551
|
Abstract
Two experiments were conducted to investigate possible relationships between cognitive function and hearing aid use. In Experiment 1, 72 first-time hearing aid users were tested for speech recognition in noise (Hagerman sentence test) with and without hearing aids. Cognitive function was assessed by tests of working memory (reading span test) and verbal information-processing speed. The results indicate that, after controlling for age and hearing loss, significant correlations exist between the measures of cognitive performance and speech recognition in noise, both with and without hearing aids. High cognitive performance was associated with high performance in the speech recognition task. In Experiment 2, 17 first-time hearing aid users with either high or low working-memory capacity tested an experimental hearing aid which processed the sound differently depending on whether or not speech was detected. The results revealed that those with high working-memory capacity were better than those with low capacity at identifying and reporting the specific processing effects of the aid. This may have implications for how reported results should be interpreted in a research context, how a person's rehabilitation needs are formulated, and how hearing aid controls should be supervised. In conclusion, careful attention should be paid to the cognitive status of listeners, as it can have a significant influence on their ability to utilize their hearing aids.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Thomas Lunner
- Oticon A/S, Research Centre Eriksholm, Snekkersten, Denmark.
| |
Collapse
|
552
|
Lyxell B, Andersson U, Borg E, Ohlsson IS. Working-memory capacity and phonological processing in deafened adults and individuals with a severe hearing impairment. Int J Audiol 2003; 42 Suppl 1:S86-9. [PMID: 12918614 DOI: 10.3109/14992020309074628] [Citation(s) in RCA: 43] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/13/2022]
Abstract
The purpose of the present article is to review a number of studies conducted in our own laboratory with respect to working memory capacity and phonological processing in deafened adults and individuals with a severe hearing impairment, and how these two cognitive components relate to speech processing. The results demonstrate that one specific component in the phonological processing system (i.e., the phonological representation system) is deteriorating, whereas other parts are preserved intact. The characteristic of the individual's phonological representation is further correlated with success in speech reading and speech understanding with some cochlear implant systems. Working memory capacity is a capacity that remains intact despite a long duration of deafness/severe hearing loss. The size of the working memory is related to skill in speech reading and level of speech understanding with cochlear implants and perceived effort in a noisy environment.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Björn Lyxell
- Department of Behavioural Sciences, Linköping University, Linköping, Sweden.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
553
|
Rönnberg J. Cognition in the hearing impaired and deaf as a bridge between signal and dialogue: a framework and a model. Int J Audiol 2003; 42 Suppl 1:S68-76. [PMID: 12918612 DOI: 10.3109/14992020309074626] [Citation(s) in RCA: 172] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/13/2022]
Abstract
This paper focuses on the role of cognition in visual language processing in the deaf and hard of hearing. Although there are modality-specific cognitive findings in the literature on comparisons across speech communication modes and language (sign and speech), there is an impressive bulk of evidence that supports the notion of general modality-free cognitive functions in speech and sign processing. A working-memory framework is proposed for the cognitive involvement in language understanding (sign and speech). On the basis of multiple sources of behavioural and neuroscience data, four important parameters for language understanding are described in some detail: quality and precision of phonology, long-term memory access speed, degree of explicit processing, and general processing and storage capacity. Their interaction forms an important parameter space, and general predictions and applications can be derived for both spoken and signed language conditions. The model is mathematically formulated at a general level, hypothetical ease-of-language-understanding (ELU) functions are presented, and similarities and differences from current working-memory and speech perception formulations are pointed out.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jerker Rönnberg
- Department of Behavioural Sciences, Linköping University and The Swedish Institute for Disability Research, Linköping, Sweden.
| |
Collapse
|
554
|
Abstract
To test the notion that aging brings an inability to self-initiate processing, the authors investigated the effects of memory load on online sentence understanding. Younger and older adults read a series of short passages with or without a simultaneous updating task, which would be expected to deplete resources by consuming memory capacity. Regression analyses of word-by-word reading times onto text variables within each condition were used to decompose reading times into resources allocated to the array of word-level and textbase-level processes needed for comprehension. Among neither the young nor the old were word-level processes disrupted by a simultaneous memory load. However, older readers showed relatively greater levels of resource allocation to conceptual integration than the younger adults when under load, regardless of working-memory span or task priority. These results suggest that the ability to self-initiate the allocation of processing resources during reading is preserved among older readers.
Collapse
|
555
|
Abstract
Working memory involves the temporary storage and manipulation of information that is assumed to be necessary for a wide range of complex cognitive activities. In 1974, Baddeley and Hitch proposed that it could be divided into three subsystems, one concerned with verbal and acoustic information, the phonological loop, a second, the visuospatial sketchpad providing its visual equivalent, while both are dependent upon a third attentionally-limited control system, the central executive. A fourth subsystem, the episodic buffer, has recently been proposed. These are described in turn, with particular reference to implications for both the normal processing of language, and its potential disorders. The reader will be introduced to the concept of a multi-component working memory. Particular emphasis will be placed on the phonological loop component, and (a) its fractionation into a storage and processing component, (b) the neuropsychological evidence for this distinction, and (c) its implication for both native and second language learning. This will be followed by (d) a brief overview of the visuospatial sketchpad and its possible role in language, culminating in (e) discussion of the higher-level control functions of working memory which include (f) the central executive and its multi-dimensional storage system, the episodic buffer. An attempt throughout is made to link the model to its role in both normal and disordered language functions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alan Baddeley
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Bristol, 8 Woodland Rd, Bristol, BS8 1TN, UK.
| |
Collapse
|
556
|
Smith RE. The cost of remembering to remember in event-based prospective memory: investigating the capacity demands of delayed intention performance. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 2003; 29:347-61. [PMID: 12776746 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.29.3.347] [Citation(s) in RCA: 324] [Impact Index Per Article: 15.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Prospective memory tasks are often accomplished during the performance of other activities. Despite the dual-task nature of prospective memory, little attention has been paid to how successful prospective memory performance affects ongoing activities. In the first 2 experiments, participants performing an embedded prospective memory task had longer response times on nonprospective memory target trials of a lexical decision task than participants performing the lexical decision task alone. In the prospective memory groups, longer lexical decision response times were associated with better prospective memory performance (Experiments 1, 2, and 3), a pattern not demonstrated with an embedded retrospective memory task (Experiment 2). The results of Experiment 3 suggest that the retrieval of a delayed intention, or the prospective component, can require capacity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rebekah E Smith
- Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, 27599-3270, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
557
|
Bagner DM, Melinder MRD, Barch DM. Language comprehension and working memory language comprehension and working memory deficits in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2003; 60:299-309. [PMID: 12591591 DOI: 10.1016/s0920-9964(02)00280-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 50] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
Abstract
The present study examined the hypothesis that patients with schizophrenia have deficits in language comprehension compared to normal controls, and that these deficits are associated with disturbances in working memory (WM). In addition, we hypothesized that language comprehension deficits would be associated with the severity of specific symptoms in the patients (formal thought disorder and hallucinations). Participants were 27 stable outpatients with schizophrenia and 28 demographically similar controls. Language comprehension was measured by presenting sentences auditorily that varied in length and syntactic complexity, followed by two or three comprehension questions. We measured working memory by administering a reading span task. Results indicated that, as predicted, language comprehension deficits were significantly greater in patients with schizophrenia than controls. Also as predicted, working memory was strongly correlated with language comprehension performance in both patients with schizophrenia and controls. Contrary to our predictions, language comprehension and working memory deficits were not associated with either formal thought disorder or hallucinations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Daniel M Bagner
- Department of Psychology, Washington University, Box 1125, One Brookings Drive, 63130, St. Louis, MO, USA
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
558
|
Berti S, Schröger E. Working memory controls involuntary attention switching: evidence from an auditory distraction paradigm. Eur J Neurosci 2003; 17:1119-22. [PMID: 12653989 DOI: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02527.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 160] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
One function of working memory is to protect current mental processes against interference. In contrast, to be able to react flexibly on unpredictable environmental changes working memory should not totally be encapsulated from processing task unrelated information; that is, it should remain distractible. By manipulating the task load of the primary task in an auditory distraction paradigm we investigated how these opposing functions are coordinated by working memory. The behavioural results show that distraction effects were still present but reduced markedly with higher task demands. This suggests that working memory exerts some control over involuntary attention. In addition, event-related brain potentials related to the different processing stages reveal that the preattentive change detection system underlying distraction was not modulated by task demand whereas distraction per se was. The present data suggest that working memory is able to coordinate the maintenance of distractibility and the focus on the task at hand.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Stefan Berti
- Institut für Allgemeine Psychologie, Universität Leipzig, Seeburgstr 14-20, D-04103 Leipzig, Leipzig, Germany.
| | | |
Collapse
|
559
|
Vos SH, Friederici AD. Intersentential syntactic context effects on comprehension: the role of working memory. BRAIN RESEARCH. COGNITIVE BRAIN RESEARCH 2003; 16:111-22. [PMID: 12589896 DOI: 10.1016/s0926-6410(02)00226-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
The study investigated the influence of a syntactically biasing context sentence on the processing of syntactically complex object-first relative clauses and even more complex object-first complement clauses in readers with individual differences in working memory. Behavioral as well as brain responses (event-related potentials, ERPs) were recorded from two groups of participants with either a high or a low working memory span. The behavioral data taken from a post-sentence comprehension question task indicate an intersentential syntactic contextual influence on the comprehension of object-first relative clauses for low span readers, but not for high span readers. The on-line ERP measures taken at the disambiguating item revealed for the high span readers a main effect of structure (subject- versus object-first) i.e., a P600, independent of context. Low span readers, in contrast, did not show any P600 structure effect. Thus, the combined data indicate a differential context effect on sentence comprehension in low and high span readers. High span readers parse the incoming information independent of intersentential context, whereas low span readers use the intersentential context off-line.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sandra H Vos
- F.C. Donders Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | | |
Collapse
|
560
|
Osaka M, Osaka N, Kondo H, Morishita M, Fukuyama H, Aso T, Shibasaki H. The neural basis of individual differences in working memory capacity: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 2003; 18:789-97. [PMID: 12667855 DOI: 10.1016/s1053-8119(02)00032-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 130] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/27/2022] Open
Abstract
Using fMRI, neural substrates of verbal working memory were investigated with respect to differences in working memory capacity. Listening-span test (LST), Listen, and Remember conditions were performed. Two subjects groups were selected: those who had large working memory capacities, labeled high-span subjects (HSS) according to the working memory span test, and those who had small working memory capacities, labeled low-span subjects (LSS). Significant activation was found mainly in three regions in comparison with resting control: left prefrontal cortex (PFC), anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and temporal language area. For both groups, fMRI signal intensity increased in PFC during the LST condition compared to the Listen condition. A group difference was found in the ACC region; specifically, a significant increase in signal intensity was observed in ACC only for the HSS group and not for the LSS group. Behavioral data also showed that the performance was better in HSS than in LSS. These results indicate that the attention controlling system, supported by ACC, is more effective in HSS compared to that of LSS.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mariko Osaka
- Department of Psychology, Osaka University of Foreign Studies, Osaka 562-8558, Japan.
| | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
561
|
Cowan N, Towse JN, Hamilton Z, Saults JS, Elliott EM, Lacey JF, Moreno MV, Hitch GJ. Children's working-memory processes: a response-timing analysis. J Exp Psychol Gen 2003; 132:113-32. [PMID: 12656300 DOI: 10.1037/0096-3445.132.1.113] [Citation(s) in RCA: 110] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Recall response durations were used to clarify processing in working-memory tasks. Experiment 1 examined children's performance in reading span, a task in which sentences were processed and the final word of each sentence was retained for subsequent recall. Experiment 2 examined the development of listening-, counting-, and digit-span task performance. Responses were much longer in the reading-and listening-span tasks than in the other span tasks, suggesting that participants in sentence-based span tasks take time to retrieve the semantic or linguistic structure as cues to recall of the sentence-final words. Response durations in working-memory tasks helped to predict academic skill and achievement, largely separate from the contributions of the memory spans themselves. Response durations thus are important in the interpretation of span task performance.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nelson Cowan
- Department of Psychology, University of Missouri-Columbia, 210 McAlester Hall, Columbia, Missouri 65211, USA.
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
562
|
De Beni R, Palladino P, Borella E, Lo Presti S. Reading comprehension and aging: does an age-related difference necessarily mean impairment? Aging Clin Exp Res 2003; 15:67-76. [PMID: 12841421 DOI: 10.1007/bf03324482] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND AND AIMS This research investigated reading comprehension in groups of younger-old (55-69 years) and older-old (70-90 years) Italian adults to determine age-related differences and explore their extent. The second aim of our research was to investigate the nature of individual age-related differences and their relation to working memory and metacognition. METHODS In Experiment 1,250 participants read two passages, a narrative and an expository text, and answered a series of multiple-choice inferential questions. In Experiment 2, three groups: younger-old good and poor comprehenders and older-old poor comprehenders were compared for working memory and metacognitive tasks. RESULTS Although older-old adults had some difficulty compared with younger-old, a comparison with normative control scores (comprehension level achieved at the end of 8th grade compulsory education) showed that their reading comprehension of a narrative text was adequate, demonstrating basic comprehension skills for everyday life. Younger-old good comprehenders had higher working memory and metacognitive scores than younger-old poor comprehenders, consistent with results obtained in the literature with younger participants. Older-old adults had poorer working memory than younger-old poor comprehenders, although they did not differ in metacognitive performance. CONCLUSIONS Results show that age differences influence the relationship between reading comprehension and memory and metacognition.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rossana De Beni
- Department of General Psychology, University of Padova, Padova, Italy
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
563
|
Executive Attention, Working Memory Capacity, and a Two-Factor Theory of Cognitive Control. PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING AND MOTIVATION 2003. [DOI: 10.1016/s0079-7421(03)44005-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 241] [Impact Index Per Article: 11.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/12/2022]
|
564
|
Kane MJ, Engle RW. The role of prefrontal cortex in working-memory capacity, executive attention, and general fluid intelligence: an individual-differences perspective. Psychon Bull Rev 2002; 9:637-71. [PMID: 12613671 DOI: 10.3758/bf03196323] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1201] [Impact Index Per Article: 54.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
We provide an "executive-attention" framework for organizing the cognitive neuroscience research on the constructs of working-memory capacity (WMC), general fluid intelligence, and prefrontal cortex (PFC) function. Rather than provide a novel theory of PFC function, we synthesize a wealth of single-cell, brain-imaging, and neuropsychological research through the lens of our theory of normal individual differences in WMC and attention control (Engle, Kane, & Tuholski, 1999; Engle, Tuholski, Laughlin, & Conway, 1999). Our critical review confirms the prevalent view that dorsolateral PFC circuitry is critical to executive-attention functions. Moreover, although the dorsolateral PFC is but one critical structure in a network of anterior and posterior "attention control" areas, it does have a unique executive-attention role in actively maintaining access to stimulus representations and goals in interference-rich contexts. Our review suggests the utility of an executive-attention framework for guiding future research on both PFC function and cognitive control.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michael J Kane
- Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina, Greensboro, North Carolina 27402-6164, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
565
|
Cowan N, Saults JS, Elliott EM. The search for what is fundamental in the development of working memory. ADVANCES IN CHILD DEVELOPMENT AND BEHAVIOR 2002; 29:1-49. [PMID: 11957571 DOI: 10.1016/s0065-2407(02)80050-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 28] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Nelson Cowan
- Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri, 213 McAlester Hall, Columbia, MO 65211, USA.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
566
|
Service E, Simola M, Metsänheimo O, Maury S. Bilingual working memory span is affected by language skill. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2002. [DOI: 10.1080/09541440143000140] [Citation(s) in RCA: 54] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/16/2022]
|
567
|
Osak M, Nishizaki Y, Komori M, Osaka N. Effect of focus on verbal working memory: critical role of the focus word in reading. Mem Cognit 2002; 30:562-71. [PMID: 12184557 DOI: 10.3758/bf03194957] [Citation(s) in RCA: 43] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
The effect of focus on working memory was investigated with the reading span test (RST). In two experiments, the span scores and the number of intrusion errors were compared between the focused RST the and the nonfocused RST. Focus word was defined as the most important word for comprehending a sentence. For the focused RST, the target word to be maintained was the focus word for the sentence. In the nonfocused RST, however, the target word was not the focus word for the sentence. The results of both experiments showed that RST span scores were higher for the focused RST than for the nonfocused RST, and intrusion errors were found to increase for the nonfocused RST. In Experiment 2, the effect of focus was compared between high-span and low-span subjects. An effect of sentence length was also investigated. The result showed that low-span subjects were more affected than were high-span subjects by whether the word to be remembered was the focus word. The effect of sentence length was not confirmed. These findings suggest that the low-span subjects had deficits in their ability to establish and/or inhibit mental focus when faced with conflict situations in reading.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mariko Osak
- Department of Psychology, Osaka University of Foreign Studies, Mino, Japan.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
568
|
Baddeley AD. Is Working Memory Still Working? 1Copyright © 2001 by the American Psychological Association. Reprinted with permission from the original publication: . “Is Working Memory Still Working?” American Psychologist, 56, 849-864. This re-print publication is arranged in recognition of the conferral on Dr. Baddeley of the Aristotle Prize at the VIIth European Congress of Psychology, London, in July 2001, for his outstanding research on human working memory. The original publication in the American Psychologist related to Prof. Baddeley's receipt of the Distinguished Scientific Contribution Award of the American Psychological Association in 2001. EUROPEAN PSYCHOLOGIST 2002. [DOI: 10.1027//1016-9040.7.2.85] [Citation(s) in RCA: 339] [Impact Index Per Article: 15.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
Abstract
The current state of A.D. Baddeley and G.J. Hitch's (1974) multicomponent working memory model is reviewed. The phonological and visuospatial subsystems have been extensively investigated, leading both to challenges over interpretation of individual phenomena and to more detailed attempts to model the processes underlying the subsystems. Analysis of the controlling central executive has proved more challenging, leading to a proposed clarification in which the executive is assumed to be a limited capacity attentional system, aided by a newly postulated fourth system, the episodic buffer. Current interest focuses most strongly on the link between working memory and long-term memory and on the processes allowing the integration of information from the component subsystems. The model has proved valuable in accounting for data from a wide range of participant groups under a rich array of task conditions. Working memory does still appear to be working.
Collapse
|
569
|
Towse JN, Hitch GJ, Hutton U. On the nature of the relationship between processing activity and item retention in children. J Exp Child Psychol 2002; 82:156-84. [PMID: 12083794 DOI: 10.1016/s0022-0965(02)00003-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 58] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
The concept of working memory emphasizes the interrelationship between the transient retention of information and concurrent processing activity. Three experiments address this relationship in children between 8 and 17 years of age by examining forgetting when a processing task is interpolated between presentation and recall of the memory items. Unlike previous studies, delivery of interpolated stimuli was under computer control and responses to these stimuli were timed. There were consistent effects of the duration of the interpolated task, but no effects of either its difficulty or similarity to memory material and no qualitative developmental differences in task performance. The absence of an effect of difficulty provides no support for models of working memory in which limited capacity is shared between the dual functions of processing and storage, but is compatible with an alternative "task switching" account. However, task switching did not explain developmental differences in recall. Other aspects of the results suggest that there can be interactions between processing and storage but it is argued that these cannot be straightforwardly explained in terms of either task switching or resource sharing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- John N Towse
- Department of Psychology, Lancaster University, Lancaster LA1 4YF, UK.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
570
|
Hambrick DZ, Engle RW. Effects of domain knowledge, working memory capacity, and age on cognitive performance: an investigation of the knowledge-is-power hypothesis. Cogn Psychol 2002; 44:339-87. [PMID: 12018938 DOI: 10.1006/cogp.2001.0769] [Citation(s) in RCA: 110] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
Domain knowledge facilitates performance in many cognitive tasks. However, very little is known about the interplay between domain knowledge and factors that are believed to reflect general, and relatively stable, characteristics of the individual. The primary goal of this study was to investigate the interplay between domain knowledge and one such factor: working memory capacity. Adults from wide ranges of working memory capacity, age, and knowledge about the game of baseball listened to, and then answered questions about, simulated radio broadcasts of baseball games. There was a strong facilitative effect of preexisting knowledge of baseball on memory performance, particularly for information judged to be directly relevant to the baseball games. However, there was a positive effect of working memory capacity on memory performance as well, and there was no indication that domain knowledge attenuated this effect. That is, working memory capacity contributed to memory performance even at high levels of domain knowledge. Similarly, there was no evidence that domain knowledge attenuated age-related differences (favoring young adults) in memory performance. We discuss implications of the results for understanding proficiency in cognitive domains from an individual-differences perspective.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- David Z Hambrick
- Department of Psychology, Michigan State University, East Lansing 48824, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
571
|
A latent variable analysis of working memory capacity, short-term memory capacity, processing speed, and general fluid intelligence. INTELLIGENCE 2002. [DOI: 10.1016/s0160-2896(01)00096-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 631] [Impact Index Per Article: 28.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
|
572
|
Posthuma D, De Geus EJC, Baaré WFC, Hulshoff Pol HE, Kahn RS, Boomsma DI. The association between brain volume and intelligence is of genetic origin. Nat Neurosci 2002; 5:83-4. [PMID: 11818967 DOI: 10.1038/nn0202-83] [Citation(s) in RCA: 271] [Impact Index Per Article: 12.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
|
573
|
Just MA, Varma S. A hybrid architecture for working memory: Reply to MacDonald and Christiansen (2002). Psychol Rev 2002. [DOI: 10.1037/0033-295x.109.1.55] [Citation(s) in RCA: 34] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
|
574
|
Verguts T, De Boeck P. On the correlation between working memory capacity and performance on intelligence tests. LEARNING AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES 2002. [DOI: 10.1016/s1041-6080(02)00049-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
|
575
|
Gyselinck V, Cornoldi C, Dubois V, De Beni R, Ehrlich MF. Visuospatial memory and phonological loop in learning from multimedia. APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2002. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.823] [Citation(s) in RCA: 52] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
|
576
|
Caplan D, Waters G. Working memory and connectionist models of parsing: A reply to MacDonald and Christiansen (2002). Psychol Rev 2002. [DOI: 10.1037/0033-295x.109.1.66] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
|
577
|
Abstract
Situation model updating requires managing the availability of information as a function of its relevance to the current situation. This is thought to involve some aspect of working memory. The present study assesses the relation between updating ability and various measures of working memory span or capacity. In addition, a primitive general measure of situation model processing, a situation model identification test, and its relation to updating ability was also assessed. The present experiment used a version of a paradigm developed by Glenberg, Meyer, and Lindem (1987) to assess updating. Although updating was observed in both anaphoric reading time and recognition test accuracy measures, the reading time measure was relatively weak. Importantly, the updating effect on the recognition test was unrelated to working memory capacity. In contrast, updating was related to performance on the situation model identification task. Specifically, people who were good at model processing were better able to keep associated objects available than were people who were less adept. There were no differences in the maintenance of dissociated objects. These results suggest that the relationship between situation model processing and working memory capacity is relatively weak.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- G A Radvansky
- Department of Psychology, University of Notre Dame, Indiana 46556, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
578
|
Miyake A, Friedman NP, Rettinger DA, Shah P, Hegarty M. How are visuospatial working memory, executive functioning, and spatial abilities related? A latent-variable analysis. J Exp Psychol Gen 2001; 130:621-640. [PMID: 11757872 DOI: 10.1037/0096-3445.130.4.621] [Citation(s) in RCA: 519] [Impact Index Per Article: 22.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
This study examined the relationships among visuospatial working memory (WM) executive functioning, and spatial abilities. One hundred sixty-seven participants performed visuospatial short-term memory (STM) and WM span tasks, executive functioning tasks, and a set of paper-and-pencil tests of spatial abilities that load on 3 correlated but distinguishable factors (Spatial Visualization, Spatial Relations, and Perceptual Speed). Confirmatory factor analysis results indicated that, in the visuospatial domain, processing-and-storage WM tasks and storage-oriented STM tasks equally implicate executive functioning and are not clearly distinguishable. These results provide a contrast with existing evidence from the verbal domain and support the proposal that the visuospatial sketchpad may be closely tied to the central executive. Further, structural equation modeling results supported the prediction that, whereas they all implicate some degree of visuospatial storage, the 3 spatial ability factors differ in the degree of executive involvement (highest for Spatial Visualization and lowest for Perceptual Speed). Such results highlight the usefulness of a WM perspective in characterizing the nature of cognitive abilities and, more generally, human intelligence.
Collapse
|
579
|
|
580
|
Abstract
Eye fixations during reading were monitored to examine the relationship between individual differences in working memory capacity-as assessed by the reading span task-and inferences about predictable events. Context sentences predicting likely events, or non-predicting control sentences, were presented. They were followed by continuation sentences in which a target word represented an event to be inferred (inferential word) or an unlikely event (non-predictable word). A main effect of reading span showed that high working memory capacity was related to shorter gaze durations across sentence regions. More specific findings involved an interaction between context, target, and reading span on late processing measures and regions. Thus, for high- but not for low-span readers, the predicting condition, relative to the control condition, facilitated reanalysis of the continuation sentence that represented the inference concept. This effect was revealed by a reduction in regression-path reading time in the last region of the sentence, involving less time reading that region and fewer regressions from it. These results indicate that working memory facilitates elaborative inferences during reading, but that this occurs at late text-integration processes, rather than at early lexical-access processes.
Collapse
|
581
|
MacDonald MC, Almor A, Henderson VW, Kempler D, Andersen ES. Assessing working memory and language comprehension in Alzheimer's disease. BRAIN AND LANGUAGE 2001; 78:17-42. [PMID: 11412013 DOI: 10.1006/brln.2000.2436] [Citation(s) in RCA: 58] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/23/2023]
Abstract
Studies of language impairments in patients with Alzheimer's disease have often assumed that impairments in linguistic working memory underlie comprehension deficits. Assessment of this hypothesis has been hindered both by vagueness of key terms such as "working memory" and by limitations of available working memory tasks, in that many such tasks either seem to have little relationship to language comprehension or are too confusing or difficult for Alzheimer's patients. Four experiments investigated the usefulness of digit ordering, a new task assessing linguistic working memory and/or language processing skill, in normal adults and patients with probable Alzheimer's disease. The digit ordering task was shown to be strongly correlated with the degree of dementia in Alzheimer's patients. The task correlated with measures of language processing on which patients and normal controls performed differently. The results are interpreted as indicating that linguistic representations, linguistic processing, and linguistic working memory are intertwined, such that a deficit of one (e.g., working memory) cannot be said to "cause" a deficit in the other. The implications of this approach are explored in terms of task demands in comprehension and memory measures, and interpretation of previous results in the literature.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- M C MacDonald
- University of Southern California, Los Angeles 90089-2520, USA.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
582
|
Conway AR, Cowan N, Bunting MF. The cocktail party phenomenon revisited: the importance of working memory capacity. Psychon Bull Rev 2001; 8:331-5. [PMID: 11495122 DOI: 10.3758/bf03196169] [Citation(s) in RCA: 416] [Impact Index Per Article: 18.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Wood and Cowan (1995) replicated and extended Moray's (1959) investigation of the cocktail party phenomenon, which refers to a situation in which one can attend to only part of a noisy environment, yet highly pertinent stimuli such as one's own name can suddenly capture attention. Both of these previous investigations have shown that approximately 33% of subjects report hearing their own name in an unattended, irrelevant message. Here we show that subjects who detect their name in the irrelevant message have relatively low working-memory capacities, suggesting that they have difficulty blocking out, or inhibiting, distracting information.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- A R Conway
- Department of Psychology, University of Illinois, Chicago 60607-7137, USA.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
583
|
Swanson HL, Sachse-Lee C. A subgroup analysis of working memory in children with reading disabilities: domain-general or domain-specific deficiency? JOURNAL OF LEARNING DISABILITIES 2001; 34:249-263. [PMID: 15499879 DOI: 10.1177/002221940103400305] [Citation(s) in RCA: 51] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/24/2023]
Abstract
The purpose of this study was to investigate whether changes in working memory (WM) of children with reading disabilities (RD) were related to a domain-specific or a domain-general system. Based on Daneman and Carpenter's (1980) Sentence Listening Span task, children were subgrouped into a group of high executive processing (high listening span) children without RD, a group of low executive processing (low listening span) children with RD, and a group of children with and without RD matched on executive processing (moderate listening span). Subgroups were compared on phonological, visual-spatial, and semantic WM tasks across initial (no probes or cues), gain (cues that bring performance to an asymptotic level), and maintenance conditions (asymptotic conditions without cues). The results showed that (a) children without RD high in executive processing ability outperformed all other subgroups, (b) the RD subgroup low in executive processing performed poorly relative to all other subgroups across task and memory conditions, (c) children with and without RD matched on executive processing were comparable in WM span and changes in WM for all tasks, and (d) WM performance of children with RD was a strong linear function of the high executive processing group, suggesting that the nature or the specific componential makeup of the tasks are not the main contributors to WM performance. Taken together, the results suggest that a domain-general system may partially contribute to poor WM in children with RD, and that this system may operate independently of their reading deficits.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- H L Swanson
- Educational Psychology, School of Education, University of California, Riverside 92521, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
584
|
Hannon B, Daneman M. Susceptibility to semantic illusions: an individual-differences perspective. Mem Cognit 2001; 29:449-61. [PMID: 11407422 DOI: 10.3758/bf03196396] [Citation(s) in RCA: 36] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
When asked How many animals of each kind did Moses take on the ark?, people frequently respond "two" even though they know it was Noah, not Moses, who took animals on the ark. We replicate previous research by showing that susceptibility to semantic illusions is influenced by the semantic relatedness of both the impostor word and the surrounding context. However, we also show that the two text manipulations make independent contributions to semantic illusions, and we propose two individual-differences mechanisms that might underlie these two effects. We propose that the ability to resist the lure of a semantically related impostor word is related to the individual's skill at accessing and reasoning about knowledge from long-term memory. And we propose that the ability to resist the lure of the surrounding sentential context is related to the individual's capacity to simultaneously process and store information in working memory.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- B Hannon
- Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Mississauga, Ontario, Canada.
| | | |
Collapse
|
585
|
Kemtes KA, Kemper S. Cognitive construct measurement in small samples of younger and older adults: an example of verbal working memory. Exp Aging Res 2001; 27:167-80. [PMID: 11330212 DOI: 10.1080/036107301750074033] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/16/2022]
Abstract
An important issue in experimental aging research is the accurate measurement of cognitive constructs, particularly in small-sample studies. Latent variable modeling approaches to assessing age-based construct similarity are difficult to implement in smaller-scale studies, which tend to have small samples and measurement of a single construct. We discuss factor score comparison methods for assessing age-based construct similarity that may be more appropriate for small-scale studies. We then examine these methods for a series of single-factor models of verbal working memory (VWM) based on data from three separate studies in which small samples of younger and older adults' completed VWM-based tasks. Our single-factor models accounted well for the associations among the sets of VWM tasks. This construct was also measured well across age groups and different samples. Our analyses suggest that factor score comparison methods may be useful for small-scale studies that require assessment of age-based measurement similarity in cognitive constructs.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- K A Kemtes
- Volen Center for Complex Systems and Department of Psychology, Brandeis University, P.O. Box 9110 MS013, Waltham, MA 02454-9110, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
586
|
Palladino P, Cornoldi C, De Beni R, Pazzaglia F. Working memory and updating processes in reading comprehension. Mem Cognit 2001; 29:344-54. [PMID: 11352218 DOI: 10.3758/bf03194929] [Citation(s) in RCA: 128] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
In this study, we examine the relation between reading comprehension ability and success in working memory updating tasks. Groups of poor and good comprehenders, matched for logical reasoning ability, but different in reading comprehension ability, were administered various updating tasks in a series of experiments. In the first experiment, the participants were presented with lists of words, the length of which (4-10 words) was unknown beforehand, and were required to remember the last 4 words in each series. In this task, we found a decrease in performance that was related to longer series and poor reading ability. In the second experiment, we presented lists of nouns referring to items of different sizes, in a task that simulated the selection and updating of relevant information that occurs in the on-line comprehension process. The participants were required to remember a limited, predefined number of the smallest items presented. We found that poor comprehenders not only had a poorer memory, but also made a greater number of intrusion errors. In the third and fourth experiments, memory load (number of items to be selected) and suppression request (number of potentially relevant items) were manipulated within subjects. Increases in both memory load and suppression requests impaired performance. Furthermore, we found that poor comprehenders produced a greater number of intrusion errors, particularly when the suppression request was increased. Finally, in a fifth experiment, a request to specify the size of presented items was introduced. Poor comprehenders were able to select the appropriate items, although their recall was poorer. Altogether, the data show that working memory abilities, based on selecting and updating relevant information and avoiding intrusion errors, are related to reading comprehension.
Collapse
|
587
|
Waters GS, Caplan D. Age, working memory, and on-line syntactic processing in sentence comprehension. Psychol Aging 2001; 16:128-44. [PMID: 11302362 DOI: 10.1037/0882-7974.16.1.128] [Citation(s) in RCA: 95] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
One hundred twenty-seven individuals who ranged in age from 18 to 90 years were tested on a reading span test and on measures of on-line and off-line sentence processing efficiency. Older participants had reduced working-memory spans compared with younger participants. The on-line measures were sensitive to local increases in processing load, and the off-line measures were sensitive to the syntactic complexity of the sentences. Older and younger participants showed similar effects of syntactic complexity on the on-line measures. There was some evidence that older participants were more affected than younger participants by syntactic complexity on the off-line measures. The results support the hypothesis that on-line processes involved in recognizing linguistic forms and determining the literal, preferred, discourse-coherent meaning of sentences constitute a domain of language processing that relies on its own processing resource or working-memory system.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- G S Waters
- Department of Communication Disorders, Boston University, Massachusetts, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
588
|
Ambrose ML, Bowden SC, Whelan G. Working Memory Impairments in Alcohol-Dependent Participants Without Clinical Amnesia. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2001. [DOI: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2001.tb02197.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 54] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
|
589
|
Duff SC, Logie RH. Processing and storage in working memory span. THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY. A, HUMAN EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2001; 54:31-48. [PMID: 11216320 DOI: 10.1080/02724980042000011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 88] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/17/2022]
Abstract
Two experiments are reported that address theoretical assumptions as to the nature of working memory involved in working memory span tasks (Daneman & Carpenter, 1980). Experiment 1 used a version of the sentence span task, and Experiment 2 combined arithmetic verification with recall of presented words. In each experiment, working memory processing span was assessed independently of temporary storage span prior to their combination. Combined task performance under high demand for each component resulted in substantial residual performance for both task elements, particularly in Experiment 2. The results do not challenge the utility of the sentence span task as a measure of on-line cognition, but they raise concerns as to how resource might be allocated to processing and storage elements of the task within a single flexible resource pool, or between different resources of a multiple component working memory system. Although both models lack predictive power regarding resource allocation in these tasks, the multiple resource model appears to offer the better account.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- S C Duff
- Institute for Psychosocial Science, University of Bergen, Norway
| | | |
Collapse
|
590
|
Whitney P, Arnett PA, Driver A, Budd D. Measuring central executive functioning: what's in a reading span? Brain Cogn 2001; 45:1-14. [PMID: 11161358 DOI: 10.1006/brcg.2000.1243] [Citation(s) in RCA: 51] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
Although variations of Daneman and Carpenter's (1980) Reading Span Test (RST) have seen increasing use in both cognitive and neuropsychological research, the specific mental operations involved in performing it remain unclear. We tested 80 undergraduates to examine the extent to which speed of processing, manipulation capacity, and susceptibility to interference contributed to RST performance. The results suggest that, rather than unitary central executive or processing speed functions underlying RST performance, at least two factors, manipulation capacity and susceptibility to interference, underlie the task. Further study of RST operations may lead to a better understanding of the nature of the central executive itself.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- P Whitney
- Department of Psychology, Washington State University, Pullman 99164-4820, USA.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
591
|
Abstract
In reading and other high-level cognitive tasks, Ericsson and Kintsch (1995) proposed that the limited capacity of short-term working memory (STWM) is supplemented by long- term working memory (LTWM) for individuals with a high degree of domain-specific knowledge. In Experiment 1, college students (N = 80) wrote persuasive and narrative texts concerning baseball; domain-specific knowledge about baseball and verbal ability was assessed. The results showed that verbal ability and domain-specific knowledge independently affected writing skill, supporting the view that literacy depends on both knowledge sources and refuting one argument raised in support of the LTWM hypothesis. Experiment 2 (N = 42) replicated this outcome and tested the prediction that a high degree of domain-specific knowledge would lessen interference on a secondary task. The data supported the interference prediction, offering evidence that LTWM plays a role in the production of text.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- R T Kellogg
- Department of Psychology, University of Missouri, Rolla 65409-1270, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
592
|
Hannon B, Daneman M. A new tool for measuring and understanding individual differences in the component processes of reading comprehension. JOURNAL OF EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY 2001. [DOI: 10.1037/0022-0663.93.1.103] [Citation(s) in RCA: 136] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
|
593
|
Abstract
In this study, we examine the role of strategy use in working memory (WM) tasks by providing short-term memory (STM) task strategy training to participants. In Experiment 1, the participants received four sessions of training to use a story-formation (i.e., chaining) strategy. There were substantial improvements from pretest to posttest (after training) in terms of both STM and WM task performance. Experiment 2 demonstrated that WM task improvement did not occur for control participants, who were given the same amount of practice but were not provided with strategy instructions. An assessment of participants' strategy use on the STM task before training indicated that more strategic participants displayed better WM task performance and better verbal skills. These results support our hypothesis that strategy use influences performance on WM tasks.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- D S McNamara
- Psychology Department, Old Dominion University, Norfolk, Virginia 23529, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
594
|
|
595
|
14 Capacity, Control and Conflict: An Individual Differences Perspective on Attentional Capture. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2001. [DOI: 10.1016/s0166-4115(01)80016-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register]
|
596
|
Oberauer K, Süss HM. Working memory and interference: a comment on Jenkins, Myerson, Hale, and Fry (1999). Psychon Bull Rev 2000; 7:727-33; discussion 734-40. [PMID: 11206216 DOI: 10.3758/bf03213013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Jenkins, Myerson, Hale, and Fry (1999) showed that slopes relating complex spans to simple spans were considerably smaller than one, indicating that persons with higher simple spans suffered more interference when the span task was combined with a processing demand. They argued that this finding ruled out accounts of working memory based on interference and/or inhibition of interfering information. We demonstrate that the effect is mainly an artifact from regression to the mean, owing to the low reliability of span scores as used by Jenkins et al. Data from 133 young adults for two verbal and two spatial span tasks show that the slopes relating complex to simple performance are considerably higher for sum scores than for span scores. Furthermore, an adequate test for an interference or an inhibition account of working memory is to predict interference from complex span tasks, not from simple span tasks. Interference effects in the verbal span tasks were negatively correlated with an independent measure of working memory capacity, consistent with the interference/inhibition account.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- K Oberauer
- Allgemeine Psychologie I, University of Potsdam, Germany.
| | | |
Collapse
|
597
|
|
598
|
Titone D, Prentice KJ, Wingfield A. Resource allocation during spoken discourse processing: effects of age and passage difficulty as revealed by self-paced listening. Mem Cognit 2000; 28:1029-40. [PMID: 11105529 DOI: 10.3758/bf03209351] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
The allocation of processing resources during spoken discourse comprehension was studied in a manner analogous to self-paced reading using the auditory moving window technique (Ferreira, Henderson, Anes, Weeks, & McFarlane, 1996). Young and older participants listened to spoken passages in a self-paced segment-by-segment fashion. In Experiment 1, we examined the influence of speech rate and passage complexity on discourse encoding and recall performance. In Experiment 2, we examined the influence of speech rate and presentation mode (self-paced vs. full-passage presentation) on recall performance. Results suggest that diminished memory performance in the older adult group relative to the young adult group is attributable to age-related differences in how resources were allocated during the initial encoding of the spoken discourse.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- D Titone
- Brandeis University, Waltham, Massachusetts 02454-9110, USA
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
599
|
Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Wager TD. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex "Frontal Lobe" tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn Psychol 2000; 41:49-100. [PMID: 10945922 DOI: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7885] [Impact Index Per Article: 328.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/11/2022]
Abstract
This individual differences study examined the separability of three often postulated executive functions-mental set shifting ("Shifting"), information updating and monitoring ("Updating"), and inhibition of prepotent responses ("Inhibition")-and their roles in complex "frontal lobe" or "executive" tasks. One hundred thirty-seven college students performed a set of relatively simple experimental tasks that are considered to predominantly tap each target executive function as well as a set of frequently used executive tasks: the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST), Tower of Hanoi (TOH), random number generation (RNG), operation span, and dual tasking. Confirmatory factor analysis indicated that the three target executive functions are moderately correlated with one another, but are clearly separable. Moreover, structural equation modeling suggested that the three functions contribute differentially to performance on complex executive tasks. Specifically, WCST performance was related most strongly to Shifting, TOH to Inhibition, RNG to Inhibition and Updating, and operation span to Updating. Dual task performance was not related to any of the three target functions. These results suggest that it is important to recognize both the unity and diversity of executive functions and that latent variable analysis is a useful approach to studying the organization and roles of executive functions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- A Miyake
- Department of Psychology, University of Colorado at Boulder, 80309-0345, USA
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
600
|
Hacker W, Sieler R, Pietzcker F. Dekompositionsuntersuchungen zu Kernfunktionen des Arbeitsgedächtnisses. Exp Psychol 2000. [DOI: 10.1026//0949-3964.47.3.195] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/04/2022]
Abstract
Zusammenfassung. In einem experimentellen Design (n = 160) wird mittels Dekomposition der Einfluß verschiedener Kombinationen von Verarbeitungs- und Behaltensanforderungen auf die Leistung untersucht. Verglichen werden Varianten von Behalten-beim-Verarbeiten-Spannen (als mentale Doppeltätigkeiten bei gleichem Gewicht sowie Kontrolle des Verarbeitens und des Behaltens) mit der Wortspanne beim Satzlesen. Sodann werden in einem korrelativen Design die Beziehungen dieser Spannen und eines nicht-intentionalen Arbeitsgedächtnisindikators (pronominale Inferenzen) mit fluiden intellektuellen Leistungen verglichen. Die Wirkungen des Verarbeitens auf die Spanne sind nicht reduzierbar auf eine artikulatorische Rehearsalunterdrückung. Beim Behalten verbessert zwar die Substitution verarbeitungsfremder Items die Spanne, nicht aber die Substitution von Verarbeitungsergebnissen. Weder eine Zusatzaktivität ohne Behalten, noch Behalten an sich, sondern das Koordinieren von Behalten und Verarbeiten bestimmen die Leistung (Spanne). Das Koordinieren hat einen Kapazitätsbedarf hinausgehend über den der zu koordinierenden Aktivitäten. Unterschiedliche Spannen entstehen durch unterschiedlich effizientes Vorgehen bei der Behalten-beim-Verarbeiten-Koordination. Neben intentionalen (“kontrollierten”) Prozessen sind nicht-intentionale in Doppeltätigkeiten des Behalten-beim-Verarbeiten-Typs beteiligt. Die engsten Beziehungen bestehen zwischen fluiden verbalen und fluiden nicht-verbalen intellektuellen Leistungen und den nicht-intentionalen pronominalen Inferenzen sowie der intentionalen komplexen Behalten-beim-Verarbeiten-Spanne. Die Ergebnisse sprechen für ein Arbeitsgedächtnis-Modell als Funktionen-Set.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Ralph Sieler
- Professur für Allgemeine Psychologie, TU Dresden
| | | |
Collapse
|