101
|
Bollu T, Whitehead SC, Prasad N, Walker J, Shyamkumar N, Subramaniam R, Kardon B, Cohen I, Goldberg JH. Automated home cage training of mice in a hold-still center-out reach task. J Neurophysiol 2018; 121:500-512. [PMID: 30540551 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00667.2018] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/13/2022] Open
Abstract
An obstacle to understanding neural mechanisms of movement is the complex, distributed nature of the mammalian motor system. Here we present a novel behavioral paradigm for high-throughput dissection of neural circuits underlying mouse forelimb control. Custom touch-sensing joysticks were used to quantify mouse forelimb trajectories with micron-millisecond spatiotemporal resolution. Joysticks were integrated into computer-controlled, rack-mountable home cages, enabling batches of mice to be trained in parallel. Closed loop behavioral analysis enabled online control of reward delivery for automated training. We used this system to show that mice can learn, with no human handling, a direction-specific hold-still center-out reach task in which a mouse first held its right forepaw still before reaching out to learned spatial targets. Stabilogram diffusion analysis of submillimeter-scale micromovements produced during the hold demonstrate that an active control process, akin to upright balance, was implemented to maintain forepaw stability. Trajectory decomposition methods, previously used in primates, were used to segment hundreds of thousands of forelimb trajectories into millions of constituent kinematic primitives. This system enables rapid dissection of neural circuits for controlling motion primitives from which forelimb sequences are built. NEW & NOTEWORTHY A novel joystick design resolves mouse forelimb kinematics with micron-millisecond precision. Home cage training is used to train mice in a hold-still center-out reach task. Analytical methods, previously used in primates, are used to decompose mouse forelimb trajectories into kinematic primitives.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tejapratap Bollu
- Department of Neurobiology and Behavior, Cornell University , Ithaca, New York
| | | | - Nikil Prasad
- Department of Neurobiology and Behavior, Cornell University , Ithaca, New York
| | - Jackson Walker
- Department of Neurobiology and Behavior, Cornell University , Ithaca, New York
| | - Nitin Shyamkumar
- Department of Neurobiology and Behavior, Cornell University , Ithaca, New York
| | - Raghav Subramaniam
- Department of Neurobiology and Behavior, Cornell University , Ithaca, New York
| | - Brian Kardon
- Department of Neurobiology and Behavior, Cornell University , Ithaca, New York
| | - Itai Cohen
- Department of Physics, Cornell University , Ithaca, New York
| | - Jesse H Goldberg
- Department of Neurobiology and Behavior, Cornell University , Ithaca, New York
| |
Collapse
|
102
|
Verdi S, Jackson MA, Beaumont M, Bowyer RCE, Bell JT, Spector TD, Steves CJ. An Investigation Into Physical Frailty as a Link Between the Gut Microbiome and Cognitive Health. Front Aging Neurosci 2018; 10:398. [PMID: 30564113 PMCID: PMC6288358 DOI: 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00398] [Citation(s) in RCA: 41] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/14/2018] [Accepted: 11/19/2018] [Indexed: 12/26/2022] Open
Abstract
The preservation of cognitive abilities with aging is a priority both for individuals and nations given the aging populations of many countries. Recently the gut microbiome has been identified as a new territory to explore in relation to cognition. Experiments using rodents have identified a link between the gut microbiome and cognitive function, particularly that low microbial diversity leads to poor cognition function. Similar studies in humans could identify novel targets to encourage healthy cognition in an aging population. Here, we investigate the association of gut microbiota and cognitive function in a human cohort considering the influence of physical frailty. We analyzed 16S rRNA gene sequence data, derived from fecal samples obtained from 1,551 individuals over the age of 40. Cognitive data was collected using four cognitive tests: verbal fluency (n = 1,368), Deary-Liewald Reaction Time Test (DLRT; n = 873), Mini Mental State Examination (recall; n = 1,374) and Paired Associates Learning from the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB-PAL; n = 405). We use mixed effects models to identify associations with alpha diversity, operational taxonomic units (OTUs) and taxa and performed further analyses adjusting for physical frailty. We then repeated the analyses in a subset of individuals with dietary data, also excluding those using medications shown to influence gut microbiome composition. DLRT and verbal fluency were negatively associated with alpha diversity of the gut microbiota (False-Discovery Rate, FDR, p < 0.05). However, when considering frailty as a covariate, only associations between the DLRT and diversity measures remained. Repeating analyses excluding Proton pump inhibitor (PPI) and antibiotic users and accounting for diet, we similarly observe significant negative associations between the DLRT and alpha diversity measures and a further negative association between DLRT and the abundance of the order Burkholderiales that remains significant after adjusting for host frailty. This highlights the importance of considering concurrent differences in physical health in studies of cognitive performance and suggests that physical health has a relatively larger association with the gut microbiome. However, the frailty independent cognitive-gut microbiota associations that were observed might represent important targets for further research, with potential for use in diagnostic surveillance in cognitive aging and interventions to improve vitality.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Serena Verdi
- Department of Twin Research and Genetic Epidemiology, King’s College London, London, United Kingdom
- Neuropsychopharmacology Unit, Centre for Psychiatry, Division of Brain Sciences, Imperial College London, London, United Kingdom
| | - Matthew A. Jackson
- Department of Twin Research and Genetic Epidemiology, King’s College London, London, United Kingdom
- Kennedy Institute of Rheumatology, University of Oxford, Oxford, United Kingdom
| | - Michelle Beaumont
- Department of Twin Research and Genetic Epidemiology, King’s College London, London, United Kingdom
| | - Ruth C. E. Bowyer
- Department of Twin Research and Genetic Epidemiology, King’s College London, London, United Kingdom
| | - Jordana T. Bell
- Department of Twin Research and Genetic Epidemiology, King’s College London, London, United Kingdom
| | - Tim D. Spector
- Department of Twin Research and Genetic Epidemiology, King’s College London, London, United Kingdom
| | - Claire J. Steves
- Department of Twin Research and Genetic Epidemiology, King’s College London, London, United Kingdom
- Clinical Age Research Unit, Department of Clinical Gerontology, King’s College Hospital, NHS Foundation Trust, London, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
103
|
Seegelke C, Wühr P. Compatibility between object size and response side in grasping: the left hand prefers smaller objects, the right hand prefers larger objects. PeerJ 2018; 6:e6026. [PMID: 30533312 PMCID: PMC6282946 DOI: 10.7717/peerj.6026] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/10/2018] [Accepted: 10/29/2018] [Indexed: 11/20/2022] Open
Abstract
It has been proposed that the brain processes quantities such as space, size, number, and other magnitudes using a common neural metric, and that this common representation system reflects a direct link to motor control, because the integration of spatial, temporal, and other quantity-related information is fundamental for sensorimotor transformation processes. In the present study, we examined compatibility effects between physical stimulus size and spatial (response) location during a sensorimotor task. Participants reached and grasped for a small or large object with either their non-dominant left or their dominant right hand. Our results revealed that participants initiated left hand movements faster when grasping the small cube compared to the large cube, whereas they initiated right hand movements faster when grasping the large cube compared to the small cube. Moreover, the compatibility effect influenced the timing of grip aperture kinematics. These findings indicate that the interaction between object size and response hand affects the planning of grasping movements and supports the notion of a strong link between the cognitive representation of (object) size, spatial (response) parameters, and sensorimotor control.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Christian Seegelke
- Biopsychology and Cognitive Neuroscience, Faculty of Psychology and Sport Sciences, Bielefeld University, Bielefeld, Germany
- Center of Excellence Cognitive Interaction Technology (CITEC), Bielefeld University, Bielefeld, Germany
| | - Peter Wühr
- Institute of Psychology, TU Dortmund University, Dortmund, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
104
|
Ingram TGJ, Solomon JP, Westwood DA, Boe SG. Movement related sensory feedback is not necessary for learning to execute a motor skill. Behav Brain Res 2018; 359:135-142. [PMID: 30392851 DOI: 10.1016/j.bbr.2018.10.030] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/03/2018] [Revised: 10/20/2018] [Accepted: 10/21/2018] [Indexed: 12/20/2022]
Abstract
Sensory feedback has traditionally been considered critical for motor learning. While it has been shown that motor learning can occur in the absence of visual or somatosensory feedback, it is thought that at least one must be present. This assumption contrasts with literature demonstrating that motor imagery (MI) - the mental rehearsal of a movement - is capable of driving motor learning even though the lack of actual execution precludes sensory feedback related to movement. However, studies of MI typically employ simple tasks that do not require improvements in motor execution per se, suggesting that MI might improve task performance primarily through perceptual mechanisms. To avoid this limitation, we designed a novel motor task requiring the repeated execution of unfamiliar kinematic trajectories where learning was assessed through changes in the speed-accuracy function (SAF) across five sessions. General task performance was controlled for by assessing performance on randomly generated trajectories. Groups included physical practice (PP; with and without added visual feedback), MI, and perceptual control (PC), the latter of which only observed the trajectories. All groups performed physically on the final session. Upon the final session, the MI group performed better than the PC group, and better than initial session PP performance. These results suggest that motor learning occurred in the MI group despite the lack of sensory feedback related to the movement, and that this learning was not simply the result of perceptual learning. Our results question long-standing assumptions about MI based learning and the necessity of feedback in motor learning generally.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tony G J Ingram
- Laboratory for Brain Recovery and Function, Dalhousie University, Canada; Department of Psychology & Neuroscience, Dalhousie University, Canada
| | - Jack P Solomon
- Laboratory for Brain Recovery and Function, Dalhousie University, Canada; Department of Psychology & Neuroscience, Dalhousie University, Canada
| | - David A Westwood
- Department of Psychology & Neuroscience, Dalhousie University, Canada; School of Health and Human Performance, Dalhousie University, Canada
| | - Shaun G Boe
- Laboratory for Brain Recovery and Function, Dalhousie University, Canada; Department of Psychology & Neuroscience, Dalhousie University, Canada; School of Health and Human Performance, Dalhousie University, Canada; School of Physiotherapy, Dalhousie University, Canada.
| |
Collapse
|
105
|
Delmas S, Casamento-Moran A, Park SH, Yacoubi B, Christou EA. Motor planning perturbation: muscle activation and reaction time. J Neurophysiol 2018; 120:2059-2065. [PMID: 29947595 PMCID: PMC6230771 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00323.2018] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/15/2018] [Revised: 06/22/2018] [Accepted: 06/26/2018] [Indexed: 12/15/2022] Open
Abstract
Reaction time (RT) is the time interval between the appearance of a stimulus and initiation of a motor response. Within RT, two processes occur, selection of motor goals and motor planning. An unresolved question is whether perturbation to the motor planning component of RT slows the response and alters the voluntary activation of muscle. The purpose of this study was to determine how the modulation of muscle activity during an RT response changes with motor plan perturbation. Twenty-four young adults (20.5 ±1.1 yr, 13 women) performed 15 trials of an isometric RT task with ankle dorsiflexion using a sinusoidal anticipatory strategy (10-20% maximum voluntary contraction). We compared the processing part of the RT and modulation of muscle activity from 10 to 60 Hz of the tibialis anterior (primary agonist) when the stimulus appeared at the trough or at the peak of the sinusoidal task. We found that RT ( P = 0.003) was longer when the stimulus occurred at the peak compared with the trough. During the time of the reaction, the electromyography (EMG) power from 10 to 35 Hz was less at the peak than the trough ( P = 0.019), whereas the EMG power from 35 to 60 Hz was similar between the peak and trough ( P = 0.92). These results suggest that perturbation to motor planning lengthens the processing part of RT and alters the voluntary activation of the muscle by decreasing the relative amount of power from 10 to 35 Hz. NEW & NOTEWORTHY We aimed to determine whether perturbation to motor planning would alter the speed and muscle activity of the response. We compared trials when a stimulus appeared at the peak or trough of an oscillatory reaction time task. When the stimulus occurred at the trough, participants responded faster, with greater force, and less EMG power from 10-35 Hz. We provide evidence that motor planning perturbation slows the response and alters the voluntary activity of the muscle.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Stefan Delmas
- Department of Applied Physiology and Kinesiology, University of Florida , Gainesville, Florida
| | | | - Seoung Hoon Park
- Department of Applied Physiology and Kinesiology, University of Florida , Gainesville, Florida
| | - Basma Yacoubi
- Department of Applied Physiology and Kinesiology, University of Florida , Gainesville, Florida
| | - Evangelos A Christou
- Department of Applied Physiology and Kinesiology, University of Florida , Gainesville, Florida
| |
Collapse
|
106
|
The grasping side of post-error slowing. Cognition 2018; 179:1-13. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.05.026] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/29/2017] [Revised: 05/28/2018] [Accepted: 05/31/2018] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
|
107
|
Pereira J, Sburlea AI, Müller-Putz GR. EEG patterns of self-paced movement imaginations towards externally-cued and internally-selected targets. Sci Rep 2018; 8:13394. [PMID: 30190543 PMCID: PMC6127278 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-018-31673-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/13/2018] [Accepted: 08/23/2018] [Indexed: 11/25/2022] Open
Abstract
In this study, we investigate the neurophysiological signature of the interacting processes which lead to a single reach-and-grasp movement imagination (MI). While performing this task, the human healthy participants could either define their movement targets according to an external cue, or through an internal selection process. After defining their target, they could start the MI whenever they wanted. We recorded high density electroencephalographic (EEG) activity and investigated two neural correlates: the event-related potentials (ERPs) associated with the target selection, which reflect the perceptual and cognitive processes prior to the MI, and the movement-related cortical potentials (MRCPs), associated with the planning of the self-paced MI. We found differences in frontal and parietal areas between the late ERP components related to the internally-driven selection and the externally-cued process. Furthermore, we could reliably estimate the MI onset of the self-paced task. Next, we extracted MRCP features around the MI onset to train classifiers of movement vs. rest directly on self-paced MI data. We attained performance significantly higher than chance level for both time-locked and asynchronous classification. These findings contribute to the development of more intuitive brain-computer interfaces in which movement targets are defined internally and the movements are self-paced.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Joana Pereira
- Institute of Neural Engineering, Graz University of Technology, Graz, Austria
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
108
|
Surkar SM, Hoffman RM, Davies B, Harbourne R, Kurz MJ. Impaired anticipatory vision and visuomotor coordination affects action planning and execution in children with hemiplegic cerebral palsy. RESEARCH IN DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES 2018; 80:64-73. [PMID: 29940386 DOI: 10.1016/j.ridd.2018.06.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/08/2017] [Revised: 03/12/2018] [Accepted: 06/19/2018] [Indexed: 06/08/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Action-planning and execution deficits in children with hemiplegic cerebral palsy (HCP) are potentially due to deficits in the integration of sensory information, such as vision, with motor output. AIMS To determine differences in anticipatory visual patterns in children with HCP compared to typically developing (TD) children, and to assess visuomotor coordination in children with HCP. METHODS AND PROCEDURES We included 13 children with HCP (Age = 6.8 + 2.9 yrs) and 15 TD children (Age = 5.8 + 1.1 yrs). The experimental task used in this study is a valid action-planning task, which consisted of initially reaching and grasping an object placed at a fixed position, followed by placing the object in a random target position. Visual patterns were recorded using a head-mounted eye-tracker system and arm movements were recorded using motion capture (120 Hz). OUTCOMES AND RESULTS Children with HCP had delayed anticipatory gaze time and longer latency than TD children during the planning and execution phases. Children with HCP also had a higher frequency of gaze shifts, longer reaction times (RT) and movement times (MT) than TD children. CONCLUSIONS AND IMPLICATIONS Children with HCP may have deficits in anticipatory vision, which potentially affected planning and executing a goal-directed action. Therapeutic interventions focusing on improving visuomotor coordination may improve the motor performance in children with HCP.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Swati M Surkar
- Munroe Meyer Institute of Genetics and Rehabilitation, University of Nebraska Medical Center, Sensorimotor Learning Laboratory, Department of Physical Therapy, 985450 Nebraska Medical Center Omaha, NE, 68198-5450, United States.
| | - Rashelle M Hoffman
- Munroe Meyer Institute of Genetics and Rehabilitation, University of Nebraska Medical Center, Sensorimotor Learning Laboratory, Department of Physical Therapy, 985450 Nebraska Medical Center Omaha, NE, 68198-5450, United States.
| | - Brenda Davies
- Munroe Meyer Institute of Genetics and Rehabilitation, University of Nebraska Medical Center, Sensorimotor Learning Laboratory, Department of Physical Therapy, 985450 Nebraska Medical Center Omaha, NE, 68198-5450, United States.
| | - Regina Harbourne
- John G. Rangos School of Health Sciences, Duquesne University, Department of Physical Therapy, 600 Forbes Avenue, Pittsburgh, PA, 15282, United States.
| | - Max J Kurz
- Munroe Meyer Institute of Genetics and Rehabilitation, University of Nebraska Medical Center, Sensorimotor Learning Laboratory, Department of Physical Therapy, 985450 Nebraska Medical Center Omaha, NE, 68198-5450, United States.
| |
Collapse
|
109
|
Venkataratamani PV, Murthy A. Distinct mechanisms explain the control of reach speed planning: evidence from a race model framework. J Neurophysiol 2018; 120:1293-1306. [PMID: 29766768 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00707.2017] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Previous studies have investigated the computational architecture underlying the voluntary control of reach movements that demands a change in position or direction of movement planning. Here we used a novel task in which subjects had to either increase or decrease the movement speed according to a change in target color that occurred randomly during a trial. The applicability of different race models to such a speed redirect task was assessed. We found that the predictions of an independent race model that instantiated an abort-and-replan strategy was consistent with all aspects of performance in the fast-to-slow speed condition. The results from modeling indicated a peculiar asymmetry, in that although the fast-to-slow speed change required inhibition, none of the standard race models was able to explain how movements changed from slow to fast speeds. Interestingly, a weighted averaging model that simulated the gradual merging of two kinematic plans explained behavior in the slow-to-fast speed task. In summary, our work shows how a race model framework can provide an understanding of how the brain controls different aspects of reach movement planning and help distinguish between an abort-and-replan strategy and merging of plans. NEW & NOTEWORTHY For the first time, a race model framework was used to understand how reach speeds are modified. We provide evidence that a fast-to-slow speed change required aborting the current plan and a complete respecification of a new plan, while none of the race models was able to explain an instructed increase of hand movement speed, which was instead accomplished by a merging of a new kinematic plan with the existing kinematic plan.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Aditya Murthy
- Center for Neuroscience, Indian Institute of Science , Bangalore , India
| |
Collapse
|
110
|
Lara AH, Elsayed GF, Zimnik AJ, Cunningham JP, Churchland MM. Conservation of preparatory neural events in monkey motor cortex regardless of how movement is initiated. eLife 2018; 7:31826. [PMID: 30132759 PMCID: PMC6112854 DOI: 10.7554/elife.31826] [Citation(s) in RCA: 52] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/08/2017] [Accepted: 07/26/2018] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
A time-consuming preparatory stage is hypothesized to precede voluntary movement. A putative neural substrate of motor preparation occurs when a delay separates instruction and execution cues. When readiness is sustained during the delay, sustained neural activity is observed in motor and premotor areas. Yet whether delay-period activity reflects an essential preparatory stage is controversial. In particular, it has remained ambiguous whether delay-period-like activity appears before non-delayed movements. To overcome that ambiguity, we leveraged a recently developed analysis method that parses population responses into putatively preparatory and movement-related components. We examined cortical responses when reaches were initiated after an imposed delay, at a self-chosen time, or reactively with low latency and no delay. Putatively preparatory events were conserved across all contexts. Our findings support the hypothesis that an appropriate preparatory state is consistently achieved before movement onset. However, our results reveal that this process can consume surprisingly little time.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Antonio H Lara
- Department of Neuroscience, Columbia University Medical Center, New York, United States
| | - Gamaleldin F Elsayed
- Department of Neuroscience, Columbia University Medical Center, New York, United States.,Center for Theoretical Neuroscience, Columbia University, New York, United States
| | - Andrew J Zimnik
- Department of Neuroscience, Columbia University Medical Center, New York, United States
| | - John P Cunningham
- Center for Theoretical Neuroscience, Columbia University, New York, United States.,Grossman Center for the Statistics of Mind, Columbia University Medical Center, New York, Unitedstate.,Department of Statistics, Columbia University, New York, United States
| | - Mark M Churchland
- Department of Neuroscience, Columbia University Medical Center, New York, United States.,Grossman Center for the Statistics of Mind, Columbia University Medical Center, New York, Unitedstate.,David Mahoney Center for Brain and Behavior Research, Columbia University Medical Center, New York, United States.,Kavli Institute for Brain Science, Columbia University Medical Center, New York, United States
| |
Collapse
|
111
|
Neural Dynamics of Variable Grasp-Movement Preparation in the Macaque Frontoparietal Network. J Neurosci 2018; 38:5759-5773. [PMID: 29798892 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.2557-17.2018] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/06/2017] [Revised: 05/01/2018] [Accepted: 05/20/2018] [Indexed: 01/20/2023] Open
Abstract
Our voluntary grasping actions lie on a continuum between immediate action and waiting for the right moment, depending on the context. Therefore, studying grasping requires an investigation into how preparation time affects this process. Two macaque monkeys (Macaca mulatta; one male, one female) performed a grasping task with a short instruction followed by an immediate or delayed go cue (0-1300 ms) while we recorded in parallel from neurons in the grasp preparation relevant area F5 that is part of the ventral premotor cortex, and the anterior intraparietal area (AIP). Initial population dynamics followed a fixed trajectory in the neural state space unique to each grip type, reflecting unavoidable movement selection, then diverged depending on the delay, reaching unique states not achieved for immediately cued movements. Population activity in the AIP was less dynamic, whereas F5 activity continued to evolve throughout the delay. Interestingly, neuronal populations from both areas allowed for a readout tracking subjective anticipation of the go cue that predicted single-trial reaction time. However, the prediction of reaction time was better from F5 activity. Intriguingly, activity during movement initiation clustered into two trajectory groups, corresponding to movements that were either "as fast as possible" or withheld movements, demonstrating a widespread state shift in the frontoparietal grasping network when movements must be withheld. Our results reveal how dissociation between immediate and delay-specific preparatory activity, as well as differentiation between cortical areas, is possible through population-level analysis.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Sometimes when we move, we consciously plan our movements. At other times, we move instantly, seemingly with no planning at all. Yet, it's unclear how preparation for movements along this spectrum of planned and seemingly unplanned movement differs in the brain. Two macaque monkeys made reach-to-grasp movements after varying amounts of preparation time while we recorded from the premotor and parietal cortex. We found that the initial response to a grasp instruction was specific to the required movement, but not to the preparation time, reflecting required movement selection. However, when more preparation time was given, neural activity achieved unique states that likely related to withholding movements and anticipation of movement, shedding light on the roles of the premotor and parietal cortex in grasp planning.
Collapse
|
112
|
Bakker RS, Selen LPJ, Medendorp WP. Reference frames in the decisions of hand choice. J Neurophysiol 2018; 119:1809-1817. [DOI: 10.1152/jn.00738.2017] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
For the brain to decide on a reaching movement, it needs to select which hand to use. A number of body-centered factors affect this decision, such as the anticipated movement costs of each arm, recent choice success, handedness, and task demands. While the position of each hand relative to the target is also known to be an important spatial factor, it is unclear which reference frames coordinate the spatial aspects in the decisions of hand choice. Here we tested the role of gaze- and head-centered reference frames in a hand selection task. With their head and gaze oriented in different directions, we measured hand choice of 19 right-handed subjects instructed to make unimanual reaching movements to targets at various directions relative to their body. Using an adaptive procedure, we determined the target angle that led to equiprobable right/left hand choices. When gaze remained fixed relative to the body this balanced target angle shifted systematically with head orientation, and when head orientation remained fixed this choice measure shifted with gaze. These results suggest that a mixture of head- and gaze-centered reference frames is involved in the spatially guided decisions of hand choice, perhaps to flexibly bind this process to the mechanisms of target selection. NEW & NOTEWORTHY Decisions of target and hand choice are fundamental aspects of human reaching movements. While the reference frames involved in target choice have been identified, it is unclear which reference frames are involved in hand selection. We tested the role of gaze- and head-centered reference frames in a hand selection task. Findings emphasize the role of both spatial reference frames in the decisions of hand choice, in addition to known body-centered computations such anticipated movement costs and handedness.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Romy S. Bakker
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Nijmegen, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | - Luc P. J. Selen
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Nijmegen, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | - W. Pieter Medendorp
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Nijmegen, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
113
|
Magalhães F, Rocha K, Marinho V, Ribeiro J, Oliveira T, Ayres C, Bento T, Leite F, Gupta D, Bastos VH, Velasques B, Ribeiro P, Orsini M, Teixeira S. Neurochemical changes in basal ganglia affect time perception in parkinsonians. J Biomed Sci 2018; 25:26. [PMID: 29554962 PMCID: PMC5858149 DOI: 10.1186/s12929-018-0428-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/10/2018] [Accepted: 03/08/2018] [Indexed: 01/25/2023] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND Parkinson's disease is described as resulting from dopaminergic cells progressive degeneration, specifically in the substantia nigra pars compacta that influence the voluntary movements control, decision making and time perception. AIM This review had a goal to update the relation between time perception and Parkinson's Disease. METHODOLOGY We used the PRISMA methodology for this investigation built guided for subjects dopaminergic dysfunction in the time judgment, pharmacological models with levodopa and new studies on the time perception in Parkinson's Disease. We researched on databases Scielo, Pubmed / Medline and ISI Web of Knowledge on August 2017 and repeated in September 2017 and February 2018 using terms and associations relevant for obtaining articles in English about the aspects neurobiology incorporated in time perception. No publication status or restriction of publication date was imposed, but we used as exclusion criteria: dissertations, book reviews, conferences or editorial work. RESULTS/DISCUSSION We have demonstrated that the time cognitive processes are underlying to performance in cognitive tasks and that many are the brain areas and functions involved and the modulators in the time perception performance. CONCLUSIONS The influence of dopaminergic on Parkinson's Disease is an important research tool in Neuroscience while allowing for the search for clarifications regarding behavioral phenotypes of Parkinson's disease patients and to study the areas of the brain that are involved in the dopaminergic circuit and their integration with the time perception mechanisms.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Francisco Magalhães
- Brain Mapping and Plasticity Laboratory, Federal University of Piauí, Av. São Sebastião n° 2819, Nossa Sra. de Fátima, Parnaíba, PI, 64202-020, Brazil. .,The Northeast Biotechnology Network (RENORBIO), Federal University of Piauí, Teresina, Brazil.
| | - Kaline Rocha
- Brain Mapping and Plasticity Laboratory, Federal University of Piauí, Av. São Sebastião n° 2819, Nossa Sra. de Fátima, Parnaíba, PI, 64202-020, Brazil.,The Northeast Biotechnology Network (RENORBIO), Federal University of Piauí, Teresina, Brazil
| | - Victor Marinho
- Brain Mapping and Plasticity Laboratory, Federal University of Piauí, Av. São Sebastião n° 2819, Nossa Sra. de Fátima, Parnaíba, PI, 64202-020, Brazil.,The Northeast Biotechnology Network (RENORBIO), Federal University of Piauí, Teresina, Brazil
| | - Jéssica Ribeiro
- Brain Mapping and Plasticity Laboratory, Federal University of Piauí, Av. São Sebastião n° 2819, Nossa Sra. de Fátima, Parnaíba, PI, 64202-020, Brazil
| | - Thomaz Oliveira
- Brain Mapping and Plasticity Laboratory, Federal University of Piauí, Av. São Sebastião n° 2819, Nossa Sra. de Fátima, Parnaíba, PI, 64202-020, Brazil
| | - Carla Ayres
- Brain Mapping and Plasticity Laboratory, Federal University of Piauí, Av. São Sebastião n° 2819, Nossa Sra. de Fátima, Parnaíba, PI, 64202-020, Brazil
| | - Thalys Bento
- Brain Mapping and Plasticity Laboratory, Federal University of Piauí, Av. São Sebastião n° 2819, Nossa Sra. de Fátima, Parnaíba, PI, 64202-020, Brazil
| | - Francisca Leite
- Brain Mapping and Plasticity Laboratory, Federal University of Piauí, Av. São Sebastião n° 2819, Nossa Sra. de Fátima, Parnaíba, PI, 64202-020, Brazil
| | - Daya Gupta
- Department of Biology, Camden County College, Blackwood, NJ, USA
| | - Victor Hugo Bastos
- Laboratory of Brain Mapping and Functionality, Federal University of Piauí, Parnaíba, Brazil
| | - Bruna Velasques
- Brain Mapping and Sensory-Motor Integration Laboratory, Psychiatry Institute of Federal University of Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro, Brazil.,Brain Mapping and Sensory Motor Integration Laboratory, Institute of Psychiatry of Federal University of Rio de Janeiro, Av. Venceslau Braz, 71 - Botafogo, Rio de Janeiro, RJ, 22290-140, Brazil
| | - Pedro Ribeiro
- Brain Mapping and Sensory-Motor Integration Laboratory, Psychiatry Institute of Federal University of Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro, Brazil.,Brain Mapping and Sensory Motor Integration Laboratory, Institute of Psychiatry of Federal University of Rio de Janeiro, Av. Venceslau Braz, 71 - Botafogo, Rio de Janeiro, RJ, 22290-140, Brazil
| | - Marco Orsini
- Rehabilitation Science Program, Analysis of Human Movement Laboratory, Augusto Motta University Center, Rio de Janeiro, Brazil.,Program Professional Master in Applied Science in Health/UNISUAM, Av. Paris, 84, Bonsucesso, Rio de Janeiro, RJ, 21041-020, Brazil
| | - Silmar Teixeira
- Brain Mapping and Plasticity Laboratory, Federal University of Piauí, Av. São Sebastião n° 2819, Nossa Sra. de Fátima, Parnaíba, PI, 64202-020, Brazil.,The Northeast Biotechnology Network (RENORBIO), Federal University of Piauí, Teresina, Brazil
| |
Collapse
|
114
|
Stiers P, Goulas A. Functional connectivity of task context representations in prefrontal nodes of the multiple demand network. Brain Struct Funct 2018; 223:2455-2473. [PMID: 29502145 PMCID: PMC5968070 DOI: 10.1007/s00429-018-1638-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/23/2017] [Accepted: 02/21/2018] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
A subset of regions in the lateral and medial prefrontal cortex and the anterior insula increase their activity level whenever a cognitive task becomes more demanding, regardless of the specific nature of this demand. During execution of a task, these areas and the surrounding cortex temporally encode aspects of the task context in spatially distributed patterns of activity. It is not clear whether these patterns reflect underlying anatomical subnetworks that still exist when task execution has finished. We use fMRI in 12 participants performing alternating blocks of three cognitive tasks to address this question. A first data set is used to define multiple demand regions in each participant. A second dataset from the same participants is used to determine multiple demand voxel assemblies with a preference for one task over the others. We then show that these voxels remain functionally coupled during execution of non-preferred tasks and that they exhibit stronger functional connectivity during rest. This indicates that the assemblies of task preference sharing voxels reflect patterns of underlying anatomical connections. Moreover, we show that voxels preferring the same task have more similar whole brain functional connectivity profiles that are consistent across participants. This suggests that voxel assemblies differ in patterns of input-output connections, most likely reflecting task demand-specific information exchange.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Peter Stiers
- Department of Neuropsychology and Psychopharmacology, Maastricht University, Universiteitssingel 40 (East), 6229 ER, Maastricht, The Netherlands.
| | - Alexandros Goulas
- Department of Computational Neuroscience, University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Martinistr. 52, 20246, Hamburg, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
115
|
Poole BJ, Mather M, Livesey EJ, Harris IM, Harris JA. Motor-evoked potentials reveal functional differences between dominant and non-dominant motor cortices during response preparation. Cortex 2018. [PMID: 29533856 DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2018.02.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Abstract
Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) of the motor cortex produces motor-evoked potentials (MEPs) in contralateral muscles. The amplitude of these MEPs can be used to measure the excitability of the corticospinal tract during motor planning. In two experiments, we investigated learning-related changes in corticospinal excitability as subjects prepared to respond in a choice reaction-time task. Subjects responded with their left or right hand to a left or right arrow, and on some trials the arrow was immediately preceded by a warning cue that signaled which response would be required. TMS was applied to the motor cortex during the warning cues, and MEPs were measured in the dominant or non-dominant hand. We observed changes in corticospinal excitability during the warning cue, but these depended on which hand the subject was preparing to respond with, and how experienced they were with the task. When subjects prepared to respond with the non-dominant hand, excitability increased in the non-dominant hemisphere and decreased in the dominant hemisphere. These changes became stronger with task experience, and were accompanied by behavioral improvements in the task. When subjects were preparing a dominant-hand response, the non-dominant hemisphere was suppressed, but this effect disappeared as subjects gained experience with the task. There were no changes in the dominant hemisphere before dominant-hand responses. We conclude that preparing to respond with the non-dominant hand involves temporarily reversing an asymmetry in excitability that normally favors the dominant hemisphere, and that this pattern is enhanced by learning during the task.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Marius Mather
- School of Psychology, University of Sydney, Australia
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
116
|
Stimulating the Healthy Brain to Investigate Neural Correlates of Motor Preparation: A Systematic Review. Neural Plast 2018; 2018:5846096. [PMID: 29670648 PMCID: PMC5835236 DOI: 10.1155/2018/5846096] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/29/2017] [Revised: 11/08/2017] [Accepted: 11/22/2017] [Indexed: 12/30/2022] Open
Abstract
Objective Noninvasive brain stimulation techniques can be used to selectively increase or decrease the excitability of a cortical region, providing a unique opportunity to assess the causal contribution of that region to the process being assessed. The objective of this paper is to systematically examine studies investigating changes in reaction time induced by noninvasive brain stimulation in healthy participants during movement preparation. Methods A systematic review of the literature was performed in the PubMed, MEDLINE, EMBASE, PsycINFO, and Web of science databases. A combination of keywords related to motor preparation, associated behavioral outcomes, and noninvasive brain stimulation methods was used. Results Twenty-seven studies were included, and systematic data extraction and quality assessment were performed. Reaction time results were transformed in standardised mean difference and graphically pooled in forest plots depending on the targeted cortical area and the type of stimulation. Conclusions Despite methodological heterogeneity among studies, results support a functional implication of five cortical regions (dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, posterior parietal cortex, supplementary motor area, dorsal premotor cortex, and primary motor cortex), integrated into a frontoparietal network, in various components of motor preparation ranging from attentional to motor aspects.
Collapse
|
117
|
Modulation of Corticospinal Excitability of Trunk Muscles in Preparation of Rapid Arm Movement. Neuroscience 2018; 369:231-241. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.11.024] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/14/2017] [Revised: 11/14/2017] [Accepted: 11/15/2017] [Indexed: 12/22/2022]
|
118
|
Abstract
Limb apraxia is a heterogeneous disorder of skilled action and tool use that has long perplexed clinicians and researchers. It occurs after damage to various loci in a densely interconnected network of regions in the left temporal, parietal, and frontal lobes. Historically, a highly classificatory approach to the study of apraxia documented numerous patterns of performance related to two major apraxia subtypes: ideational and ideomotor apraxia. More recently, there have been advances in our understanding of the functional neuroanatomy and connectivity of the left-hemisphere "tool use network," and the patterns of performance that emerge from lesions to different loci within this network. This chapter focuses on the left inferior parietal lobe, and its role in tool and body representation, action prediction, and action selection, and how these functions relate to the deficits seen in patients with apraxia subsequent to parietal lesions. Finally, suggestions are offered for several future directions that will benefit the study of apraxia, including increased attention to research on rehabilitation of this disabling disorder.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Laurel J Buxbaum
- Moss Rehabilitation Research Institute, Elkins Park, PA, United States.
| | - Jennifer Randerath
- Motor Cognition Group, Clinical Neuropsychology and Lurija Institute for Rehabilitation and Health Sciences, University of Konstanz, Konstanz; and Schmieder Foundation for Sciences and Research, Allensbach, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
119
|
Neige C, Massé-Alarie H, Gagné M, Bouyer LJ, Mercier C. Modulation of corticospinal output in agonist and antagonist proximal arm muscles during motor preparation. PLoS One 2017; 12:e0188801. [PMID: 29186189 PMCID: PMC5706717 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0188801] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/31/2017] [Accepted: 11/13/2017] [Indexed: 12/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Previous studies have shown modulation of corticospinal output of the agonist muscle when a known-movement is prepared but withheld until a response signal appearance, reflecting motor preparation processes. However, modulation in the antagonist muscles has not been described, despite the fact that reaching movements require precise coordination between the activation of agonist and antagonist muscles. In this study, participants performed an instructed-delay reaction time (RT) task, with randomized elbow flexion and extension movements. The aim was to assess the time course modulation of corticospinal output in two antagonist muscles, by simultaneously quantified the amplitude of motor evoked potentials (MEPs) in biceps brachii and triceps brachii, and the amplitude and direction of elbow movements evoked by transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS). Depending on the prepared movement direction, a specific modulation of corticospinal output was observed, MEPs and TMS-evoked movements amplitude being relatively greater for extension compared to flexion. At the end of motor preparation, a decrease in MEPs amplitude was observed for both biceps brachii and triceps brachii, regardless of the prepared movement direction. In contrast, the probability of evoking movement in the flexion direction and the amplitude of TMS-evoked movement decreased at the end of preparation for flexion, but not for extension. Together, these results confirm the existence of inhibitory processes at the end of the motor preparation, probably to avoid a premature motor response. Moreover, they provide evidence of differences in the corticospinal control of elbow flexor and extensor muscles with patterns of modulation that are not necessarily reciprocal during motor preparation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Cécilia Neige
- Center for Interdisciplinary Research in Rehabilitation and Social Integration, Québec, QC, Canada
- Department of Rehabilitation, Laval University, Québec, QC, Canada
| | - Hugo Massé-Alarie
- Center for Interdisciplinary Research in Rehabilitation and Social Integration, Québec, QC, Canada
| | - Martin Gagné
- Center for Interdisciplinary Research in Rehabilitation and Social Integration, Québec, QC, Canada
| | - Laurent J. Bouyer
- Center for Interdisciplinary Research in Rehabilitation and Social Integration, Québec, QC, Canada
- Department of Rehabilitation, Laval University, Québec, QC, Canada
| | - Catherine Mercier
- Center for Interdisciplinary Research in Rehabilitation and Social Integration, Québec, QC, Canada
- Department of Rehabilitation, Laval University, Québec, QC, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
120
|
Svoboda K, Li N. Neural mechanisms of movement planning: motor cortex and beyond. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2017; 49:33-41. [PMID: 29172091 DOI: 10.1016/j.conb.2017.10.023] [Citation(s) in RCA: 182] [Impact Index Per Article: 26.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/17/2017] [Revised: 10/22/2017] [Accepted: 10/29/2017] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
Neurons in motor cortex and connected brain regions fire in anticipation of specific movements, long before movement occurs. This neural activity reflects internal processes by which the brain plans and executes volitional movements. The study of motor planning offers an opportunity to understand how the structure and dynamics of neural circuits support persistent internal states and how these states influence behavior. Recent advances in large-scale neural recordings are beginning to decipher the relationship of the dynamics of populations of neurons during motor planning and movements. New behavioral tasks in rodents, together with quantified perturbations, link dynamics in specific nodes of neural circuits to behavior. These studies reveal a neural network distributed across multiple brain regions that collectively supports motor planning. We review recent advances and highlight areas where further work is needed to achieve a deeper understanding of the mechanisms underlying motor planning and related cognitive processes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Karel Svoboda
- Janelia Research Campus, HHMI, 19700 Helix Drive, Ashburn, VA 20147, United States.
| | - Nuo Li
- Department of Neuroscience, Baylor College of Medicine, One Baylor Plaza, Houston, TX 77030, United States.
| |
Collapse
|
121
|
Skurvydas A, Juodzbaliene V, Darbutas T, Brazaitis M. One year after ischemic stroke: Changes in leg movement path stability in a speed-accuracy task but no major effects on the hands. Hum Mov Sci 2017; 57:50-58. [PMID: 29149655 DOI: 10.1016/j.humov.2017.11.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/25/2017] [Revised: 11/06/2017] [Accepted: 11/07/2017] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
First year after the stroke is essential for motor recovery. The main motor control strategy (i.e., faster movement production at the expense of lower movement accuracy and stability, or greater movement accuracy and stability at the expense of slower movement) selected by poststroke patients during a unilateral speed-accuracy task (SAT) remains unclear. We aimed to investigate the poststroke (12 months after stroke) effects on the trade-off between movement speed and accuracy, and intraindividual variability during a motor performance task. Healthy right-handed men (n = 20; age ∼ 66 years) and right-handed men after ischemic stroke during their post rehabilitation period (n = 20; age ∼ 69 years) were asked to perform a simple reaction task, a maximal velocity performance task and a SAT with the right and left hand, and with the right and left leg. In the hand movement trial, reaction time and movement velocity (Vmax) in the SAT were slower and time to Vmax in the SAT was longer in the poststroke group (P < .01). In the leg movement trial, poststroke participants reached a greater Vmax in the SAT than the healthy participants (P < .01). The greatest poststroke effect on intraindividual variability in movements was found for movement path in the SAT, which was significantly greater in the legs than in the hands. Poststroke patients in the first year after stroke mainly selected an impulsive strategy for speed over hand and leg motor control, but at the expense of lower movement accuracy and greater variability in movement.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Albertas Skurvydas
- Institute of Sports Science and Innovation, Lithuanian Sports University, Sporto St. 6, LT-44221 Kaunas, Lithuania; Department of Applied Biology and Rehabilitation, Lithuanian Sports University, Sporto Street 6, LT-44221 Kaunas, Lithuania
| | - Vilma Juodzbaliene
- Department of Applied Biology and Rehabilitation, Lithuanian Sports University, Sporto Street 6, LT-44221 Kaunas, Lithuania
| | - Tomas Darbutas
- Department of Applied Biology and Rehabilitation, Lithuanian Sports University, Sporto Street 6, LT-44221 Kaunas, Lithuania
| | - Marius Brazaitis
- Institute of Sports Science and Innovation, Lithuanian Sports University, Sporto St. 6, LT-44221 Kaunas, Lithuania; Department of Applied Biology and Rehabilitation, Lithuanian Sports University, Sporto Street 6, LT-44221 Kaunas, Lithuania.
| |
Collapse
|
122
|
Carvalho WA, Bahia CP, Teixeira JC, Gomes-Leal W, Pereira A. Interlimb Dynamic after Unilateral Focal Lesion of the Cervical Dorsal Corticospinal Tract with Endothelin-1. Front Neuroanat 2017; 11:89. [PMID: 29081738 PMCID: PMC5645515 DOI: 10.3389/fnana.2017.00089] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/07/2017] [Accepted: 09/21/2017] [Indexed: 11/23/2022] Open
Abstract
Handedness is one of the most recognized lateralized behavior in humans. Usually, it is associated with manual superiority regarding performance proficiency. For instance, more than 90% of the human population is considered more skilled with the right hand, which is controlled by the left hemisphere, than with the left. However, during the performance of bimanual tasks, the two hands usually assume asymmetric roles, with one hand acting on objects while the other provides support, stabilizing the object. Traditionally, the role of the two hands is viewed as fixed. However, several studies support an alternate view with flexible assignments for the two hands depending on the task. The supporting role of the hand depends on a closed loop pathway based on proprioceptive inputs from the periphery. The circuit’s efferent arm courses through the dorsal corticospinal tract (dCST) in rodents and terminate on spinal cord interneurons which modulate the excitability of motoneurons in the ventral horn. In the present work, we developed an experimental model of unilateral lesion targeting the cervical dCST with microinjections of the vasoconstrictor endothelin-1 (ET-1) to evaluate the degree of flexibility of forelimb assignment during a food manipulation task. Our results show that just 3 days after unilateral corticospinal tract (CST) injury in the cervical region, rats display severe motor impairment of the ipsilateral forepaw together with a remarkable reversal of motor assignment between the forelimbs.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Walther A Carvalho
- Pará State University Center, Belém, Brazil.,Laboratory of Neuroplasticity, Institute of Health Sciences, Federal University of Pará (UFPA), Belém, Brazil
| | - Carlomagno P Bahia
- Laboratory of Neuroplasticity, Institute of Health Sciences, Federal University of Pará (UFPA), Belém, Brazil
| | - Jéssica C Teixeira
- Laboratory of Neuroplasticity, Institute of Health Sciences, Federal University of Pará (UFPA), Belém, Brazil
| | - Walace Gomes-Leal
- Institute of Biological Sciences, Federal University of Pará (UFPA), Belém, Brazil
| | - Antonio Pereira
- Laboratory of Neuroplasticity, Institute of Health Sciences, Federal University of Pará (UFPA), Belém, Brazil.,Institute of Technology, Federal University of Pará (UFPA), Belém, Brazil
| |
Collapse
|
123
|
Marinho V, Oliveira T, Rocha K, Ribeiro J, Magalhães F, Bento T, Pinto GR, Velasques B, Ribeiro P, Di Giorgio L, Orsini M, Gupta DS, Bittencourt J, Bastos VH, Teixeira S. The dopaminergic system dynamic in the time perception: a review of the evidence. Int J Neurosci 2017; 128:262-282. [PMID: 28950734 DOI: 10.1080/00207454.2017.1385614] [Citation(s) in RCA: 32] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
Abstract
Dopaminergic system plays a key role in perception, which is an important executive function of the brain. Modulation in dopaminergic system forms an important biochemical underpinning of neural mechanisms of time perception in a very wide range, from milliseconds to seconds to longer daily rhythms. Distinct types of temporal experience are poorly understood, and the relationship between processing of different intervals by the brain has received little attention. A comprehensive understanding of interval timing functions should be sought within a wider context of temporal processing, involving genetic aspects, pharmacological models, cognitive aspects, motor control and the neurological diseases with impaired dopaminergic system. Particularly, an unexplored question is whether the role of dopamine in interval timing can be integrated with the role of dopamine in non-interval timing temporal components. In this review, we explore a wider perspective of dopaminergic system, involving genetic polymorphisms, pharmacological models, executive functions and neurological diseases on the time perception. We conclude that the dopaminergic system has great participation in impact on time perception and neurobiological basis of the executive functions and neurological diseases.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Victor Marinho
- a Brain Mapping and Plasticity Laboratory, Federal University of Piauí (UFPI) , Parnaíba , Brazil.,b Genetics and Molecular Biology Laboratory, Federal University of Piauí , Parnaíba , Brazil
| | - Thomaz Oliveira
- a Brain Mapping and Plasticity Laboratory, Federal University of Piauí (UFPI) , Parnaíba , Brazil.,b Genetics and Molecular Biology Laboratory, Federal University of Piauí , Parnaíba , Brazil
| | - Kaline Rocha
- a Brain Mapping and Plasticity Laboratory, Federal University of Piauí (UFPI) , Parnaíba , Brazil
| | - Jéssica Ribeiro
- a Brain Mapping and Plasticity Laboratory, Federal University of Piauí (UFPI) , Parnaíba , Brazil
| | - Francisco Magalhães
- a Brain Mapping and Plasticity Laboratory, Federal University of Piauí (UFPI) , Parnaíba , Brazil
| | - Thalys Bento
- a Brain Mapping and Plasticity Laboratory, Federal University of Piauí (UFPI) , Parnaíba , Brazil
| | - Giovanny R Pinto
- b Genetics and Molecular Biology Laboratory, Federal University of Piauí , Parnaíba , Brazil
| | - Bruna Velasques
- c Brain Mapping and Sensory Motor Integration Laboratory, Institute of Psychiatry of Federal University of Rio de Janeiro (IPUB/UFRJ) , Rio de Janeiro , Brazil
| | - Pedro Ribeiro
- c Brain Mapping and Sensory Motor Integration Laboratory, Institute of Psychiatry of Federal University of Rio de Janeiro (IPUB/UFRJ) , Rio de Janeiro , Brazil
| | - Luiza Di Giorgio
- c Brain Mapping and Sensory Motor Integration Laboratory, Institute of Psychiatry of Federal University of Rio de Janeiro (IPUB/UFRJ) , Rio de Janeiro , Brazil
| | - Marco Orsini
- c Brain Mapping and Sensory Motor Integration Laboratory, Institute of Psychiatry of Federal University of Rio de Janeiro (IPUB/UFRJ) , Rio de Janeiro , Brazil.,d Rehabilitation Science Program, Analysis of Human Movement Laboratory, Augusto Motta University Center (UNISUAM) , Rio de Janeiro , Brazil
| | - Daya S Gupta
- e Department of Biology , Camden County College , Blackwood , NJ , USA
| | - Juliana Bittencourt
- f Biomedical Engineering Program (COPPE), Federal University of Rio de Janeiro (UFRJ) , Rio de Janeiro , Brazil
| | - Victor Hugo Bastos
- g Brain Mapping and Functionality Laboratory, Federal University of Piauí (UFPI) , Parnaíba , Brazil
| | - Silmar Teixeira
- a Brain Mapping and Plasticity Laboratory, Federal University of Piauí (UFPI) , Parnaíba , Brazil
| |
Collapse
|
124
|
The Dorsal Frontoparietal Network: A Core System for Emulated Action. Trends Cogn Sci 2017; 21:589-599. [DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2017.05.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 117] [Impact Index Per Article: 16.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/18/2017] [Revised: 04/16/2017] [Accepted: 05/09/2017] [Indexed: 01/13/2023]
|
125
|
Zhang L, Turpin NA, Feldman AG. Threshold position control of anticipation in humans: a possible role of corticospinal influences. J Physiol 2017; 595:5359-5374. [PMID: 28560812 DOI: 10.1113/jp274309] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/24/2017] [Accepted: 05/26/2017] [Indexed: 11/08/2022] Open
Abstract
KEY POINTS Sudden unloading of preloaded wrist muscles elicits motion to a new wrist position. Such motion is prevented if subjects unload muscles using the contralateral arm (self-unloading). Corticospinal influences originated from the primary motor cortex maintain tonic influences on motoneurons of wrist muscles before sudden unloading but modify these influences prior to the onset and until the end of self-unloading. Results are interpreted based on the previous finding that intentional actions are caused by central, particularly corticospinal, shifts in the spatial thresholds at which wrist motoneurons are activated, thus predetermining the attractor point at which the neuromuscular periphery achieves mechanical balance with environment forces. By maintaining or shifting the thresholds, descending systems let body segments go to the equilibrium position in the respective unloading tasks without the pre-programming of kinematics or muscle activation patterns. The study advances the understanding of how motor actions in general, and anticipation in particular, are controlled. ABSTRACT The role of corticospinal (CS) pathways in anticipatory motor actions was evaluated using transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) of the primary motor cortex projecting to motoneurons (MNs) of wrist muscles. Preloaded wrist flexors were suddenly unloaded by the experimenter or by the subject using the other hand (self-unloading). After sudden unloading, the wrist joint involuntarily flexed to a new position. In contrast, during self-unloading the wrist remained almost motionless, implying that an anticipatory postural adjustment occurred. In the self-unloading task, anticipation was manifested by a decrease in descending facilitation of pre-activated flexor MNs starting ∼72 ms before changes in the background EMG activity. Descending facilitation of extensor MNs began to increase ∼61 ms later. Conversely, these influences remained unchanged before sudden unloading, implying the absence of anticipation. We also tested TMS responses during EMG silent periods produced by brief muscle shortening, transiently resulting in similar EMG levels before the onset and after the end of self-unloading. We found reduced descending facilitation of flexor MNs after self-unloading. To explain why the wrist excursion was minimized in self-unloading due to these changes in descending influences, we relied on previous demonstrations that descending systems pre-set the threshold positions of body segments at which muscles begin to be activated, thus predetermining the equilibrium point to which the system is attracted. Based on this notion, a more consistent explanation of the kinematic, EMG and descending patterns in the two types of unloading is proposed compared to the alternative notion of direct pre-programming of kinematic and/or EMG patterns.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lei Zhang
- Center for Interdisciplinary Research in Rehabilitation (CRIR), Institut de réadaptation Gingras-Lindsay de Montréal and Jewish Rehabilitation Hospital, Laval, Quebec, Canada.,Department of Neuroscience, University of Montréal, Quebec, Canada
| | - Nicolas A Turpin
- Center for Interdisciplinary Research in Rehabilitation (CRIR), Institut de réadaptation Gingras-Lindsay de Montréal and Jewish Rehabilitation Hospital, Laval, Quebec, Canada.,Department of Neuroscience, University of Montréal, Quebec, Canada
| | - Anatol G Feldman
- Center for Interdisciplinary Research in Rehabilitation (CRIR), Institut de réadaptation Gingras-Lindsay de Montréal and Jewish Rehabilitation Hospital, Laval, Quebec, Canada.,Department of Neuroscience, University of Montréal, Quebec, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
126
|
Abstract
The motor symptoms of Parkinson's disease are not limited to the cardinal symptoms of bradykinesia, rigidity, and resting tremor, but also include a variety of interrelated motor phenomena such as deficits in spatiotemporal planning and movement sequencing, scaling and timing of movements, and intermuscular coordination that can be clinically observed. Although many of these phenomena overlap, a review of the full breadth of the motor phenomenon can aid in the diagnosis and monitoring of disease progression.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Christopher W. Hess
- University of Florida Center for Movement Disorders & Neurorestoration, Gainesville, FL, 32607, USA
| | - Mark Hallett
- Human Motor Control Section, Medical Neurology Branch, NINDS, NIH, Bethesda, Maryland, 20892, USA
| |
Collapse
|
127
|
Wong AL, Haith AM. Motor planning flexibly optimizes performance under uncertainty about task goals. Nat Commun 2017; 8:14624. [PMID: 28256513 PMCID: PMC5337982 DOI: 10.1038/ncomms14624] [Citation(s) in RCA: 45] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/28/2016] [Accepted: 01/18/2017] [Indexed: 11/09/2022] Open
Abstract
In an environment full of potential goals, how does the brain determine which movement to execute? Existing theories posit that the motor system prepares for all potential goals by generating several motor plans in parallel. One major line of evidence for such theories is that presenting two competing goals often results in a movement intermediate between them. These intermediate movements are thought to reflect an unintentional averaging of the competing plans. However, normative theories suggest instead that intermediate movements might actually be deliberate, generated because they improve task performance over a random guessing strategy. To test this hypothesis, we vary the benefit of making an intermediate movement by changing movement speed. We find that participants generate intermediate movements only at (slower) speeds where they measurably improve performance. Our findings support the normative view that the motor system selects only a single, flexible motor plan, optimized for uncertain goals.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Aaron L Wong
- Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, Maryland 21287, USA
| | - Adrian M Haith
- Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, Maryland 21287, USA
| |
Collapse
|
128
|
Factors influencing planning of a familiar grasp to an object: what it is to pick a cup. Exp Brain Res 2017; 235:1281-1296. [PMID: 28204861 PMCID: PMC5348548 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-017-4883-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/04/2016] [Accepted: 01/13/2017] [Indexed: 10/31/2022]
Abstract
We assessed the factors influencing the planning of actions required to manipulate one of two everyday objects with matching dimensions but openings at opposite ends: a cup and a vase. We found that, for cups, measures of movement preparation to reach and grasp the object were influenced by whether the grasp was made to the functional part of the object (wide opening) and whether the action would end in a supinated as opposed to a pronated grasp. These factors interacted such that effects of hand posture were found only when a less familiar grasp was made to the non-functional part of the cup (the base). These effects were not found with the vase, which has a less familiar location for grasping. We interpret the results in terms of a parallel model of action selection, modulated by both the familiarity of the grasp to a part of the object, likely to reflect object 'affordances' and the end state comfort of the action.
Collapse
|
129
|
Orban de Xivry JJ, Legrain V, Lefèvre P. Overlap of movement planning and movement execution reduces reaction time. J Neurophysiol 2016; 117:117-122. [PMID: 27733598 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00728.2016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/08/2016] [Accepted: 10/11/2016] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Motor planning is the process of preparing the appropriate motor commands in order to achieve a goal. This process has largely been thought to occur before movement onset and traditionally has been associated with reaction time. However, in a virtual line bisection task we observed an overlap between movement planning and execution. In this task performed with a robotic manipulandum, we observed that participants (n = 30) made straight movements when the line was in front of them (near target) but often made curved movements when the same target was moved sideways (far target, which had the same orientation) in such a way that they crossed the line perpendicular to its orientation. Unexpectedly, movements to the far targets had shorter reaction times than movements to the near targets (mean difference: 32 ms, SE: 5 ms, max: 104 ms). In addition, the curvature of the movement modulated reaction time. A larger increase in movement curvature from the near to the far target was associated with a larger reduction in reaction time. These highly curved movements started with a transport phase during which accuracy demands were not taken into account. We conclude that an accuracy demand imposes a reaction time penalty if processed before movement onset. This penalty is reduced if the start of the movement consists of a transport phase and if the movement plan can be refined with respect to accuracy demands later in the movement, hence demonstrating an overlap between movement planning and execution. NEW & NOTEWORTHY In the planning of a movement, the brain has the opportunity to delay the incorporation of accuracy requirements of the motor plan in order to reduce the reaction time by up to 100 ms (average: 32 ms). Such shortening of reaction time is observed here when the first phase of the movement consists of a transport phase. This forces us to reconsider the hypothesis that motor plans are fully defined before movement onset.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jean-Jacques Orban de Xivry
- Movement Control and Neuroplasticity Research Group, Department of Kinesiology, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium; .,Institute of Neuroscience, Université catholique de Louvain, Brussels, Belgium.,Institute of Information and Communication Technologies, Electronics, and Applied Mathematics, Université catholique de Louvain, Louvain-La-Neuve, Belgium; and
| | - Valéry Legrain
- Institute of Neuroscience, Université catholique de Louvain, Brussels, Belgium.,Institute of Psychological Sciences, Université catholique de Louvain, Louvain-La-Neuve, Belgium
| | - Philippe Lefèvre
- Institute of Neuroscience, Université catholique de Louvain, Brussels, Belgium.,Institute of Information and Communication Technologies, Electronics, and Applied Mathematics, Université catholique de Louvain, Louvain-La-Neuve, Belgium; and
| |
Collapse
|
130
|
Orban de Xivry JJ, Lefèvre P. A switching cost for motor planning. J Neurophysiol 2016; 116:2857-2868. [PMID: 27655964 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00319.2016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/21/2016] [Accepted: 09/19/2016] [Indexed: 01/05/2023] Open
Abstract
Movement planning consists of choosing the intended endpoint of the movement and selecting the motor program that will bring the effector on the endpoint. It is widely accepted that movement endpoint is updated on a trial-by-trial basis with respect to the observed errors and that the motor program for a given movement follows the rules of optimal feedback control. In this article, we show clear limitations of these theories. First, participants in the current study could not tune their motor program appropriately for each individual trial. This was true even when the participants selected the width of the target that they reached toward or when they had learned the appropriate motor program previously. These data are compatible with the existence of a switching cost for motor planning, which relates to the drop in performance due to an imposed switch of motor programs. This cost of switching shares many features of costs reported in cognitive task switching experiments and, when tested in the same participants, was correlated with it. Second, we found that randomly changing the width of a target over the course of a reaching experiment prevents the motor system from updating the endpoint of movements on the basis of the performance on the previous trial if the width of the target has changed. These results provide new insights into the process of motor planning and how it relates to optimal control theory and to an action selection based on the reward consequences of the motor program rather than that based on the observed error.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jean-Jacques Orban de Xivry
- Movement Control and Neuroplasticity Research Group, Department of Kinesiology, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium; and .,Institute of Information and Communication Technologies, Electronics and Applied Mathematics and Institute of Neuroscience, Université catholique de Louvain, Louvain-La-Neuve, Belgium
| | - Philippe Lefèvre
- Institute of Information and Communication Technologies, Electronics and Applied Mathematics and Institute of Neuroscience, Université catholique de Louvain, Louvain-La-Neuve, Belgium
| |
Collapse
|
131
|
Abstract
Initiating a movement in response to a visual stimulus takes significantly longer than might be expected on the basis of neural transmission delays, but it is unclear why. In a visually guided reaching task, we forced human participants to move at lower-than-normal reaction times to test whether normal reaction times are strictly necessary for accurate movement. We found that participants were, in fact, capable of moving accurately ∼80 ms earlier than their reaction times would suggest. Reaction times thus include a seemingly unnecessary delay that accounts for approximately one-third of their duration. Close examination of participants' behavior in conventional reaction-time conditions revealed that they generated occasional, spontaneous errors in trials in which their reaction time was unusually short. The pattern of these errors could be well accounted for by a simple model in which the timing of movement initiation is independent of the timing of movement preparation. This independence provides an explanation for why reaction times are usually so sluggish: delaying the mean time of movement initiation relative to preparation reduces the risk that a movement will be initiated before it has been appropriately prepared. Our results suggest that preparation and initiation of movement are mechanistically independent and may have a distinct neural basis. The results also demonstrate that, even in strongly stimulus-driven tasks, presentation of a stimulus does not directly trigger a movement. Rather, the stimulus appears to trigger an internal decision whether to make a movement, reflecting a volitional rather than reactive mode of control.
Collapse
|
132
|
Oostwoud Wijdenes L, Ivry RB, Bays PM. Competition between movement plans increases motor variability: evidence of a shared resource for movement planning. J Neurophysiol 2016; 116:1295-303. [PMID: 27358315 PMCID: PMC5023412 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00113.2016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/08/2016] [Accepted: 06/22/2016] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Various lines of evidence indicate that multiple movements can be prepared in parallel. Here, we show that preparing more than one movement comes with a cost: a movement plan is more variable if it is prepared simultaneously with another plan. This suggests that the representations of movement plans share a common neural resource and implies that the number of alternative plans is constrained by noise. Do movement plans, like representations in working memory, share a limited pool of resources? If so, the precision with which each individual movement plan is specified should decrease as the total number of movement plans increases. To explore this, human participants made speeded reaching movements toward visual targets. We examined if preparing one movement resulted in less variability than preparing two movements. The number of planned movements was manipulated in a delayed response cueing procedure that limited planning to a single target (experiment 1) or hand (experiment 2) or required planning of movements toward two targets (or with two hands). For both experiments, initial movement direction variability was higher in the two-plan condition than in the one-plan condition, demonstrating a cost associated with planning multiple movements, consistent with the limited resource hypothesis. In experiment 3, we showed that the advantage in initial variability of preparing a single movement was present only when the trajectory could be fully specified. This indicates that the difference in variability between one and two plans reflects the specification of full motor plans, not a general preparedness to move. The precision cost related to concurrent plans represents a novel constraint on motor preparation, indicating that multiple movements cannot be planned independently, even if they involve different limbs.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Leonie Oostwoud Wijdenes
- Institute of Neurology, University College London, London, United Kingdom; Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Nijmegen, Nijmegen, The Netherlands;
| | - Richard B Ivry
- Institute of Cognitive and Brain Sciences, University of California, Berkeley, California; and
| | - Paul M Bays
- Institute of Neurology, University College London, London, United Kingdom; Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
133
|
Wong AL, Goldsmith J, Krakauer JW. A motor planning stage represents the shape of upcoming movement trajectories. J Neurophysiol 2016; 116:296-305. [PMID: 27098032 DOI: 10.1152/jn.01064.2015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/01/2015] [Accepted: 04/17/2016] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Interactions with our environment require curved movements that depend not only on the final position of the hand but also on the path used to achieve it. Current studies in motor control, however, largely focus on point-to-point movements and do not consider how movements with specific desired trajectories might arise. In this study, we examined intentionally curved reaching movements that navigate paths around obstacles. We found that the preparation of these movements incurred a large reaction-time cost. This cost could not be attributed to nonmotor task requirements (e.g., stimulus perception) and was independent of the execution difficulty (i.e., extent of curvature) of the movement. Additionally, this trajectory representation cost was not observed for point-to-point reaches but could be optionally included if the task encouraged consideration of straight trajectories. Therefore, when the path of a movement is task relevant, the shape of the desired trajectory is overtly represented as a stage of motor planning. This trajectory representation ability may help explain the vast repertoire of human motor behaviors.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Aaron L Wong
- Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, Maryland;
| | - Jeff Goldsmith
- Department of Biostatistics, Mailman School of Public Health, Columbia University, New York, New York; and
| | - John W Krakauer
- Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, Maryland; Department of Neuroscience, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, Maryland
| |
Collapse
|
134
|
Gallivan JP, Bowman NAR, Chapman CS, Wolpert DM, Flanagan JR. The sequential encoding of competing action goals involves dynamic restructuring of motor plans in working memory. J Neurophysiol 2016; 115:3113-22. [PMID: 27030738 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00951.2015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/14/2015] [Accepted: 03/24/2016] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Recent neural and behavioral findings provide support for the influential idea that in situations in which multiple action options are presented simultaneously, we prepare action plans for each competing option before deciding between and executing one of those plans. However, in natural, everyday environments, our available action options frequently change from one moment to the next, and there is often uncertainty as to whether additional options will become available before having to select a particular course of action. Here, with the use of a target-directed reaching task, we show that in this situation, the brain specifies a competing action for each new, sequentially presented potential target and that recently formed action plans can be revisited and updated so as to conform with separate, more newly developed, plans. These findings indicate that the brain forms labile motor plans for sequentially arising target options that can be flexibly restructured to accommodate new motor plans.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jason P Gallivan
- Department of Psychology, Queen's University, Kingston, Ontario, Canada; Department of Biomedical and Molecular Sciences, Queen's University, Kingston, Ontario, Canada; Centre for Neuroscience Studies, Queen's University, Kingston, Ontario, Canada;
| | | | - Craig S Chapman
- Faculty of Physical Education and Recreation, University of Alberta, Alberta, Canada; and
| | - Daniel M Wolpert
- Department of Engineering, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom
| | - J Randall Flanagan
- Department of Psychology, Queen's University, Kingston, Ontario, Canada; Centre for Neuroscience Studies, Queen's University, Kingston, Ontario, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
135
|
Abstract
Preparing actions requires the operation of several cognitive control processes that influence the state of the motor system to ensure that the appropriate behavior is ultimately selected and executed. For example, some form of competition resolution ensures that the right action is chosen among alternatives, often in the presence of conflict; at the same time, impulse control ought to be deployed to prevent premature responses. Here we review how state-changes in the human motor system during action preparation can be studied through motor-evoked potentials (MEPs) elicited by transcranial magnetic stimulation over the contralateral primary motor cortex (M1). We discuss how the physiological fingerprints afforded by MEPs have helped to decompose some of the dynamic and effector-specific influences on the motor system during action preparation. We focus on competition resolution, conflict and impulse control, as well as on the influence of higher cognitive decision–related variables. The selected examples demonstrate the usefulness of MEPs as physiological readouts for decomposing the influence of distinct, but often overlapping, control processes on the human motor system during action preparation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sven Bestmann
- Sobell Department for Motor Neuroscience and Movement Disorders, UCL Institute of Neurology, University College London, London, UK
| | - Julie Duque
- Institute of Neuroscience, Université catholique de Louvain, Brussels, Belgium
| |
Collapse
|
136
|
Taubert M, Wenzel U, Draganski B, Kiebel SJ, Ragert P, Krug J, Villringer A. Investigating Neuroanatomical Features in Top Athletes at the Single Subject Level. PLoS One 2015; 10:e0129508. [PMID: 26079870 PMCID: PMC4469455 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0129508] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/07/2015] [Accepted: 05/08/2015] [Indexed: 11/25/2022] Open
Abstract
In sport events like Olympic Games or World Championships competitive athletes keep pushing the boundaries of human performance. Compared to team sports, high achievements in many athletic disciplines depend solely on the individual's performance. Contrasting previous research looking for expertise-related differences in brain anatomy at the group level, we aim to demonstrate changes in individual top athlete's brain, which would be averaged out in a group analysis. We compared structural magnetic resonance images (MRI) of three professional track-and-field athletes to age-, gender- and education-matched control subjects. To determine brain features specific to these top athletes, we tested for significant deviations in structural grey matter density between each of the three top athletes and a carefully matched control sample. While total brain volumes were comparable between athletes and controls, we show regional grey matter differences in striatum and thalamus. The demonstrated brain anatomy patterns remained stable and were detected after 2 years with Olympic Games in between. We also found differences in the fusiform gyrus in two top long jumpers. We interpret our findings in reward-related areas as correlates of top athletes' persistency to reach top-level skill performance over years.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marco Taubert
- Max-Planck-Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Uwe Wenzel
- Institute of General Kinesiology and Athletics Training, University of Leipzig, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Bogdan Draganski
- Max-Planck-Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany
- LREN, Département des Neurosciences Cliniques, CHUV, Université de Lausanne, Lausanne, Switzerland
| | - Stefan J. Kiebel
- Max-Planck-Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany
- Department of Psychology, Neuroimaging Center, Technical University, Dresden, Germany
| | - Patrick Ragert
- Max-Planck-Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany
- Institute of General Kinesiology and Athletics Training, University of Leipzig, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Jürgen Krug
- Institute of General Kinesiology and Athletics Training, University of Leipzig, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Arno Villringer
- Max-Planck-Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
137
|
Kaufman MT, Churchland MM, Ryu SI, Shenoy KV. Vacillation, indecision and hesitation in moment-by-moment decoding of monkey motor cortex. eLife 2015; 4:e04677. [PMID: 25942352 PMCID: PMC4415122 DOI: 10.7554/elife.04677] [Citation(s) in RCA: 68] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/09/2014] [Accepted: 04/03/2015] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
When choosing actions, we can act decisively, vacillate, or suffer momentary indecision. Studying how individual decisions unfold requires moment-by-moment readouts of brain state. Here we provide such a view from dorsal premotor and primary motor cortex. Two monkeys performed a novel decision task while we recorded from many neurons simultaneously. We found that a decoder trained using 'forced choices' (one target viable) was highly reliable when applied to 'free choices'. However, during free choices internal events formed three categories. Typically, neural activity was consistent with rapid, unwavering choices. Sometimes, though, we observed presumed 'changes of mind': the neural state initially reflected one choice before changing to reflect the final choice. Finally, we observed momentary 'indecision': delay forming any clear motor plan. Further, moments of neural indecision accompanied moments of behavioral indecision. Together, these results reveal the rich and diverse set of internal events long suspected to occur during free choice.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Matthew T Kaufman
- Department of Electrical Engineering, Stanford University, Stanford, United States
| | - Mark M Churchland
- Department of Neuroscience, Columbia University Medical Center, New York, United States
| | - Stephen I Ryu
- Department of Electrical Engineering, Stanford University, Stanford, United States
| | - Krishna V Shenoy
- Department of Electrical Engineering, Stanford University, Stanford, United States
| |
Collapse
|
138
|
Hedging your bets: intermediate movements as optimal behavior in the context of an incomplete decision. PLoS Comput Biol 2015; 11:e1004171. [PMID: 25821964 PMCID: PMC4379031 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004171] [Citation(s) in RCA: 50] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/13/2014] [Accepted: 02/03/2015] [Indexed: 11/23/2022] Open
Abstract
Existing theories of movement planning suggest that it takes time to select and prepare the actions required to achieve a given goal. These theories often appeal to circumstances where planning apparently goes awry. For instance, if reaction times are forced to be very low, movement trajectories are often directed between two potential targets. These intermediate movements are generally interpreted as errors of movement planning, arising either from planning being incomplete or from parallel movement plans interfering with one another. Here we present an alternative view: that intermediate movements reflect uncertainty about movement goals. We show how intermediate movements are predicted by an optimal feedback control model that incorporates an ongoing decision about movement goals. According to this view, intermediate movements reflect an exploitation of compatibility between goals. Consequently, reducing the compatibility between goals should reduce the incidence of intermediate movements. In human subjects, we varied the compatibility between potential movement goals in two distinct ways: by varying the spatial separation between targets and by introducing a virtual barrier constraining trajectories to the target and penalizing intermediate movements. In both cases we found that decreasing goal compatibility led to a decreasing incidence of intermediate movements. Our results and theory suggest a more integrated view of decision-making and movement planning in which the primary bottleneck to generating a movement is deciding upon task goals. Determining how to move to achieve a given goal is rapid and automatic. Two critical processes need to occur before a movement can be made: identification of the goal of the movement and selection and preparation of the motor commands that will be sent to muscles to generate the movement—in other words, what movement to make, and how to make it. It has long been thought that preparing motor commands is a time-consuming process, and theories advocating this view have pointed to instances where apparently the wrong motor commands are issued if insufficient time is available to prepare them. The usual pattern of these wayward movements is that they are intermediate between two potential targets. In this article we show how such intermediate movements can alternatively be viewed as reflecting an intelligent and deliberate decision about how to move, given uncertainty about task goals. Our theory is supported by experiments that show that intermediate movements only occur in conditions where they are advantageous. The implication of our theory is that the primary bottleneck to generating a movement is deciding on exactly what to do; deciding how to do it is rapid and automatic.
Collapse
|
139
|
The uses and interpretations of the motor-evoked potential for understanding behaviour. Exp Brain Res 2015; 233:679-89. [DOI: 10.1007/s00221-014-4183-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 202] [Impact Index Per Article: 22.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/13/2014] [Accepted: 12/09/2014] [Indexed: 12/21/2022]
|
140
|
Seegelke C, Hughes CML, Schack T. Manual (a)symmetries in grasp posture planning: a short review. Front Psychol 2014; 5:1480. [PMID: 25566153 PMCID: PMC4265983 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01480] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/26/2014] [Accepted: 12/01/2014] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Many activities of daily living require that we physically interact with one or more objects. Object manipulation provides an intriguing domain in which the presence and extent of manual asymmetries can be studied on a motor planning and a motor execution level. In this literature review we present a state of the art for manual asymmetries at the level of motor planning during object manipulation. First, we introduce pioneering work on grasp posture planning. We then sketch the studies investigating the impact of future task demands during unimanual and bimanual object manipulation tasks in healthy adult populations. In sum, in contrast to motor execution, there is little evidence for hand-based performance differences in grasp posture planning. We discuss potential reasons for the lack of manual asymmetries in motor planning and outline potential avenues of future research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Christian Seegelke
- Neurocognition and Action Research Group, Faculty of Psychology and Sport Sciences, Bielefeld University Bielefeld, Germany ; Center of Excellence Cognitive Interaction Technology, Bielefeld University Bielefeld, Germany
| | - Charmayne Mary Lee Hughes
- Robotics Research Centre, School of Mechanical and Aerospace Engineering, Nanyang Technological University Singapore, Singapore
| | - Thomas Schack
- Neurocognition and Action Research Group, Faculty of Psychology and Sport Sciences, Bielefeld University Bielefeld, Germany ; Center of Excellence Cognitive Interaction Technology, Bielefeld University Bielefeld, Germany ; Research Institute for Cognition and Robotics (CoR-Lab), Bielefeld University Bielefeld, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
141
|
Buxbaum LJ, Shapiro AD, Coslett HB. Reply: apraxia: a gestural or a cognitive disorder? ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2014; 138:e334. [PMID: 25173604 DOI: 10.1093/brain/awu240] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/03/2023]
Affiliation(s)
- Laurel J Buxbaum
- 1 Moss Rehabilitation Research Institute, 50 Township Line Rd, Elkins Park, PA, 19027, USA
| | - Allison D Shapiro
- 2 Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of California, Santa Barbara, CA, 93106, USA
| | - H Branch Coslett
- 3 Department of Neurology, University of Pennsylvania School of Medicine, 3400 Spruce St., Philadelphia, PA, USA
| |
Collapse
|