51
|
A common probabilistic framework for perceptual and statistical learning. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2019; 58:218-228. [PMID: 31669722 DOI: 10.1016/j.conb.2019.09.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/01/2019] [Revised: 08/24/2019] [Accepted: 09/09/2019] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
System-level learning of sensory information is traditionally divided into two domains: perceptual learning that focuses on acquiring knowledge suitable for fine discrimination between similar sensory inputs, and statistical learning that explores the mechanisms that develop complex representations of unfamiliar sensory experiences. The two domains have been typically treated in complete separation both in terms of the underlying computational mechanisms and the brain areas and processes implementing those computations. However, a number of recent findings in both domains call in question this strict separation. We interpret classical and more recent results in the general framework of probabilistic computation, provide a unifying view of how various aspects of the two domains are interlinked, and suggest how the probabilistic approach can also alleviate the problem of dealing with widely different types of neural correlates of learning. Finally, we outline several directions along which our proposed approach fosters new types of experiments that can promote investigations of natural learning in humans and other species.
Collapse
|
52
|
Addleman DA, Jiang YV. Experience-Driven Auditory Attention. Trends Cogn Sci 2019; 23:927-937. [PMID: 31521482 DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2019.08.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/06/2019] [Revised: 08/19/2019] [Accepted: 08/19/2019] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
Abstract
In addition to conscious goals and stimulus salience, an observer's prior experience also influences selective attention. Early studies demonstrated experience-driven effects on attention mainly in the visual modality, but increasing evidence shows that experience drives auditory selection as well. We review evidence for a multiple-levels framework of auditory attention, in which experience-driven attention relies on mechanisms that acquire control settings and mechanisms that guide attention towards selected stimuli. Mechanisms of acquisition include cue-target associative learning, reward learning, and sensitivity to prior selection history. Once acquired, implementation of these biases can occur either consciously or unconsciously. Future research should more fully characterize the sources of experience-driven auditory attention and investigate the neural mechanisms used to acquire and implement experience-driven auditory attention.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Douglas A Addleman
- Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN 55455, USA.
| | - Yuhong V Jiang
- Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN 55455, USA
| |
Collapse
|
53
|
Demarchi G, Sanchez G, Weisz N. Automatic and feature-specific prediction-related neural activity in the human auditory system. Nat Commun 2019; 10:3440. [PMID: 31371713 PMCID: PMC6672009 DOI: 10.1038/s41467-019-11440-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 35] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/07/2018] [Accepted: 07/11/2019] [Indexed: 12/04/2022] Open
Abstract
Prior experience enables the formation of expectations of upcoming sensory events. However, in the auditory modality, it is not known whether prediction-related neural signals carry feature-specific information. Here, using magnetoencephalography (MEG), we examined whether predictions of future auditory stimuli carry tonotopic specific information. Participants passively listened to sound sequences of four carrier frequencies (tones) with a fixed presentation rate, ensuring strong temporal expectations of when the next stimulus would occur. Expectation of which frequency would occur was parametrically modulated across the sequences, and sounds were occasionally omitted. We show that increasing the regularity of the sequence boosts carrier-frequency-specific neural activity patterns during both the anticipatory and omission periods, indicating that prediction-related neural activity is indeed feature-specific. Our results illustrate that even without bottom-up input, auditory predictions can activate tonotopically specific templates. After listening to a predictable sequence of sounds, we can anticipate and predict the next sound in the sequence. Here, the authors show that during expectation of a sound, the brain generates neural activity matching that which is produced by actually hearing the same sound.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gianpaolo Demarchi
- Centre for Cognitive Neuroscience and Division of Physiological Psychology, University of Salzburg, Hellbrunnerstraße 34, 5020, Salzburg, Austria.
| | - Gaëtan Sanchez
- Centre for Cognitive Neuroscience and Division of Physiological Psychology, University of Salzburg, Hellbrunnerstraße 34, 5020, Salzburg, Austria.,Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, Brain Dynamics and Cognition Team, INSERM UMRS 1028, CNRS UMR 5292, Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1, Université de Lyon, F-69000, Lyon, France
| | - Nathan Weisz
- Centre for Cognitive Neuroscience and Division of Physiological Psychology, University of Salzburg, Hellbrunnerstraße 34, 5020, Salzburg, Austria
| |
Collapse
|
54
|
Santolin C, Saffran JR. Non-Linguistic Grammar Learning by 12-Month-Old Infants: Evidence for Constraints on Learning. JOURNAL OF COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT 2019; 20:433-441. [PMID: 32042276 DOI: 10.1080/15248372.2019.1604525] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
Abstract
Infants acquiring their native language are adept at discovering grammatical patterns. However, it remains unknown whether these learning abilities are limited to language, or available more generally for sequenced input. The current study is a conceptual replication of a prior language study, and was designed to ask whether infants can track phrase structure-like patterns from nonlinguistic auditory materials (sequences of computer alert sounds). One group of 12-month-olds was familiarized with an artificial grammar including predictive dependencies between sounds concatenated into strings, simulating the basic structure of phrases in natural languages. A second group of infants was familiarized with a grammar that lacked predictive dependencies. All infants were tested on the same set of familiar strings vs. novel (grammar-inconsistent) strings. Only infants exposed to the materials containing predictive dependencies showed successful discrimination between the test sentences, replicating the results from linguistic materials, and suggesting that predictive dependencies facilitate learning from nonlinguistic input.
Collapse
|
55
|
Bart E, Hegdé J. Editorial: Deep Learning in Biological, Computer, and Neuromorphic Systems. Front Comput Neurosci 2019; 13:11. [PMID: 30930760 PMCID: PMC6423909 DOI: 10.3389/fncom.2019.00011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/13/2018] [Accepted: 02/14/2019] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Affiliation(s)
- Evgeniy Bart
- Palo Alto Research Center, Palo Alto, CA, United States
| | - Jay Hegdé
- Department of Neuroscience and Regenerative Medicine, James and Jean Culver Vision Discovery Institute, The Graduate School, and Department of Ophthalmology, Medical College of Georgia, Augusta University, Augusta, GA, United States
| |
Collapse
|
56
|
Fló A, Brusini P, Macagno F, Nespor M, Mehler J, Ferry AL. Newborns are sensitive to multiple cues for word segmentation in continuous speech. Dev Sci 2019; 22:e12802. [PMID: 30681763 DOI: 10.1111/desc.12802] [Citation(s) in RCA: 31] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/02/2018] [Revised: 01/19/2019] [Accepted: 01/21/2019] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
Before infants can learn words, they must identify those words in continuous speech. Yet, the speech signal lacks obvious boundary markers, which poses a potential problem for language acquisition (Swingley, Philos Trans R Soc Lond. Series B, Biol Sci 364(1536), 3617-3632, 2009). By the middle of the first year, infants seem to have solved this problem (Bergelson & Swingley, Proc Natl Acad Sci 109(9), 3253-3258, 2012; Jusczyk & Aslin, Cogn Psychol 29, 1-23, 1995), but it is unknown if segmentation abilities are present from birth, or if they only emerge after sufficient language exposure and/or brain maturation. Here, in two independent experiments, we looked at two cues known to be crucial for the segmentation of human speech: the computation of statistical co-occurrences between syllables and the use of the language's prosody. After a brief familiarization of about 3 min with continuous speech, using functional near-infrared spectroscopy, neonates showed differential brain responses on a recognition test to words that violated either the statistical (Experiment 1) or prosodic (Experiment 2) boundaries of the familiarization, compared to words that conformed to those boundaries. Importantly, word recognition in Experiment 2 occurred even in the absence of prosodic information at test, meaning that newborns encoded the phonological content independently of its prosody. These data indicate that humans are born with operational language processing and memory capacities and can use at least two types of cues to segment otherwise continuous speech, a key first step in language acquisition.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ana Fló
- Language, Cognition, and Development Laboratory, Scuola Internazionale di Studi Avanzati, Trieste, Italy.,Cognitive Neuroimaging Unit, Commissariat à l'Energie Atomique (CEA), Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM) U992, NeuroSpin Center, Gif-sur-Yvette, France
| | - Perrine Brusini
- Language, Cognition, and Development Laboratory, Scuola Internazionale di Studi Avanzati, Trieste, Italy.,Institute of Psychology Health and Society, University of Liverpool, Liverpool, UK
| | - Francesco Macagno
- Neonatology Unit, Azienda Ospedaliera Santa Maria della Misericordia, Udine, Italy
| | - Marina Nespor
- Language, Cognition, and Development Laboratory, Scuola Internazionale di Studi Avanzati, Trieste, Italy
| | - Jacques Mehler
- Language, Cognition, and Development Laboratory, Scuola Internazionale di Studi Avanzati, Trieste, Italy
| | - Alissa L Ferry
- Language, Cognition, and Development Laboratory, Scuola Internazionale di Studi Avanzati, Trieste, Italy.,Division of Human Communication, Hearing, and Development, University of Manchester, Manchester, UK
| |
Collapse
|
57
|
Capacities and neural mechanisms for auditory statistical learning across species. Hear Res 2019; 376:97-110. [PMID: 30797628 DOI: 10.1016/j.heares.2019.02.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/24/2018] [Revised: 01/09/2019] [Accepted: 02/06/2019] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
Statistical learning has been proposed as a possible mechanism by which individuals can become sensitive to the structures of language fundamental for speech perception. Since its description in human infants, statistical learning has been described in human adults and several non-human species as a general process by which animals learn about stimulus-relevant statistics. The neurobiology of statistical learning is beginning to be understood, but many questions remain about the underlying mechanisms. Why is the developing brain particularly sensitive to stimulus and environmental statistics, and what neural processes are engaged in the adult brain to enable learning from statistical regularities in the absence of external reward or instruction? This review will survey the statistical learning abilities of humans and non-human animals with a particular focus on communicative vocalizations. We discuss the neurobiological basis of statistical learning, and specifically what can be learned by exploring this process in both humans and laboratory animals. Finally, we describe advantages of studying vocal communication in rodents as a means to further our understanding of the cortical plasticity mechanisms engaged during statistical learning. We examine the use of rodents in the context of pup retrieval, which is an auditory-based and experience-dependent form of maternal behavior.
Collapse
|
58
|
Maheu M, Dehaene S, Meyniel F. Brain signatures of a multiscale process of sequence learning in humans. eLife 2019; 8:41541. [PMID: 30714904 PMCID: PMC6361584 DOI: 10.7554/elife.41541] [Citation(s) in RCA: 46] [Impact Index Per Article: 9.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/29/2018] [Accepted: 01/18/2019] [Indexed: 01/08/2023] Open
Abstract
Extracting the temporal structure of sequences of events is crucial for perception, decision-making, and language processing. Here, we investigate the mechanisms by which the brain acquires knowledge of sequences and the possibility that successive brain responses reflect the progressive extraction of sequence statistics at different timescales. We measured brain activity using magnetoencephalography in humans exposed to auditory sequences with various statistical regularities, and we modeled this activity as theoretical surprise levels using several learning models. Successive brain waves related to different types of statistical inferences. Early post-stimulus brain waves denoted a sensitivity to a simple statistic, the frequency of items estimated over a long timescale (habituation). Mid-latency and late brain waves conformed qualitatively and quantitatively to the computational properties of a more complex inference: the learning of recent transition probabilities. Our findings thus support the existence of multiple computational systems for sequence processing involving statistical inferences at multiple scales.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Maxime Maheu
- Cognitive Neuroimaging Unit, CEA DRF/JOLIOT, INSERM, Université Paris-Sud, Université Paris-Saclay, NeuroSpin center, Gif-sur-Yvette, France.,Université Paris Descartes, Sorbonne Paris Cité, Paris, France
| | - Stanislas Dehaene
- Cognitive Neuroimaging Unit, CEA DRF/JOLIOT, INSERM, Université Paris-Sud, Université Paris-Saclay, NeuroSpin center, Gif-sur-Yvette, France.,Collège de France, Paris, France
| | - Florent Meyniel
- Cognitive Neuroimaging Unit, CEA DRF/JOLIOT, INSERM, Université Paris-Sud, Université Paris-Saclay, NeuroSpin center, Gif-sur-Yvette, France
| |
Collapse
|
59
|
Saffran JR. Statistical learning as a window into developmental disabilities. J Neurodev Disord 2018; 10:35. [PMID: 30541453 PMCID: PMC6292000 DOI: 10.1186/s11689-018-9252-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/15/2017] [Accepted: 11/14/2018] [Indexed: 01/30/2023] Open
Abstract
Until recently, most behavioral studies of children with intellectual and developmental disabilities (IDD) have used standardized assessments as a means to probe etiology and to characterize phenotypes. Over the past decade, however, tasks originally developed to investigate learning processes in typical development have been brought to bear on developmental processes in children with IDD. This brief review will focus on one learning process in particular—statistical learning—and will provide an overview of what has been learned thus far from studies using statistical learning tasks with different groups of children with IDD conditions. While a full picture is not yet available, results to date suggest that studies of learning are both feasible and informative about learning processes that may differ across diagnostic groups, particularly as they relate to language acquisition. More generally, studies focused on learning processes may be highly informative about different developmental trajectories both across groups and within groups of children.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jenny R Saffran
- Waisman Center, University of Wisconsin-Madison, 1500 Highland Ave, Madison, WI, 53705, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
60
|
Spontaneous Learning of Visual Structures in Domestic Chicks. Animals (Basel) 2018; 8:ani8080135. [PMID: 30082590 PMCID: PMC6115858 DOI: 10.3390/ani8080135] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/11/2018] [Revised: 07/31/2018] [Accepted: 08/02/2018] [Indexed: 11/17/2022] Open
Abstract
Simple Summary Our aim is to investigate the recognition of the structure of multi-element configurations; one mechanism that supports communicative functions in different species. Cognitive mechanisms involved in this ability might not have evolved specifically for communicative use, but derive from other functions. Thus, it is crucial to study these abilities in species that are not vocal learners and with stimuli from other modalities. We know already that domestic chicks can learn the temporal statistical structure of sequences of visual shapes, however their abilities to encode the spatial structure of visual patterns (configurations composed of multiple visual elements presented simultaneously side-by-side) is much less known. Using filial imprinting learning, we showed that chicks spontaneously recognize the structure of their imprinting stimulus, preferring it to one composed of the same elements in different configurations. Moreover, we found that in their affiliative responses chicks give priority to information located at the stimulus edges, a phenomenon that was so far observed only with temporal sequences. This first evidence of a spontaneous edge bias with spatial stimuli further stresses the importance of studying similarities and differences between the processing of linguistic and nonlinguistic stimuli and of stimuli presented in various sensory modalities. Abstract Effective communication crucially depends on the ability to produce and recognize structured signals, as apparent in language and birdsong. Although it is not clear to what extent similar syntactic-like abilities can be identified in other animals, recently we reported that domestic chicks can learn abstract visual patterns and the statistical structure defined by a temporal sequence of visual shapes. However, little is known about chicks’ ability to process spatial/positional information from visual configurations. Here, we used filial imprinting as an unsupervised learning mechanism to study spontaneous encoding of the structure of a configuration of different shapes. After being exposed to a triplet of shapes (ABC or CAB), chicks could discriminate those triplets from a permutation of the same shapes in different order (CAB or ABC), revealing a sensitivity to the spatial arrangement of the elements. When tested with a fragment taken from the imprinting triplet that followed the familiar adjacency-relationships (AB or BC) vs. one in which the shapes maintained their position with respect to the stimulus edges (AC), chicks revealed a preference for the configuration with familiar edge elements, showing an edge bias previously found only with temporal sequences.
Collapse
|
61
|
Auditory midbrain coding of statistical learning that results from discontinuous sensory stimulation. PLoS Biol 2018; 16:e2005114. [PMID: 30048446 PMCID: PMC6065201 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pbio.2005114] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/13/2017] [Accepted: 06/21/2018] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Detecting regular patterns in the environment, a process known as statistical
learning, is essential for survival. Neuronal adaptation is a key mechanism in
the detection of patterns that are continuously repeated across short (seconds
to minutes) temporal windows. Here, we found in mice that a subcortical
structure in the auditory midbrain was sensitive to patterns that were repeated
discontinuously, in a temporally sparse manner, across windows of minutes to
hours. Using a combination of behavioral, electrophysiological, and molecular
approaches, we found changes in neuronal response gain that varied in mechanism
with the degree of sound predictability and resulted in changes in frequency
coding. Analysis of population activity (structural tuning) revealed an increase
in frequency classification accuracy in the context of increased overlap in
responses across frequencies. The increase in accuracy and overlap was
paralleled at the behavioral level in an increase in generalization in the
absence of diminished discrimination. Gain modulation was accompanied by changes
in gene and protein expression, indicative of long-term plasticity.
Physiological changes were largely independent of corticofugal feedback, and no
changes were seen in upstream cochlear nucleus responses, suggesting a key role
of the auditory midbrain in sensory gating. Subsequent behavior demonstrated
learning of predictable and random patterns and their importance in auditory
conditioning. Using longer timescales than previously explored, the combined
data show that the auditory midbrain codes statistical learning of temporally
sparse patterns, a process that is critical for the detection of relevant
stimuli in the constant soundscape that the animal navigates through. Some things are learned simply because they are there and not because they are
relevant at that moment in time. This is particularly true of surrounding
sounds, which we process automatically and continuously, detecting their
repetitive patterns or singularities. Learning about rewards and punishment is
typically attributed to cortical structures in the brain and known to occur over
long time windows. Learning of surrounding regularities, on the other hand, is
attributed to subcortical structures and has been shown to occur in seconds. The
brain can, however, also detect the regularity in sounds that are
discontinuously repeated across intervals of minutes and hours. For example, we
learn to identify people by the sound of their steps through an unconscious
process involving repeated but isolated exposures to the coappearance of sound
and person. Here, we show that a subcortical structure, the auditory midbrain,
can code such temporally spread regularities. Neurons in the auditory midbrain
changed their response pattern in mice that heard a fixed tone whenever they
went into one room in the environment they lived in. Learning of temporally
spread sound patterns can, therefore, occur in subcortical structures.
Collapse
|
62
|
Colagè I, d'Errico F. Culture: The Driving Force of Human Cognition. Top Cogn Sci 2018; 12:654-672. [DOI: 10.1111/tops.12372] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/29/2018] [Revised: 06/20/2018] [Accepted: 06/22/2018] [Indexed: 12/18/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Ivan Colagè
- Faculty of Philosophy Pontifical Antonianum University
- DISF Centre Pontifical University of the Holy Cross
| | - Francesco d'Errico
- UMR‐CNRS 5199 de la Préhistoire à l'Actuel: Culture, Environnement et Anthropologie (PACEA) Université de Bordeaux
- SFF Centre for Early Sapiens Behaviour (SapienCE) University of Bergen
| |
Collapse
|
63
|
|
64
|
Montefusco-Siegmund R, Toro M, Maldonado PE, Aylwin MDLL. Unsupervised visual discrimination learning of complex stimuli: Accuracy, bias and generalization. Vision Res 2018; 148:37-48. [PMID: 29775623 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2018.05.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/11/2018] [Revised: 05/01/2018] [Accepted: 05/04/2018] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
Through same-different judgements, we can discriminate an immense variety of stimuli and consequently, they are critical in our everyday interaction with the environment. The quality of the judgements depends on familiarity with stimuli. A way to improve the discrimination is through learning, but to this day, we lack direct evidence of how learning shapes the same-different judgments with complex stimuli. We studied unsupervised visual discrimination learning in 42 participants, as they performed same-different judgments with two types of unfamiliar complex stimuli in the absence of labeling or individuation. Across nine daily training sessions with equiprobable same and different stimuli pairs, participants increased the sensitivity and the criterion by reducing the errors with both same and different pairs. With practice, there was a superior performance for different pairs and a bias for different response. To evaluate the process underlying this bias, we manipulated the proportion of same and different pairs, which resulted in an additional proportion-induced bias, suggesting that the bias observed with equal proportions was a stimulus processing bias. Overall, these results suggest that unsupervised discrimination learning occurs through changes in the stimulus processing that increase the sensory evidence and/or the precision of the working memory. Finally, the acquired discrimination ability was fully transferred to novel exemplars of the practiced stimuli category, in agreement with the acquisition of a category specific perceptual expertise.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rodrigo Montefusco-Siegmund
- Instituto de Neurociencia Biomédica, Universidad de Chile, Santiago, Chile; Escuela de Kinesiología, Universidad Austral de Chile, Valdivia, Chile
| | - Mauricio Toro
- Centro de Investigación Avanzada en Educación, Universidad de Chile, Santiago, Chile
| | - Pedro E Maldonado
- Instituto de Neurociencia Biomédica, Universidad de Chile, Santiago, Chile; Departamento de Neurociencias, Facultad de Medicina, Universidad de Chile, Santiago, Chile
| | - María de la L Aylwin
- Centro de Investigación Avanzada en Educación, Universidad de Chile, Santiago, Chile; Escuela de Medicina, Universidad de Talca, Talca, Chile.
| |
Collapse
|
65
|
Castro L, Wasserman EA, Lauffer M. Unsupervised learning of complex associations in an animal model. Cognition 2018; 173:28-33. [PMID: 29289794 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2017.12.015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/19/2017] [Revised: 12/20/2017] [Accepted: 12/21/2017] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
Abstract
Supervised learning results from explicit corrective feedback, whereas unsupervised learning results from statistical co-occurrence. In an initial training phase, we gave pigeons an unsupervised learning task to see if mere pairing could establish associations between multiple pairs of visual images. To assess learning, we administered occasional testing trials in which pigeons were shown an object and had to choose between previously paired and unpaired tokens. Learning was evidenced by preferential choice of the previously unpaired token. In a subsequent supervised training phase, learning was facilitated if the object and token had previously been paired. These results document unsupervised learning in pigeons and resemble statistical learning in infants, suggesting an important parallel between human and animal cognition.
Collapse
|