151
|
Robertson RM, Cease AJ, Simpson SJ. Anoxia tolerance of the adult Australian Plague Locust (Chortoicetes terminifera). Comp Biochem Physiol A Mol Integr Physiol 2019; 229:81-92. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cbpa.2018.12.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/23/2018] [Revised: 12/07/2018] [Accepted: 12/09/2018] [Indexed: 12/17/2022]
|
152
|
Congruence and measurement invariance of self-report and informant-ratings of the Big Five dimensions. PERSONALITY AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES 2019. [DOI: 10.1016/j.paid.2018.10.036] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
|
153
|
Jacobucci R, Brandmaier AM, Kievit RA. A Practical Guide to Variable Selection in Structural Equation Models with Regularized MIMIC Models. ADVANCES IN METHODS AND PRACTICES IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE 2019; 2:55-76. [PMID: 31463424 PMCID: PMC6713564 DOI: 10.1177/2515245919826527] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
Methodological innovations have allowed researchers to consider increasingly sophisticated statistical models that are better in line with the complexities of real world behavioral data. However, despite these powerful new analytic approaches, sample sizes may not always be sufficiently large to deal with the increase in model complexity. This poses a difficult modeling scenario that entails large models with a comparably limited number of observations given the number of parameters. We here describe a particular strategy to overcoming this challenge, called regularization. Regularization, a method to penalize model complexity during estimation, has proven a viable option for estimating parameters in this small n, large p setting, but has so far mostly been used in linear regression models. Here we show how to integrate regularization within structural equation models, a popular analytic approach in psychology. We first describe the rationale behind regularization in regression contexts, and how it can be extended to regularized structural equation modeling (Jacobucci, Grimm, & McArdle, 2016). Our approach is evaluated through the use of a simulation study, showing that regularized SEM outperforms traditional SEM estimation methods in situations with a large number of predictors and small sample size. We illustrate the power of this approach in two empirical examples: modeling the neural determinants of visual short term memory, as well as identifying demographic correlates of stress, anxiety and depression. We illustrate the performance of the method and discuss practical aspects of modeling empirical data, and provide a step-by-step online tutorial.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Andreas M Brandmaier
- Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany
- Max Planck UCL Centre for Computational Psychiatry and Ageing Research Berlin, Germany / London, UK
| | - Rogier A Kievit
- Max Planck UCL Centre for Computational Psychiatry and Ageing Research Berlin, Germany / London, UK
- Medical Research Council Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, University of Cambridge, UK
| |
Collapse
|
154
|
Lino de Oliveira C. Basic antidepressant research: a brief assay on how to justify your alpha. BIONATURA 2019. [DOI: 10.21931/rb/cs/2019.02.01.3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/09/2022] Open
Abstract
Antidepressant research seems under risk of bias and poor reproducibility. Recent debates brought the use of the p values in hypothesis testing to the center of a reproducibility crisis. In basic biomedicine, the use of p values has been justified by tradition instead of reasoning. Here, a biomedical researcher commented concerns with the traditional use of the p values in basic antidepressant research and discussed the missing pieces limiting the plausible justifications to their use in the field.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Cilene Lino de Oliveira
- Department of Physiological Sciences, Biological Sciences Center, Federal University of Santa Catarina
| |
Collapse
|
155
|
Vilas MG, Santilli M, Mikulan E, Adolfi F, Martorell Caro M, Manes F, Herrera E, Sedeño L, Ibáñez A, García AM. Reading Shakespearean tropes in a foreign tongue: Age of L2 acquisition modulates neural responses to functional shifts. Neuropsychologia 2019; 124:79-86. [PMID: 30664853 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2019.01.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/03/2018] [Revised: 12/21/2018] [Accepted: 01/11/2019] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
Functional shifts (FSs) - morphosyntactically marked words evoking coherent but novel meanings - are ubiquitous in English and, specially, in Shakespearean literature. While their neural signatures have been explored in native speakers, no study has targeted foreign-language users, let alone comparing early and late bilinguals. Here, we administered a validated FS paradigm to subjects from both populations and evaluated time-frequency modulations evoked by FS and control sentences. Early bilinguals exhibited greater sensitivity towards FSs, indexed by reduced fronto-posterior theta-band oscillations across semantic- and structural-integration windows. Such oscillatory modulations may represent a key marker of age-of-acquisition effects during foreign-language wordplay processing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Martina G Vilas
- Laboratory of Experimental Psychology and Neuroscience (LPEN), Institute of Cognitive and Translational Neuroscience (INCYT), INECO Foundation, Favaloro University, Buenos Aires, Argentina; National Scientific and Technical Research Council (CONICET), Buenos Aires, Argentina
| | - Micaela Santilli
- Laboratory of Experimental Psychology and Neuroscience (LPEN), Institute of Cognitive and Translational Neuroscience (INCYT), INECO Foundation, Favaloro University, Buenos Aires, Argentina
| | - Ezequiel Mikulan
- Laboratory of Experimental Psychology and Neuroscience (LPEN), Institute of Cognitive and Translational Neuroscience (INCYT), INECO Foundation, Favaloro University, Buenos Aires, Argentina; National Scientific and Technical Research Council (CONICET), Buenos Aires, Argentina
| | - Federico Adolfi
- Laboratory of Experimental Psychology and Neuroscience (LPEN), Institute of Cognitive and Translational Neuroscience (INCYT), INECO Foundation, Favaloro University, Buenos Aires, Argentina; National Scientific and Technical Research Council (CONICET), Buenos Aires, Argentina
| | - Miguel Martorell Caro
- Laboratory of Experimental Psychology and Neuroscience (LPEN), Institute of Cognitive and Translational Neuroscience (INCYT), INECO Foundation, Favaloro University, Buenos Aires, Argentina
| | - Facundo Manes
- Laboratory of Experimental Psychology and Neuroscience (LPEN), Institute of Cognitive and Translational Neuroscience (INCYT), INECO Foundation, Favaloro University, Buenos Aires, Argentina
| | - Eduar Herrera
- Universidad ICESI, Departamento de Estudios Psicológicos, Cali, Colombia
| | - Lucas Sedeño
- Laboratory of Experimental Psychology and Neuroscience (LPEN), Institute of Cognitive and Translational Neuroscience (INCYT), INECO Foundation, Favaloro University, Buenos Aires, Argentina; National Scientific and Technical Research Council (CONICET), Buenos Aires, Argentina
| | - Agustín Ibáñez
- Laboratory of Experimental Psychology and Neuroscience (LPEN), Institute of Cognitive and Translational Neuroscience (INCYT), INECO Foundation, Favaloro University, Buenos Aires, Argentina; National Scientific and Technical Research Council (CONICET), Buenos Aires, Argentina; Universidad Autónoma del Caribe, Barranquilla, Colombia; Center for Social and Cognitive Neuroscience (CSCN), School of Psychology, Universidad Adolfo Ibáñez, Santiago de Chile, Chile; Centre of Excellence in Cognition and its Disorders, Australian Research Council (ACR), Sydney, Australia
| | - Adolfo M García
- Laboratory of Experimental Psychology and Neuroscience (LPEN), Institute of Cognitive and Translational Neuroscience (INCYT), INECO Foundation, Favaloro University, Buenos Aires, Argentina; National Scientific and Technical Research Council (CONICET), Buenos Aires, Argentina; Faculty of Education, National University of Cuyo (UNCuyo), Mendoza, Argentina.
| |
Collapse
|
156
|
Rousselet GA, Hazell G, Cooke A, Dalley JW. Promoting and supporting credibility in neuroscience. Brain Neurosci Adv 2019; 3:2398212819844167. [PMID: 32166181 PMCID: PMC7058234 DOI: 10.1177/2398212819844167] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/25/2019] [Accepted: 03/26/2019] [Indexed: 11/16/2022] Open
Affiliation(s)
- Guillaume A. Rousselet
- Institute of Neuroscience and Psychology, College of Medical, Veterinary and Life Sciences, University of Glasgow, Glasgow, UK
| | | | - Anne Cooke
- British Neuroscience Association, Bristol, UK
| | | |
Collapse
|
157
|
Polanin JR, Nuijten MB. Verifying the accuracy of statistical significance testing in Campbell Collaboration systematic reviews through the use of the R package statcheck. CAMPBELL SYSTEMATIC REVIEWS 2018; 14:1-36. [PMID: 37131381 PMCID: PMC8428001 DOI: 10.4073/csrm.2018.1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/03/2023]
|
158
|
Davidson IJ. The Ouroboros of Psychological Methodology: The Case of Effect Sizes (Mechanical Objectivity vs. Expertise). REVIEW OF GENERAL PSYCHOLOGY 2018. [DOI: 10.1037/gpr0000154] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
The reporting and interpretation of effect sizes is often promoted as a panacea for the ramifications of institutionalized statistical rituals associated with the null-hypothesis significance test. Mechanical objectivity—conflating the use of a method with the obtainment of truth—is a useful theoretical tool for understanding the possible failure of effect size reporting ( Porter, 1995 ). This article helps elucidate the ouroboros of psychological methodology. This is the cycle of improved tools to produce trustworthy knowledge, leading to their institutionalization and adoption as forms of thinking, leading to methodologists eventually admonishing researchers for relying too heavily on rituals, finally leading to the production of more new improved quantitative tools that may follow along this circular path. Despite many critiques and warnings, research psychologists’ superficial adoption of effect sizes might preclude expert interpretation much like in the null-hypothesis significance test as widely received. One solution to this situation is bottom-up: promoting a balance of mechanical objectivity and expertise in the teaching of methods and research. This would require the acceptance and encouragement of expert interpretation within psychological science.
Collapse
|
159
|
Cross ES, Riddoch KA, Pratts J, Titone S, Chaudhury B, Hortensius R. A neurocognitive investigation of the impact of socialising with a robot on empathy for pain.. [DOI: 10.1101/470534] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 07/19/2023]
Abstract
To what extent can humans form social relationships with robots? In the present study, we combined functional neuroimaging with a robot socialising intervention to probe the flexibility of empathy, a core component of social relationships, toward robots. Twenty-six individuals underwent identical fMRI sessions before and after being issued a social robot to take home and interact with over the course of a week. While undergoing fMRI, participants observed videos of a human actor or a robot experiencing pain or pleasure in response to electrical stimulation. Repetition suppression of activity in the pain network, a collection of brain regions associated with empathy and emotional responding, was measured to test whether socialising with a social robot leads to greater overlap in neural mechanisms when observing human and robotic agents experiencing pain or pleasure. In contrast to our hypothesis, functional region-of-interest analyses revealed no change in neural overlap for agents after the socialising intervention. Similarly, no increase in activation when observing a robot experiencing pain emerged post-socialising. Whole-brain analysis showed that, before the socialising intervention, superior parietal and early visual regions are sensitive to novel agents, while after socialising, medial temporal regions show agent sensitivity. A region of the inferior parietal lobule was sensitive to novel emotions, but only during the pre-socialising scan session. Together, these findings suggest that a short socialisation intervention with a social robot does not lead to discernible differences in empathy toward the robot, as measured by behavioural or brain responses. We discuss the extent to which longer term socialisation with robots might shape social cognitive processes and ultimately our relationships with these machines.
Collapse
|
160
|
Buller DB, Walkosz BJ, Buller MK, Wallis A, Andersen PA, Scott MD, Meenan RT, Cutter GR. Implementation of Occupational Sun Safety at a 2-Year Follow-Up in a Randomized Trial: Comparison of Sun Safe Workplaces Policy Intervention to Attention Control. Am J Health Promot 2018; 33:683-697. [PMID: 30477333 DOI: 10.1177/0890117118814398] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/12/2022]
Abstract
PURPOSE Implementation of employer sun safety actions was assessed in a 2-year follow-up to an occupational sun protection policy intervention. DESIGN Two-year follow-up assessment in a randomized pretest-posttest controlled design. SETTING Local government organizations with workers in public safety, public works, and parks and recreation. PARTICIPANTS Sixty-three local government organizations (participation = 64%) and 330 frontline supervisors and 1454 workers. INTERVENTION Sun Safe Workplaces (SSW) intervention promoting occupational sun safety policy and education. MEASURES Observations of SSW messages and sun safety items and surveys on organizations' communication and actions on sun safety. ANALYSIS Comparison between SSW and control groups was conducted using regression models and adjusted for clustering where appropriate, with α criterion set at P = .05 (2-tailed). RESULTS At intervention worksites, more SSW messages ( P < .001) and sun safety items ( P = .025) were observed; more frontline supervisors reported organizations provided free/reduced price sunscreen ( P = .005) and communicated about sun safety ( P < .001); and more workers recalled receiving sun safety messages ( P < .001) and sun safety training ( P <.001) compared to control organizations. Implementation was greater at larger than smaller intervention organizations for wide-brimmed hats ( P = .009), long work pants ( P = .017), and shade structures ( P = .036). Older workers received the most written messages ( P = .015). CONCLUSIONS Sun Safe Workplaces appeared to produce actions by organizations to support employee sun safety. Large organizations may have processes, communication channels, and slack resources to achieve more implementation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | | | - Allan Wallis
- 2 School of Public Affairs, University of Colorado Denver, Denver, CO, USA
| | - Peter A Andersen
- 3 School of Communication, San Diego State University, San Diego, CA, USA
| | | | - Richard T Meenan
- 5 Kaiser Permanente, Center for Health Research, Portland, OR, USA
| | - Gary R Cutter
- 6 Department of Biostatistics, School of Public Health, University of Alabama at Birmingham, Birmingham, AL, USA
| |
Collapse
|
161
|
Should I test more babies? Solutions for transparent data peeking. Infant Behav Dev 2018; 54:166-176. [PMID: 30470414 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2018.09.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/03/2018] [Revised: 08/03/2018] [Accepted: 09/28/2018] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
Abstract
Research with infants is often slow and time-consuming, so infant researchers face great pressure to use the available participants in an efficient way. One strategy that researchers sometimes use to optimize efficiency is data peeking (or "optional stopping"), that is, doing a preliminary analysis (whether a formal significance test or informal eyeballing) of collected data. Data peeking helps researchers decide whether to abandon or tweak a study, decide that a sample is complete, or decide to continue adding data points. Unfortunately, data peeking can have negative consequences such as increased rates of false positives (wrongly concluding that an effect is present when it is not). We argue that, with simple corrections, the benefits of data peeking can be harnessed to use participants more efficiently. We review two corrections that can be transparently reported: one can be applied at the beginning of a study to lay out a plan for data peeking, and a second can be applied after data collection has already started. These corrections are easy to implement in the current framework of infancy research. The use of these corrections, together with transparent reporting, can increase the replicability of infant research.
Collapse
|
162
|
Abstract
AbstractThe replication crisis has prompted many to call for statistical reform within the psychological sciences. Here we examine issues within Frequentist statistics that may have led to the replication crisis, and we examine the alternative—Bayesian statistics—that many have suggested as a replacement. The Frequentist approach and the Bayesian approach offer radically different perspectives on evidence and inference with the Frequentist approach prioritising error control and the Bayesian approach offering a formal method for quantifying the relative strength of evidence for hypotheses. We suggest that rather than mere statistical reform, what is needed is a better understanding of the different modes of statistical inference and a better understanding of how statistical inference relates to scientific inference.
Collapse
|
163
|
Spence JR, Stanley DJ. Concise, Simple, and Not Wrong: In Search of a Short-Hand Interpretation of Statistical Significance. Front Psychol 2018; 9:2185. [PMID: 30483192 PMCID: PMC6243056 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02185] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/14/2018] [Accepted: 10/23/2018] [Indexed: 11/20/2022] Open
Abstract
One challenge when communicating science to practitioners and the general public is accurately representing statistical results. In particular, describing the meaning of statistical significance to a non-scientific audience is especially difficult given the technical nature of a correct definition. Correct interpretations of statistical significance can be unintuitive, nuanced, and use unfamiliar technical language. As a result, when researchers are tasked with providing short and understandable interpretations of statistical significance it can be tempting to default to convenient but incorrect interpretations. In the current paper, we offer a concise, simple, and correct interpretation of statistical significance that is suitable for communications targeting a general audience.
Collapse
|
164
|
Davis WE, Giner-Sorolla R, Lindsay DS, Lougheed JP, Makel MC, Meier ME, Sun J, Vaughn LA, Zelenski JM. Peer-Review Guidelines Promoting Replicability and Transparency in Psychological Science. ADVANCES IN METHODS AND PRACTICES IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE 2018. [DOI: 10.1177/2515245918806489] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/27/2022]
Abstract
More and more psychological researchers have come to appreciate the perils of common but poorly justified research practices and are rethinking commonly held standards for evaluating research. As this methodological reform expresses itself in psychological research, peer reviewers of such work must also adapt their practices to remain relevant. Reviewers of journal submissions wield considerable power to promote methodological reform, and thereby contribute to the advancement of a more robust psychological literature. We describe concrete practices that reviewers can use to encourage transparency, intellectual humility, and more valid assessments of the methods and statistics reported in articles.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | | | | | | | | | - Jessie Sun
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Davis
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
165
|
Loesche F, Goslin J, Bugmann G. Paving the Way to Eureka-Introducing "Dira" as an Experimental Paradigm to Observe the Process of Creative Problem Solving. Front Psychol 2018; 9:1773. [PMID: 30333767 PMCID: PMC6176089 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01773] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/30/2018] [Accepted: 09/03/2018] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
"Dira" is a novel experimental paradigm to record combinations of behavioral and metacognitive measures for the creative process. This task allows assessing chronological and chronometric aspects of the creative process directly and without a detour through creative products or proxy phenomena. In a study with 124 participants we show that (a) people spend more time attending to selected vs. rejected potential solutions, (b) there is a clear connection between behavioral patterns and self-reported measures, (c) the reported intensity of Eureka experiences is a function of interaction time with potential solutions, and (d) experiences of emerging solutions can happen immediately after engaging with a problem, before participants explore all potential solutions. The conducted study exemplifies how "Dira" can be used as an instrument to narrow down the moment when solutions emerge. We conclude that the "Dira" experiment is paving the way to study the process, as opposed to the product, of creative problem solving.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Frank Loesche
- CogNovo, Cognition Institute, Plymouth University, Plymouth, United Kingdom
- School of Computing, Electronics and Mathematics, Plymouth University, Plymouth, United Kingdom
| | - Jeremy Goslin
- School of Psychology, Plymouth University, Plymouth, United Kingdom
| | - Guido Bugmann
- School of Computing, Electronics and Mathematics, Plymouth University, Plymouth, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
166
|
Vijayakumar R, Cheung MWL. Replicability of Machine Learning Models in the Social Sciences. ZEITSCHRIFT FUR PSYCHOLOGIE-JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY 2018. [DOI: 10.1027/2151-2604/a000344] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
Abstract
Abstract. Machine learning tools are increasingly used in social sciences and policy fields due to their increase in predictive accuracy. However, little research has been done on how well the models of machine learning methods replicate across samples. We compare machine learning methods with regression on the replicability of variable selection, along with predictive accuracy, using an empirical dataset as well as simulated data with additive, interaction, and non-linear squared terms added as predictors. Methods analyzed include support vector machines (SVM), random forests (RF), multivariate adaptive regression splines (MARS), and the regularized regression variants, least absolute shrinkage and selection operator (LASSO), and elastic net. In simulations with additive and linear interactions, machine learning methods performed similarly to regression in replicating predictors; they also performed mostly equal or below regression on measures of predictive accuracy. In simulations with square terms, machine learning methods SVM, RF, and MARS improved predictive accuracy and replicated predictors better than regression. Thus, in simulated datasets, the gap between machine learning methods and regression on predictive measures foreshadowed the gap in variable selection. In replications on the empirical dataset, however, improved prediction by machine learning methods was not accompanied by a visible improvement in replicability in variable selection. This disparity is explained by the overall explanatory power of the models. When predictors have small effects and noise predominates, improved global measures of prediction in a sample by machine learning methods may not lead to the robust selection of predictors; thus, in the presence of weak predictors and noise, regression remains a useful tool for model building and replication.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Mike W.-L. Cheung
- Department of Psychology, National University of Singapore, Singapore
| |
Collapse
|
167
|
|
168
|
Bradford DE, Fronk GE, Sant’Ana SJ, Magruder KP, Kaye JT, Curtin JJ. The need for precise answers for the goals of precision medicine in alcohol dependence to succeed. Neuropsychopharmacology 2018; 43:1799-1800. [PMID: 29930386 PMCID: PMC6046054 DOI: 10.1038/s41386-018-0112-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/15/2018] [Accepted: 05/18/2018] [Indexed: 01/20/2023]
Affiliation(s)
- Daniel E. Bradford
- 0000 0001 2167 3675grid.14003.36Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin–Madison, 1202W. Johnson St., Madison, WI 53706 USA
| | - Gaylen E. Fronk
- 0000 0001 2167 3675grid.14003.36Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin–Madison, 1202W. Johnson St., Madison, WI 53706 USA
| | - Sarah J. Sant’Ana
- 0000 0001 2167 3675grid.14003.36Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin–Madison, 1202W. Johnson St., Madison, WI 53706 USA
| | - Katherine P. Magruder
- 0000 0001 2167 3675grid.14003.36Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin–Madison, 1202W. Johnson St., Madison, WI 53706 USA
| | - Jesse T. Kaye
- 0000 0001 2167 3675grid.14003.36Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin–Madison, 1202W. Johnson St., Madison, WI 53706 USA
| | - John J. Curtin
- 0000 0001 2167 3675grid.14003.36Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin–Madison, 1202W. Johnson St., Madison, WI 53706 USA
| |
Collapse
|
169
|
A Practical Primer To Power Analysis for Simple Experimental Designs. INTERNATIONAL REVIEW OF SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 2018. [DOI: 10.5334/irsp.181] [Citation(s) in RCA: 144] [Impact Index Per Article: 20.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/20/2022] Open
|
170
|
Lakens D, Scheel AM, Isager PM. Equivalence Testing for Psychological Research: A Tutorial. ADVANCES IN METHODS AND PRACTICES IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE 2018. [DOI: 10.1177/2515245918770963] [Citation(s) in RCA: 502] [Impact Index Per Article: 71.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
Psychologists must be able to test both for the presence of an effect and for the absence of an effect. In addition to testing against zero, researchers can use the two one-sided tests (TOST) procedure to test for equivalence and reject the presence of a smallest effect size of interest (SESOI). The TOST procedure can be used to determine if an observed effect is surprisingly small, given that a true effect at least as extreme as the SESOI exists. We explain a range of approaches to determine the SESOI in psychological science and provide detailed examples of how equivalence tests should be performed and reported. Equivalence tests are an important extension of the statistical tools psychologists currently use and enable researchers to falsify predictions about the presence, and declare the absence, of meaningful effects.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Daniël Lakens
- Human-Technology Interaction Group, Eindhoven University of Technology
| | - Anne M. Scheel
- Human-Technology Interaction Group, Eindhoven University of Technology
| | - Peder M. Isager
- Human-Technology Interaction Group, Eindhoven University of Technology
| |
Collapse
|
171
|
Trafimow D, Amrhein V, Areshenkoff CN, Barrera-Causil CJ, Beh EJ, Bilgiç YK, Bono R, Bradley MT, Briggs WM, Cepeda-Freyre HA, Chaigneau SE, Ciocca DR, Correa JC, Cousineau D, de Boer MR, Dhar SS, Dolgov I, Gómez-Benito J, Grendar M, Grice JW, Guerrero-Gimenez ME, Gutiérrez A, Huedo-Medina TB, Jaffe K, Janyan A, Karimnezhad A, Korner-Nievergelt F, Kosugi K, Lachmair M, Ledesma RD, Limongi R, Liuzza MT, Lombardo R, Marks MJ, Meinlschmidt G, Nalborczyk L, Nguyen HT, Ospina R, Perezgonzalez JD, Pfister R, Rahona JJ, Rodríguez-Medina DA, Romão X, Ruiz-Fernández S, Suarez I, Tegethoff M, Tejo M, van de Schoot R, Vankov II, Velasco-Forero S, Wang T, Yamada Y, Zoppino FCM, Marmolejo-Ramos F. Manipulating the Alpha Level Cannot Cure Significance Testing. Front Psychol 2018; 9:699. [PMID: 29867666 PMCID: PMC5962803 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00699] [Citation(s) in RCA: 51] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/22/2018] [Accepted: 04/23/2018] [Indexed: 11/30/2022] Open
Abstract
We argue that making accept/reject decisions on scientific hypotheses, including a recent call for changing the canonical alpha level from p = 0.05 to p = 0.005, is deleterious for the finding of new discoveries and the progress of science. Given that blanket and variable alpha levels both are problematic, it is sensible to dispense with significance testing altogether. There are alternatives that address study design and sample size much more directly than significance testing does; but none of the statistical tools should be taken as the new magic method giving clear-cut mechanical answers. Inference should not be based on single studies at all, but on cumulative evidence from multiple independent studies. When evaluating the strength of the evidence, we should consider, for example, auxiliary assumptions, the strength of the experimental design, and implications for applications. To boil all this down to a binary decision based on a p-value threshold of 0.05, 0.01, 0.005, or anything else, is not acceptable.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- David Trafimow
- Department of Psychology, New Mexico State University, Las Cruces, NM, United States
| | - Valentin Amrhein
- Zoological Institute, University of Basel, Basel, Switzerland.,Swiss Ornithological Institute, Sempach, Switzerland
| | | | - Carlos J Barrera-Causil
- Faculty of Applied and Exact Sciences, Metropolitan Technological Institute, Medellín, Colombia
| | - Eric J Beh
- School of Mathematical and Physical Sciences, University of Newcastle, Callaghan, NSW, Australia
| | - Yusuf K Bilgiç
- Department of Mathematics, State University of New York at Geneseo, Geneseo, NY, United States
| | - Roser Bono
- Quantitative Psychology Unit, Faculty of Psychology, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain.,Institut de Neurociències, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
| | - Michael T Bradley
- Department of Psychology, Faculty of Arts, University of New Brunswick, Saint John, NB, Canada
| | | | | | - Sergio E Chaigneau
- Center for Social and Cognitive Neuroscience, School of Psychology, Universidad Adolfo Ibáñez, Santiago, Chile
| | - Daniel R Ciocca
- Oncology Laboratory, Instituto de Medicina y Biologia Experimental de Cuyo, CCT CONICET Mendoza, Mendoza, Argentina
| | - Juan C Correa
- School of Statistics, Faculty of Sciences, National University of Colombia, Medellín, Colombia
| | - Denis Cousineau
- School of Psychology, University of Ottawa, Ottawa, ON, Canada
| | - Michiel R de Boer
- Department of Health Sciences, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam and Amsterdam Public Health Research Institute, Amsterdam, Netherlands
| | - Subhra S Dhar
- Department of Mathematics and Statistics, Indian Institute of Technology, Kanpur, India
| | - Igor Dolgov
- Department of Psychology, New Mexico State University, Las Cruces, NM, United States
| | - Juana Gómez-Benito
- Quantitative Psychology Unit, Faculty of Psychology, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain.,Institut de Neurociències, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
| | - Marian Grendar
- Biomedical Center Martin, Jessenius Faculty of Medicine, Comenius University, Martin, Slovakia.,Institute of Measurement Science, Slovak Academy of Sciences, Bratislava, Slovakia
| | - James W Grice
- Department of Psychology, Oklahoma State University, Stillwater, OK, United States
| | - Martin E Guerrero-Gimenez
- Oncology Laboratory, Instituto de Medicina y Biologia Experimental de Cuyo, CCT CONICET Mendoza, Mendoza, Argentina
| | - Andrés Gutiérrez
- Faculty of Statistics, Saint Thomas University, Bogotá, Colombia
| | - Tania B Huedo-Medina
- Department of Allied Health Sciences, College of Health, Agriculture, and Natural Resources, University of Connecticut, Storrs, CT, United States
| | - Klaus Jaffe
- Departamento de Biología de Organismos, Universidad Simón Bolívar, Caracas, Venezuela
| | - Armina Janyan
- Department of Cognitive Science and Psychology, New Bulgarian University, Sofia, Bulgaria.,National Research Tomsk State University, Tomsk, Russia
| | - Ali Karimnezhad
- Department of Biochemistry, Microbiology, and Immunology, University of Ottawa, Ottawa, ON, Canada
| | | | - Koji Kosugi
- School of Human Sciences, Senshu University, Kawasaki, Japan
| | - Martin Lachmair
- Multimodal Interaction Lab, Leibniz-Institut für Wissensmedien, Tübingen, Germany
| | - Rubén D Ledesma
- Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas, Mar del Plata, Argentina.,Facultad de Psicología, Universidad Nacional de Mar del Plata, Mar del Plata, Argentina
| | - Roberto Limongi
- Pontificia Universidad Católica de Valparaíso, Valparaíso, Chile.,Vicerrectoría de Investigación y Desarrollo, Universidad Tecnológica de Chile INACAP, Santiago, Chile
| | - Marco T Liuzza
- Department of Medical and Surgical Sciences, "Magna Graecia" University of Catanzaro, Catanzaro, Italy
| | - Rosaria Lombardo
- Economics Department, University of Campania "Luigi Vanvitelli", Capua, Italy
| | - Michael J Marks
- Department of Psychology, New Mexico State University, Las Cruces, NM, United States
| | - Gunther Meinlschmidt
- Department of Psychosomatic Medicine, University Hospital Basel and University of Basel, Basel, Switzerland.,Division of Clinical Psychology and Cognitive Behavioral Therapy, International Psychoanalytic University, Berlin, Germany.,Division of Clinical Psychology and Epidemiology, Department of Psychology, University of Basel, Basel, Switzerland
| | - Ladislas Nalborczyk
- Université Grenoble Alpes, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, LPNC, Grenoble, France.,Department of Experimental Clinical and Health Psychology, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium
| | - Hung T Nguyen
- Department of Mathematical Sciences, New Mexico State University, Las Cruces, NM, United States
| | - Raydonal Ospina
- Computational Statistics Laboratory (CAST), Department of Statistics, Universidade Federal de Pernambuco, Recife, Brazil
| | | | - Roland Pfister
- Department of Psychology III, University of Würzburg, Würzburg, Germany
| | - Juan J Rahona
- Multimodal Interaction Lab, Leibniz-Institut für Wissensmedien, Tübingen, Germany
| | | | - Xavier Romão
- CONSTRUCT-LESE, Faculty of Engineering, University of Porto, Porto, Portugal
| | - Susana Ruiz-Fernández
- Multimodal Interaction Lab, Leibniz-Institut für Wissensmedien, Tübingen, Germany.,FOM Hochschule für Oekonomie und Management, Essen, Germany.,LEAD Graduate School & Research Network, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
| | - Isabel Suarez
- Department of Psychology, Universidad del Norte, Barranquilla, Colombia
| | - Marion Tegethoff
- Division of Clinical Psychology and Psychiatry, Department of Psychology, University of Basel, Basel, Switzerland
| | - Mauricio Tejo
- Facultad de Ciencias Naturales y Exactas, Universidad de Playa Ancha, Valparaíso, Chile
| | - Rens van de Schoot
- Department of Methods and Statistics, Faculty of Social and Behavioural Sciences, Utrecht University, Utrecht, Netherlands.,North-West University, Optentia Research Focus Area, Vanderbijlpark, South Africa
| | - Ivan I Vankov
- Department of Cognitive Science and Psychology, New Bulgarian University, Sofia, Bulgaria
| | | | - Tonghui Wang
- Department of Mathematical Sciences, New Mexico State University, Las Cruces, NM, United States
| | - Yuki Yamada
- Faculty of Arts and Science, Kyushu University, Fukuoka, Japan
| | - Felipe C M Zoppino
- Oncology Laboratory, Instituto de Medicina y Biologia Experimental de Cuyo, CCT CONICET Mendoza, Mendoza, Argentina
| | | |
Collapse
|
172
|
Cristea IA, Ioannidis JPA. P values in display items are ubiquitous and almost invariably significant: A survey of top science journals. PLoS One 2018; 13:e0197440. [PMID: 29763472 PMCID: PMC5953482 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0197440] [Citation(s) in RCA: 28] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/07/2018] [Accepted: 05/02/2018] [Indexed: 12/18/2022] Open
Abstract
P values represent a widely used, but pervasively misunderstood and fiercely contested method of scientific inference. Display items, such as figures and tables, often containing the main results, are an important source of P values. We conducted a survey comparing the overall use of P values and the occurrence of significant P values in display items of a sample of articles in the three top multidisciplinary journals (Nature, Science, PNAS) in 2017 and, respectively, in 1997. We also examined the reporting of multiplicity corrections and its potential influence on the proportion of statistically significant P values. Our findings demonstrated substantial and growing reliance on P values in display items, with increases of 2.5 to 14.5 times in 2017 compared to 1997. The overwhelming majority of P values (94%, 95% confidence interval [CI] 92% to 96%) were statistically significant. Methods to adjust for multiplicity were almost non-existent in 1997, but reported in many articles relying on P values in 2017 (Nature 68%, Science 48%, PNAS 38%). In their absence, almost all reported P values were statistically significant (98%, 95% CI 96% to 99%). Conversely, when any multiplicity corrections were described, 88% (95% CI 82% to 93%) of reported P values were statistically significant. Use of Bayesian methods was scant (2.5%) and rarely (0.7%) articles relied exclusively on Bayesian statistics. Overall, wider appreciation of the need for multiplicity corrections is a welcome evolution, but the rapid growth of reliance on P values and implausibly high rates of reported statistical significance are worrisome.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ioana Alina Cristea
- Meta-Research Innovation Center at Stanford (METRICS), Stanford University, Stanford, California, United States of America
- Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, Babes-Bolyai University, Cluj-Napoca Romania
| | - John P. A. Ioannidis
- Meta-Research Innovation Center at Stanford (METRICS), Stanford University, Stanford, California, United States of America
- Departments of Medicine, Stanford University, Stanford, California, United States of America
- Department of Health Research and Policy, Stanford University, Stanford, California, United States of America
- Department of Biomedical Data Science, Stanford University, Stanford, California, United States of America
- Department of Statistics, Stanford University, Stanford, California, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
173
|
Krefeld-Schwalb A, Witte EH, Zenker F. Hypothesis-Testing Demands Trustworthy Data-A Simulation Approach to Inferential Statistics Advocating the Research Program Strategy. Front Psychol 2018; 9:460. [PMID: 29740363 PMCID: PMC5928294 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00460] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/18/2017] [Accepted: 03/19/2018] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
In psychology as elsewhere, the main statistical inference strategy to establish empirical effects is null-hypothesis significance testing (NHST). The recent failure to replicate allegedly well-established NHST-results, however, implies that such results lack sufficient statistical power, and thus feature unacceptably high error-rates. Using data-simulation to estimate the error-rates of NHST-results, we advocate the research program strategy (RPS) as a superior methodology. RPS integrates Frequentist with Bayesian inference elements, and leads from a preliminary discovery against a (random) H0-hypothesis to a statistical H1-verification. Not only do RPS-results feature significantly lower error-rates than NHST-results, RPS also addresses key-deficits of a “pure” Frequentist and a standard Bayesian approach. In particular, RPS aggregates underpowered results safely. RPS therefore provides a tool to regain the trust the discipline had lost during the ongoing replicability-crisis.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Erich H Witte
- Institute for Psychology, University of Hamburg, Hamburg, Germany
| | - Frank Zenker
- Department of Philosophy, Lund University, Lund, Sweden
| |
Collapse
|
174
|
The influence of sensorimotor experience on the aesthetic evaluation of dance across the life span. PROGRESS IN BRAIN RESEARCH 2018; 237:291-316. [DOI: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2018.03.012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/30/2022]
|