 2017; 8:1283. [PMID: 28824483 PMCID: PMC5543282 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01283] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/29/2016] [Accepted: 07/13/2017] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Previous studies have suggested that individuals from East Asian cultures are more likely to show holistic attention-a pattern of attention that incorporates contextual information into focal stimuli-than individuals from North American cultures. Holistic attention is also prevalent in communities that require close cooperation. However, it is not yet known how cooperation is related to holistic attention. We theorized that holistic attention increases when people experience negative affect (e.g., worry, sadness, and frustration) during collective tasks (but not during individual tasks) because negative affect in social contexts signals the existence of potential threats to social harmony, thus indicating a need to restore social harmony. To examine this hypothesis, an experiment was conducted in which participants performed a musical duet either with another participant (a collective task requiring cooperation), or individually with a computer (an individual task). After the musical task, the Framed Line Task (FLT) was administered to examine their holistic attention. Participants also reported their emotional states both before and after the music task. Results suggested that negative affect in the collective task-but not the individual task-was positively correlated with a holistic pattern of attention. The function of negative affect in social contexts as motivation to restore relationships and how this enhances holistic attention is discussed. The moderating effect of social context on the link between negative affect and cognition is also discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hitoshi Tominaga
- Graduate School of Human and Environmental Studies, Kyoto UniversityKyoto, Japan
| | - Yukiko Uchida
- Kokoro Research Center, Kyoto UniversityKyoto, Japan
| | - Yuri Miyamoto
- Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin-MadisonMadison, WI, United States
| | - Teruo Yamasaki
- Department of Psychology, Osaka Shoin Women's UniversityHigashi-Osaka, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
285
|
Manfredo MJ, Bruskotter JT, Teel TL, Fulton D, Schwartz SH, Arlinghaus R, Oishi S, Uskul AK, Redford K, Kitayama S, Sullivan L. Why social values cannot be changed for the sake of conservation. CONSERVATION BIOLOGY : THE JOURNAL OF THE SOCIETY FOR CONSERVATION BIOLOGY 2017; 31:772-780. [PMID: 27757996 DOI: 10.1111/cobi.12855] [Citation(s) in RCA: 96] [Impact Index Per Article: 12.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/14/2016] [Revised: 08/17/2016] [Accepted: 10/06/2016] [Indexed: 05/26/2023]
Abstract
The hope for creating widespread change in social values has endured among conservation professionals since early calls by Aldo Leopold for a "land ethic." However, there has been little serious attention in conservation to the fields of investigation that address values, how they are formed, and how they change. We introduce a social-ecological systems conceptual approach in which values are seen not only as motivational goals people hold but also as ideas that are deeply embedded in society's material culture, collective behaviors, traditions, and institutions. Values define and bind groups, organizations, and societies; serve an adaptive role; and are typically stable across generations. When abrupt value changes occur, they are in response to substantial alterations in the social-ecological context. Such changes build on prior value structures and do not result in complete replacement. Given this understanding of values, we conclude that deliberate efforts to orchestrate value shifts for conservation are unlikely to be effective. Instead, there is an urgent need for research on values with a multilevel and dynamic view that can inform innovative conservation strategies for working within existing value structures. New directions facilitated by a systems approach will enhance understanding of the role values play in shaping conservation challenges and improve management of the human component of conservation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michael J Manfredo
- Department of Human Dimensions of Natural Resources, Colorado State University, 1480 Campus Delivery, Fort Collins, CO, 80523-1480, U.S.A
| | - Jeremy T Bruskotter
- School of Environment and Natural Resources, Ohio State University, 210 Kottman Hall, 2021 Coffey Road, Columbus, OH, 43210, U.S.A
| | - Tara L Teel
- Department of Human Dimensions of Natural Resources, Colorado State University, 1480 Campus Delivery, Fort Collins, CO, 80523-1480, U.S.A
| | - David Fulton
- U.S. Geological Survey, Minnesota Cooperative Fish and Wildlife Research Unit, University of Minnesota, 1980 Folwell Avenue, 200 Hodson Hall, Saint Paul, MN, 55108, U.S.A
| | - Shalom H Schwartz
- Department of Psychology, The Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Mount Scopus, Jerusalem, 91905, Israel
| | - Robert Arlinghaus
- Leibniz-Institute of Freshwater Ecology and Inland Fisheries, Müggelseedamm 310, 12587, Berlin, Germany
| | - Shigehiro Oishi
- Department of Psychology, University of Virginia, 102 Gilmer Hall, P.O. Box 400400, Charlottesville, VA, 22904, U.S.A
| | - Ayse K Uskul
- School of Psychology, Keynes College, University of Kent, Canterbury, Kent, CT2 7NP, U.K
| | - Kent Redford
- Archipelago Consulting, P.O. Box 4750, Portland, ME, 04112, U.S.A
| | - Shinobu Kitayama
- Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, 1004 East Hall, 530 Church Street, Ann Arbor, MI, 48109-1043, U.S.A
| | - Leeann Sullivan
- Department of Human Dimensions of Natural Resources, Colorado State University, 1480 Campus Delivery, Fort Collins, CO, 80523-1480, U.S.A
| |
Collapse
|
286
|
Rabinovich A, Morton TA. Paradoxes of praise: Identity-inconsistent praise results in praise-inconsistent responses. EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 2017. [DOI: 10.1002/ejsp.2243] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/05/2022]
|
287
|
Mizoshita T, Katano T, Tanida S, Hirano A, Miyaki T, Ozeki K, Suzuki Y, Sugimura N, Kataoka H, Joh T. Prospective comparison of preference and efficacy of adalimumab and infliximab for treating ulcerative colitis naive to antitumor necrosis factor therapy. Medicine (Baltimore) 2017; 96:e7800. [PMID: 28796080 PMCID: PMC5556246 DOI: 10.1097/md.0000000000007800] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/06/2023] Open
Abstract
There have been few reports on 2 tumor necrosis factor alpha inhibitors, infliximab and adalimumab, with respect to patient preference and efficacy in ulcerative colitis (UC).We used questionnaires to evaluate the preference and reasons for drug choice between infliximab and adalimumab in UC patients naive to antitumor necrosis factor alpha therapy. We also analyzed the efficacy of infliximab and adalimumab prospectively and endoscopically before treatment and at 14 and 54 weeks.Of the 25 UC patients, infliximab and adalimumab were chosen by 10 (40%) and 15 (60%), respectively. Patients who favored infliximab considered "fear of syringes" (7/10, 70%) as the most important influencing factor, whereas patients who favored adalimumab considered "ease of administration" (10/15, 66.7%) and "time required for therapy" (10/15, 66.7%) as the most important factors. There were no statistical differences in remission induction and maintenance between the infliximab and adalimumab groups with regard to response, remission, mucosal healing, steroid-free, and steroid-free remission rates at weeks 14 and 54.The efficacy of adalimumab in remission induction and maintenance was equivalent to that of infliximab in UC patients naive to antitumor necrosis factor alpha therapy in this prospective study, but more patients preferred adalimumab.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tsutomu Mizoshita
- Department of Gastroenterology and Metabolism, Nagoya City University Graduate School of Medical Sciences
| | - Takahito Katano
- Department of Gastroenterology and Metabolism, Nagoya City University Graduate School of Medical Sciences
| | - Satoshi Tanida
- Department of Gastroenterology and Metabolism, Nagoya City University Graduate School of Medical Sciences
| | - Atsuyuki Hirano
- Department of Gastroenterology, Nagoya City West Medical Center
| | | | - Keiji Ozeki
- Department of Gastroenterology and Metabolism, Nagoya City University Graduate School of Medical Sciences
| | - Yuka Suzuki
- Department of Gastroenterology, Japanese Red Cross Nagoya Daini Hospital
| | - Naomi Sugimura
- Department of Gastroenterology and Metabolism, Nagoya City University Graduate School of Medical Sciences
- Department of Gastroenterology, Nagoya Memorial Hospital, Nagoya, Japan
| | - Hiromi Kataoka
- Department of Gastroenterology and Metabolism, Nagoya City University Graduate School of Medical Sciences
| | - Takashi Joh
- Department of Gastroenterology and Metabolism, Nagoya City University Graduate School of Medical Sciences
| |
Collapse
|
288
|
Feng J, Ren X, Ma X. Ongoing Voluntary Settlement and Independent Agency: Evidence from China. Front Psychol 2017; 8:1287. [PMID: 28798712 PMCID: PMC5529403 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01287] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/20/2017] [Accepted: 07/13/2017] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Voluntary frontier settlement leads to independent agency. Since this type of research has not yet been implemented in ongoing voluntary settlement frontiers, we conducted several cultural tasks to investigate Shenzhen, known as China's ongoing "South Frontier," which is composed mostly of people that have emigrated from other Chinese provinces within the past 30 years. We hypothesized that residents of Shenzhen are more independent than those in other regions of Mainland China. As predicted, residents of Shenzhen scored higher than China inland residents in self-reported independent beliefs and scored lower in nepotism. The results indicate that, even in a short-term ongoing frontier, voluntary settlement is associated with independent agency.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jing Feng
- CAS Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of PsychologyBeijing, China.,University of Chinese Academy of SciencesBeijing, China
| | - Xiaopeng Ren
- CAS Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of PsychologyBeijing, China
| | - Xinran Ma
- CAS Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of PsychologyBeijing, China.,University of Chinese Academy of SciencesBeijing, China
| |
Collapse
|
289
|
Feelings and contexts: socioecological influences on the nonverbal expression of emotion. Curr Opin Psychol 2017; 17:170-175. [PMID: 28950965 DOI: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.07.025] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/12/2017] [Revised: 06/06/2017] [Accepted: 07/17/2017] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
Abstract
Despite their relative universality, nonverbal displays of emotion are often sources of cross-cultural misunderstandings. The present article considers the relevance of historical and present socio-ecological contexts, such as heterogeneity of long-history migration, pathogen prevalence, and residential mobility for cross-cultural variation in emotional expression. We review recent evidence linking these constructs to psychological processes and discuss how the findings are relevant to the nonverbal communication of emotion. We hold that socioecological variables, because of their specificity and tractability, provide a promising framework for explaining why different cultures developed varying modes of emotional expression.
Collapse
|
290
|
Szabo A, English AS, Zhijia Z, Jose P, Ward C, Jianhong M. Is the Utility of Secondary Coping a Function of Ethnicity or the Context of Reception? A Longitudinal Study Across Western and Eastern Cultures. JOURNAL OF CROSS-CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY 2017. [DOI: 10.1177/0022022117719158] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
Although cross-cultural research suggests that the development and functionality of secondary coping strategies are substantially influenced by the broader cultural context, research findings are not completely conclusive. Some studies indicate that secondary coping is more adaptive for Asian participants compared with Westerners, while others emphasize the adaptability of the coping style in Asian, but not Western, contexts. The main objective of the present study was to systematically test for ethnic and contextual differences in the effectiveness of secondary strategies (measured in the form of acceptance and positive reinterpretation) in reducing the negative effects of acculturative stress on somatic symptoms with samples of international students. A 3-month longitudinal study was conducted in two different contexts: (a) Asian and Western international students in China (Asians: n = 53, Westerners: n = 51) and (b) similar groups in New Zealand (Asians: n = 61, Westerners: n = 65). The data were subjected to a hierarchical regression analysis with changes in psychological symptoms functioning as the dependent variable. Findings indicated that stress, secondary coping, and cultural context significantly interacted in predicting changes in somatic symptoms. Specifically, secondary coping exacerbated the negative effects of acculturative stress on psychological adjustment in New Zealand. In contrast, secondary coping functioned as a buffer in China, such that it was effective at reducing the negative impact of stress over time. Findings indicated that ethnicity did not significantly moderate the stress–coping–adjustment relationship. Our results show that the effectiveness of secondary coping varies as a function of the cultural context.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Agnes Szabo
- Massey University, Manawatū, New Zealand
- Victoria University of Wellington, New Zealand
| | - Alexander S. English
- Shanghai International Studies University, China
- Zhejiang University, Hangzhou, China
| | | | - Paul Jose
- Victoria University of Wellington, New Zealand
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
291
|
Abstract
Individualism appears to have increased over the past several decades, yet most research documenting this shift has been limited to the study of a handful of highly developed countries. Is the world becoming more individualist as a whole? If so, why? To answer these questions, we examined 51 years of data on individualist practices and values across 78 countries. Our findings suggest that individualism is indeed rising in most of the societies we tested. Despite dramatic shifts toward greater individualism around the world, however, cultural differences remain sizable. Moreover, cultural differences are primarily linked to changes in socioeconomic development, and to a lesser extent to shifts in pathogen prevalence and disaster frequency.
Collapse
|
292
|
Brown ER, Steinberg M, Lu Y, Diekman AB. Is the Lone Scientist an American Dream? Perceived Communal Opportunities in STEM Offer a Pathway to Closing U.S.–Asia Gaps in Interest and Positivity. SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGICAL AND PERSONALITY SCIENCE 2017. [DOI: 10.1177/1948550617703173] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
The United States lags behind many Asian countries in engagement in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM). An unexplored factor in these country-level differences may be U.S.–Asia gaps in perceptions of the goal opportunities provided by STEM. Across four studies, U.S. students perceived fewer communal opportunities (working with/helping/relationships with others) in STEM than Asian students; this differential perception contributed to U.S.–Asia gaps in STEM interest. Perceptions of communal opportunities in STEM did not follow from a general orientation to perceive that all careers provided communal opportunities but from communal engagement in STEM. Perceptions about communal opportunities in STEM predicted STEM interest, and communal experience in STEM predicted STEM interest beyond quantity of STEM exposure. Experimentally highlighting the perceived communal opportunities in science closed the cultural gap in positivity toward a scientist career (Study 5). Perceptions of communal opportunities in STEM provide a new vantage point to improve U.S. engagement in STEM.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Elizabeth R. Brown
- Department of Psychology, University of North Florida, Jacksonville, FL, USA
| | - Mia Steinberg
- Academic Affairs, California State University, Long Beach, Long Beach, CA, USA
| | - Yun Lu
- Department of Counseling, Higher Education, and Special Education, University of Maryland, College Park, College Park, MD, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
293
|
Mellor S. Confidence at Work and Individualism-Collectivism: An Empirical Demonstration of the Distinctiveness of American Union Employees. CURRENT PSYCHOLOGY 2017. [DOI: 10.1007/s12144-017-9636-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
|
294
|
Luo L, Ding R, Gao X, Sun J, Zhao W. Socioeconomic status influences sex ratios in a Chinese rural population. PeerJ 2017; 5:e3546. [PMID: 28674672 PMCID: PMC5494181 DOI: 10.7717/peerj.3546] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/08/2017] [Accepted: 06/15/2017] [Indexed: 11/23/2022] Open
Abstract
According to the logic of the Trivers–Willard hypothesis, in a human population, if socioeconomic status is transmitted across generations to some extent, and if sons of high-status parents tend to have higher reproductive success than daughters, while daughters of low-status parents tend to have higher reproductive success than sons, then we should expect that offspring sex ratio is positively associated with socioeconomic status. This study examines whether the assumptions and prediction of this hypothesis apply to a rural population in northern China. Results show that (1) current family socioeconomic status is positively related to family head’s father’s socioeconomic status in around 1950, (2) low-status family heads have more grandchildren through their daughters than their sons, whereas high- or middle-status family heads have more grandchildren through sons, and (3) as family heads’ status increases, they tend to produce a higher offspring sex ratio. Therefore, the assumptions and prediction of the hypothesis are met in the study population. These results are discussed in reference to past studies on sex ratio manipulation among humans.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Liqun Luo
- Department of Sociology, Central China Normal University, Wuhan, China
| | - Rui Ding
- Department of Sociology, Central China Normal University, Wuhan, China
| | - Xiali Gao
- Department of Sociology, Central China Normal University, Wuhan, China
| | - Jingjing Sun
- Hubei Institute of Economic and Social Development, Central China Normal University, Wuhan, China
| | - Wei Zhao
- Department of Sociology, Central China Normal University, Wuhan, China
| |
Collapse
|
295
|
Kitayama S, Park J. Emotion and biological health: the socio-cultural moderation. Curr Opin Psychol 2017; 17:99-105. [PMID: 28950982 DOI: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.06.016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/11/2017] [Revised: 06/19/2017] [Accepted: 06/25/2017] [Indexed: 12/31/2022]
Abstract
Prior evidence shows that positive and negative emotions are associated with better and worse health, respectively. However, the emotion-health relationship may be conflated since this evidence is based nearly exclusively on subjective measures of health. Here, we reviewed more recent evidence focusing on biological health and showed that the emotion-health link is more complex. First, whereas negative emotional states such as negative affect and anger expression are inversely linked to biological health among Americans, this relationship is either not present (negative affect) or even reversed (anger expression) for Japanese. Second, whereas a meaningfulness aspect of happiness (eudaimonia) is linked consistently to better biological health, the relationship between its pleasantness aspect (hedonia) and biological health is uncertain. Moreover, a contextualized sense of meaningfulness in a work setting is strongly associated with better biological health among Japanese. Altogether, the initial evidence reviewed here underscores a need to conceptualize the biological dynamics of health and wellbeing as inherently intertwined with socio-cultural processes.
Collapse
|
296
|
Wang C, Zhang Y. Season of birth and schizophrenia: Evidence from China. Psychiatry Res 2017; 253:189-196. [PMID: 28390294 DOI: 10.1016/j.psychres.2017.03.030] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/16/2016] [Revised: 03/17/2017] [Accepted: 03/18/2017] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Abstract
Controversies exist whether season of birth is associated with schizophrenia development later in life, and evidence has mainly come from studies done in developed countries. This study examines the association between season of birth and risk for schizophrenia in China, with special attention to geographical region, urbanity, and gender. Using data from China's Second National Sampling Survey on Disability, a large-scale, nationally representative sample (N=2,052,694), this study employs discrete-time hazard models to compare the risk for schizophrenia development for people born in different seasons, and conducts subsample analyses by geographical region, urbanity, and gender. People born in the spring have the highest risk when compared to people born in the winter, summer or autumn. Furthermore, the relatively higher risk for people born in the spring is greater in the southern half of the country, in rural areas, and for women. The findings are consistent with results from a robustness check done among people who were conceived and born from 1955 to 1965, periods before, during, and after the 1959-1961 Chinese Famine. This study supports the presence of an association between season of birth and risk for schizophrenia development and of heterogeneity by geographical region, urbanity, and gender.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Cuntong Wang
- School of Social Development, Central University of Finance and Economics, Beijing, China.
| | - Yudong Zhang
- School of Social Service Administration, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA
| |
Collapse
|
297
|
Chronosequencing methanogenic archaea in ancient Longji rice Terraces in China. Sci Bull (Beijing) 2017; 62:879-887. [PMID: 36659324 DOI: 10.1016/j.scib.2017.05.024] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/25/2017] [Revised: 05/22/2017] [Accepted: 05/22/2017] [Indexed: 01/21/2023]
Abstract
Chronosequences of ancient rice terraces serve as an invaluable archive for reconstructions of historical human-environment interactions. Presently, however, these reconstructions are based on traditional soil physico-chemical properties. The microorganisms in palaeosols have been unexplored. We hypothesized that microbial information can be used as an additional proxy to complement and consolidate archaeological interpretations. To test this hypothesis, the palaeoenvironmental methanogenic archaeal DNA in Longji Terraces, one of the famous ancient terraces in China, dating back to the late Yuan Dynasty (CE 1361-1406), was chronosequenced by high-throughput sequencing. It was found that the methanogenic archaeal abundance, diversity and community composition were closely associated with the 630years of rice cultivation and in line with changes in multi-proxy data. Particularly, the centennial- and decadal-scale influences of known historical events, including social turbulences (The Taiping Rebellion, CE 1850-1865), palaeoclimate changes (the Little Ice Age) and recorded natural disasters (earthquakes and inundation), on ancient agricultural society were clearly echoed in the microbial archives as variations in alpha and beta diversity. This striking correlation suggests that the microorganisms archived in palaeosols can be quantitatively and qualitatively analyzed to provide an additional proxy, and palaeo-microbial information could be routinely incorporated in the toolkit for archaeological interpretation.
Collapse
|
298
|
Kim ES, Kim KO, Jang BI, Lee CK, Kim HJ, Lee KM, Kim YS, Eun CS, Jung SA, Yang SK, Lee J, Kim TO, Jung Y, Seo GS, Yoon SM. Factors Contributing to the Preference of Korean Patients with Crohn's Disease When Selecting an Anti-Tumor Necrosis Factor Agent (CHOICE Study). Gut Liver 2017; 10:391-8. [PMID: 26347512 PMCID: PMC4849692 DOI: 10.5009/gnl15126] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/08/2023] Open
Abstract
Background/Aims Two comparable anti-tumor necrosis factor (TNF) agents with different routes of administration (intravenous [iv] infliximab [IFX] vs subcutaneous [sc] adalimumab [ADA]) are available for patients with Crohn’s disease (CD) in Korea. This study aimed to identify the preferences of Korean CD patients for a specific anti-TNF agent and the factors contributing to the decision. Methods A prospective survey was performed among anti-TNF-naive CD patients in 10 tertiary referral hospitals. A 16-item questionnaire addressed patient preferences and the factors contributing to the decision in favor of a particular anti-TNF agent. A logistic regression was conducted to assess predictive factors for ADA preference. Results Overall, 189 patients (139 males; mean age, 32.47±11.71 years) completed the questionnaire. IFX and ADA were preferred by 63.5% (120/189) and 36.5% (69/189) of patients, respectively. The most influential reason for choosing IFX was ‘doctor’s presence’ (68.3%, 82/120), and ADA was “easy to use” (34.8%, 24/69). Amid various clinicodemographic data, having a >60-minute travel time to the hospital was a significant independent predictive factor for ADA preference. Conclusions A large number of anti-TNF-naive Korean patients with CD preferred anti-TNFs with an iv route of administration. The reassuring effect of a doctor’s presence might be the main contributing factor for this decision.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Eun Soo Kim
- Department of Internal Medicine, Keimyung University School of Medicine, Daegu, Korea
| | - Kyeong Ok Kim
- Department of Internal Medicine, Yeungnam University College of Medicine, Daegu, Korea
| | - Byung Ik Jang
- Department of Internal Medicine, Yeungnam University College of Medicine, Daegu, Korea
| | - Chang Kyun Lee
- Department of Internal Medicine, Kyung Hee University School of Medicine, Seoul, Korea
| | - Hyo Jong Kim
- Department of Internal Medicine, Kyung Hee University School of Medicine, Seoul, Korea
| | - Kang-Moon Lee
- Department of Internal Medicine, The Catholic University of Korea College of Medicine, Suwon, Korea
| | - You Sun Kim
- Department of Internal Medicine, Inje University College of Medicine, Seoul, Korea
| | - Chang Soo Eun
- Department of Internal Medicine, Hanyang University College of Medicine, Guri, Korea
| | - Sung-Ae Jung
- Department of Internal Medicine, Ewha Womans University School of Medicine, Seoul, Korea
| | - Suk-Kyun Yang
- Department of Internal Medicine, University of Ulsan College of Medicine, Seoul, Korea
| | - Jun Lee
- Department of Internal Medicine, Chosun University School of Medicine, Gwangju, Korea
| | - Tae-Oh Kim
- Department of Internal Medicine, Inje University College of Medicine, Busan, Korea
| | - Yunho Jung
- Department of Internal Medicine, Soonchunhyang University College of Medicine, Cheonan, Korea
| | - Geom Seog Seo
- Department of Internal Medicine, Wonkwang University School of Medicine, Iksan, Korea
| | - Soon Man Yoon
- Department of Internal Medicine, Chungbuk National University Hospital, Cheongju, Korea
| | | |
Collapse
|
299
|
Mou Y. Predicting the Use of Traditional Chinese Medicine Health Maintenance Approach from Cultural and Spiritual Perspectives. JOURNAL OF RELIGION AND HEALTH 2017; 56:971-985. [PMID: 27538575 DOI: 10.1007/s10943-016-0299-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/06/2023]
Abstract
Traditional Chinese medicine (TCM) health maintenance approach is widespread among the lay public as a tool of health maintenance and disease prevention across the globe. This study aims to investigate the factors that exert influences on the intention to adopt or continue using TCM health maintenance approach under the guideline of the extended theory of planned behavior. Based on an online survey on a Chinese national sample of 800 valid responses, this study indicates the respondents' spirituality and Chinese cultural orientation positively predict their attitude toward TCM health maintenance approach. In turn, their attitude, perceived behavioral control and norms positively predict the intention to adopt or continue using TCM health maintenance approach, as postulated in hypotheses. Theoretical and practical implications are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yi Mou
- School of Media and Design, Shanghai Jiao Tong University, 800 Dongchuan Road, Minhang District, 200240, Shanghai, China.
| |
Collapse
|
300
|
Obschonka M, Stuetzer M, Rentfrow PJ, Potter J, Gosling SD. Did Strategic Bombing in the Second World War Lead to ‘German Angst’? A Large–Scale Empirical Test across 89 German Cities. EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF PERSONALITY 2017. [DOI: 10.1002/per.2104] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
Abstract
A widespread stereotype holds that the Germans are notorious worriers, an idea captured by the term German angst. An analysis of country–level neurotic personality traits (trait anxiety, trait depression, and trait neuroticism; N = 7 210 276) across 109 countries provided mixed support for this idea; Germany ranked 20th, 31st, and 53rd for depression, anxiety, and neuroticism, respectively, suggesting, at best, the national stereotype is only partly valid. Theories put forward to explain the stereotypical characterization of Germany focus on the collective traumatic events experienced by Germany during World War II (WWII), such as the massive strategic bombing of German cities. We thus examined the link between strategic bombing of 89 German cities and today's regional levels in neurotic traits ( N = 33 534) and related mental health problems. Contrary to the WWII bombing hypothesis, we found negative effects of strategic bombing on regional trait depression and mental health problems. This finding was robust when controlling for a host of economic factors and social structure. We also found Resilience × Stressor interactions: Cities with more severe bombings show more resilience today (lower levels of neurotic traits and mental health problems in the face of a current major stressor—economic hardship). Copyright © 2017 European Association of Personality Psychology
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Michael Stuetzer
- Baden Wurttemberg Cooperative State University, Germany
- Ilmenau University of Technology, Germany
| | | | | | - Samuel D. Gosling
- University of Texas at Austin, USA
- School of Psychological Sciences, University of Melbourne, Parkville, Victoria Australia
| |
Collapse
|