201
|
Roebers CM, Gelhaar T, Schneider W. “It’s magic!” The effects of presentation modality on children’s event memory, suggestibility, and confidence judgments. J Exp Child Psychol 2004; 87:320-35. [PMID: 15050457 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2004.01.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/04/2003] [Revised: 01/26/2004] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
The current study investigated the influence of presentation modality (live, video, and slide show) on children's memory, suggestibility, recognition, and metamemorial monitoring processes. A total of 270 children in three age groups (5- and 6-year-olds, 7- and 8-year-olds, and 9- and 10-year-olds) watched a magic show and were questioned about it 1 week later. The live show yielded more correct answers to nonleading questions, higher resistance to misleading questions, and better recognition memory than did the video condition, which in turn resulted in better performance than did the slide show. Although presentation modality raised the general level of memory performance, the effects were equally strong in all age groups and did not affect memory phenomena such as the size of the misinformation effect and confidence judgments.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Claudia M Roebers
- Department of Psychology, University of Würzburg, 97070 Würzburg, Germany.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
202
|
Courage ML, Howe ML. Advances in early memory development research: Insights about the dark side of the moon. DEVELOPMENTAL REVIEW 2004. [DOI: 10.1016/j.dr.2003.09.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
|
203
|
Ghetti S, Alexander KW. "If It Happened, I Would Remember It": Strategic Use of Event Memorability in the Rejection of False Autobiographical Events. Child Dev 2004; 75:542-61. [PMID: 15056205 DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00692.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 56] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
The present research investigated the link between perceived event memorability and false-event rejection. In 2 studies, event salience, plausibility, and recency were manipulated. Study 1 showed that high-salience events elicited higher memorability ratings than low-salience events for 5-, 7-, 9-year-olds and adults. Plausibility and recency affected only 9-year-olds' and adults' judgments. Study 2 demonstrated that younger versus older children and adults were less likely to reject false events, and that older children and adults were more likely to reject false events based on salience than were younger children. High-recency false events were more likely to be rejected than low-recency false events. Consistent with prediction, recency moderated the effect of salience. The development of metamemorial awareness and rejection strategies is discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Simona Ghetti
- Research Institute on Judicial Systems, National Research Council, Bologna, Italy.
| | | |
Collapse
|
204
|
Clarke-Stewart KA, Malloy LC, Allhusen VD. Verbal ability, self-control, and close relationships with parents protect children against misleading suggestions. APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2004. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.1076] [Citation(s) in RCA: 41] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/05/2022]
|
205
|
Walsh C, Jamieson E, MacMillan H, Trocmé N. Measuring child sexual abuse in children and youth. JOURNAL OF CHILD SEXUAL ABUSE 2004; 13:39-68. [PMID: 15353376 DOI: 10.1300/j070v13n01_03] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/24/2023]
Abstract
Asking children and adolescents directly about their experience of sexual victimization overcomes some of the methodological weaknesses inherent in other approaches. Yet complex legal, ethical, and methodological issues remain. This paper reviews the psychometric properties of those questions or instruments that have measured exposure to child sexual abuse directly. A search of four electronic databases using descriptors "child sexual abuse" and "measurement" or "instrumentation" yielded four telephone administered tools, 13 face-to-face interviews, and 32 self-administered questionnaires. Few instruments had been subjected to rigorous evaluation. Establishing the validity and reliability of instruments measuring child sexual abuse and other forms of victimization are critical for the growth and expansion of the field.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Christine Walsh
- Faculty of Social Work, University of Toronto, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioural Neurosciences, McMaster University.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
206
|
Crossman AM, Scullin MH, Melnyk L. Individual and developmental differences in suggestibility. APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2004. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.1079] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
|
207
|
Mueller-Johnson K, Ceci SJ. Memory and suggestibility in older adults: live event participation and repeated interview. APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2004. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.1078] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/11/2022]
|
208
|
Abstract
Reductions in children's retroactive interference were examined with conceptual recoding. Children learned two 10-item lists of toys; items on the 2nd list could also be classified as vehicles. Some children were not told about this 2nd category, whereas others were told either at the end of acquisition or just prior to the retention test 24 hr later. The results showed that (a) children benefited from the recoding instruction, (b) younger but not older children failed to benefit from the recoding manipulation when it occurred just prior to the retention test, and (c) recoding reduced retroactive interference primarily through affecting storage processes. These results provide new evidence concerning the importance of making information distinctive in storage in children's retention.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mark L Howe
- Lakehead University, Department of Psychology, Thunder Bay, ON, Canada.
| |
Collapse
|
209
|
Ceci SJ. Cast in Six Ponds and You'll Reel in Something: Looking Back on 25 Years of Research. AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGIST 2003; 58:855-864. [PMID: 14609372 DOI: 10.1037/0003-066x.58.11.855] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
The author describes his program of research over the past 25 years. This research falls into 6 areas that are interdependent and that inform each other. The overall program is guided by 3 bioecological principles that posit the need for proximal processes and motivation to actualize biological potential. The author presents examples of experiments that fall into each of the 6 areas and show that human potential is highly contextualized and that, consequently, the same person who fails at a task in one domain is often able to succeed at it in a different domain.
Collapse
|
210
|
Roebers CM, Howie P. Confidence judgments in event recall: developmental progression in the impact of question format. J Exp Child Psychol 2003; 85:352-71. [PMID: 12906847 DOI: 10.1016/s0022-0965(03)00076-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 45] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
Abstract
Two studies were conducted to examine developmental progression in children's and adults' ability to adequately monitor their own attempts to recall event details as well as the dependence of such metamemorial competencies on question formats. Eight and 10-year-old children as well as adults (Study 1, N=116; Study 2, N=60) rated their confidence when responding to specific questions about an observed event. Confirming most recent results, children and adults gave higher confidence ratings after correct than after incorrect answers. This ability, however, was limited to an unbiased question format. When being asked misleading questions, children's ability to differentiate was undermined, as reflected in equally high confidence judgments after correct and incorrect answers, even when the interview contained a mix of misleading and unbiased questions. When the interviewer "bombarded" the children with an uninterrupted series of misleading questions, children's difficulties appeared to be even more pronounced. These findings highlight the importance of the way in which questions are asked, and point to age-related progression in the relative impact of questioning style.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Claudia M Roebers
- Department of Psychology IV, University of Würzburg, 97070 Würzburg, Germany.
| | | |
Collapse
|
211
|
Holliday RE. Reducing misinformation effects in children with cognitive interviews: dissociating recollection and familiarity. Child Dev 2003; 74:728-51. [PMID: 12795387 DOI: 10.1111/1467-8624.00565] [Citation(s) in RCA: 63] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
Two experiments examined the effect of a cognitive interview on 4- and 8-year-old children's correct recall and subsequent reporting of misinformation. Children viewed an event followed by misinformation that was read or self-generated either before or after a cognitive interview. Children were then given a recognition test under inclusion and exclusion instructions. A cognitive interview elicited more correct details than a control interview. Age-related changes were found such that the 8-year-old children's reports were more complete and they recalled more correct person, action, object, and location details than the 4-year-old children. A cognitive interview given after postevent misinformation reduced children's reporting of misinformation at interview and reduced reporting of self-generated misinformation at test. Process dissociation analyses revealed that recollection increased but familiarity decreased with age.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Robyn E Holliday
- Department of Psychology, Keynes College, University of Kent at Canterbury, UK.
| |
Collapse
|
212
|
Roebers CM, McConkey KM. Mental reinstatement of the misinformation context and the misinformation effect in children and adults. APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2003. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.886] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
|
213
|
Abstract
The present work investigated the role of children's and adults' metacognitive monitoring and control processes for unbiased event recall tasks and for suggestibility. Three studies were conducted in which children and adults indicated their degree of confidence that their answers were correct after (Study 1) and before (Study 2) answering either unbiased or misleading questions or (Study 3) forced-choice recognition questions. There was a strong tendency for overestimation of confidence regardless of age and question format. However, children did not lack the principal metacognitive competencies when these questions were asked in a neutral interview. Under misleading questioning, in contrast, children's monitoring skills were seriously impaired. Within each age group, better metacognitive differentiation was positively associated with recall accuracy in the suggestive interview.
Collapse
|
214
|
|
215
|
Ghetti S, Goodman GS, Eisen ML, Qin J, Davis SL. Consistency in children's reports of sexual and physical abuse. CHILD ABUSE & NEGLECT 2002; 26:977-995. [PMID: 12433141 DOI: 10.1016/s0145-2134(02)00367-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/24/2023]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE The goal of the present study was to investigate the consistency of children's reports of sexual and physical abuse. METHOD A group of 222 children, ages 3-16 years, participated. As part of legal investigations, the children were interviewed twice about their alleged experiences of abuse. The consistency of children's reports of sexual and physical abuse was examined in the two interviews, in relation to age, type of abuse, gender, memory, suggestibility, and cognitive capabilities. RESULTS Older children were more consistent than younger children in their reports of sexual and physical abuse. Children were more consistent when reporting sexual abuse than physical abuse. Girls were more consistent than boys in sexual abuse reports. Consistency in sexual abuse reports was predicted by measures of memory, whereas consistency in physical abuse reports was not. Cognitive abilities did not predict consistency in sexual abuse or physical abuse reports. CONCLUSIONS Implications for understanding children's allegations of abuse are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Simona Ghetti
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Davis 95616, USA
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
216
|
Roberts KP. Children’s ability to distinguish between memories from multiple sources: Implications for the quality and accuracy of eyewitness statements. DEVELOPMENTAL REVIEW 2002. [DOI: 10.1016/s0273-2297(02)00005-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 53] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
|
217
|
Cognitive and psychosocial correlates of adults' eyewitness accuracy and suggestibility. PERSONALITY AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES 2002. [DOI: 10.1016/s0191-8869(01)00135-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 45] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
|
218
|
Roebers CM, Bjorklund DF, Schneider W, Cassel WS. Differences and Similarities in Event Recall and Suggestibility Between Children and Adults in Germany and the United States. Exp Psychol 2002. [DOI: 10.1027//1618-3169.49.2.132] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
Abstract
Abstract. Children (5-6 year olds, 7-8 year olds, 9-10 year olds) and adults from Germany and the United States were shown a brief video of a theft. One week later, participants were asked to give a free narrative of an observed event (free recall), followed either by sets of misleading or unbiased questions, and finally they were given a three-choice recognition question for each queried item. German participants of all ages had higher levels of correct free recall than did American participants. American adults and 9-10 year olds gave more correct responses to the open-ended unbiased questions than did their German counterparts. Germans of all ages made more correct responses to the misleading questions, whereas national differences, favoring the Germans, for incorrect response to misleading questions were restricted to the 5-6 year olds. National differences were interpreted as reflecting possible differences in strategic abilities, exposure to formal instruction, and the degree to which children experience self-directed, autonomous learning opportunities.
Collapse
|
219
|
|
220
|
Holliday RE, Hayes BK. Automatic and intentional processes in children's recognition memory: the reversed misinformation effect. APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2002. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.789] [Citation(s) in RCA: 42] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/10/2022]
|
221
|
Williams SJ, Wright DB, Freeman NH. Inhibiting children's memory of an interactive event: the effectiveness of a cover-up. APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2002. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.821] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
|
222
|
|
223
|
Ridley AM, Clifford BR, Keogh E. The effects of state anxiety on the suggestibility and accuracy of child eyewitnesses. APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2002. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.813] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022]
|
224
|
Koriat A, Goldsmith M, Schneider W, Nakash-Dura M. The credibility of children's testimony: can children control the accuracy of their memory reports? J Exp Child Psychol 2001; 79:405-37. [PMID: 11511131 DOI: 10.1006/jecp.2000.2612] [Citation(s) in RCA: 93] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
In previous work with adults (A. Koriat & M. Goldsmith, 1994, 1996c), it was shown that people can enhance the accuracy of their testimony substantially when they (a) are effective in monitoring the correctness of their answers, (b) are free to control their reporting accordingly (i.e., to decide which pieces of information to volunteer and which to withhold), and (c) are given incentives for accurate reporting. A theoretical model was developed, which specifies the critical role of metacognitive monitoring and control processes in mediating free-report memory accuracy. The present study applies that model to examine the strategic regulation of memory accuracy by children. Three experiments indicate that both younger (ages 7 to 9) and older (ages 10 to 12) children can enhance the accuracy of their testimony by screening out wrong answers under free-report conditions but suggest a developmental trend in the level of memory accuracy that is thereby achieved. The implications of the results for the dependability of children's testimony in legal settings are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- A Koriat
- Institute of Information Processing and Decision Making, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
225
|
Abstract
In these experiments, we examined the relation between age-related changes in retention and age-related changes in the misinformation effect. Children (5- and 6- and 11- and 12-year-olds) and adults viewed a video, and their memory was assessed immediately, 1 day, or 6 weeks later (Experiment 1). There were large age-related differences in retention when participants were interviewed immediately and after 1 day, but after the 6-week delay, age-related differences in retention were minimal. In Experiment 2, 11- and 12-year-olds and adults were exposed to neutral, leading, and misleading postevent information 1 day or 6 weeks after they viewed the video. Exposure to misleading information increased the number of commission errors, particularly when participants were asked about peripheral aspects of the video. At both retention intervals, children were more likely than adults to incorporate the misleading postevent information into their subsequent verbal accounts. These findings indicate that age-related changes in the misinformation effect are not predicted by age-related changes in retention.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- R Sutherland
- Department of Psychology, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand
| | | |
Collapse
|
226
|
Gordon BN, Baker-Ward L, Ornstein PA. Children's testimony: a review of research on memory for past experiences. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev 2001; 4:157-81. [PMID: 11771794 DOI: 10.1023/a:1011333231621] [Citation(s) in RCA: 41] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022]
Abstract
This review of children's testimony focuses on research related to memory for past experiences. The aspects of the memory system that are involved in testimony are discussed and the development of autobiographical memory is examined. Relevant research findings are summarized in the context of an information-processing model of memory and the implications of this work for clinical practice are outlined. We conclude that (1) under certain conditions, even very young children can remember and report past experiences with some accuracy over very long periods of time; (2) substantial and significant developmental differences have been demonstrated in children's abilities to provide eyewitness testimony; (3) children can be influenced in a variety of ways to provide complete and elaborated reports of events that never occurred; and (4) even experts cannot always tell the difference between true and false reports.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- B N Gordon
- Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, USA.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
227
|
Schreiber N, Wentura D, Bilsky W. "What else could he have done?" Creating false answers in child witnesses by inviting speculation. JOURNAL OF APPLIED PSYCHOLOGY 2001; 86:525-32. [PMID: 11419811 DOI: 10.1037/0021-9010.86.3.525] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022] Open
Abstract
In 2 experiments (N = 111 children), a suggestive technique for interviewing child witnesses called "inviting speculation" was examined. Children were presented with atypical actions for common objects in a clown show. One week later, the children were asked to speculate (e.g., "What else could he have done with the knife?") in a between-subjects design on all or none of the items (Experiment 1) and in a within-subjects design on part of the items (Experiment 2), thereby getting highly probable speculations (e.g., "to cut"). After a 3-week delay, the experimenters found more highly probable but not more other false answers for the experimental items (Experiment 2). After a 5-6-month delay, the rate of (unspecified) false answers increased compared with the baseline (Experiments 1 and 2). The short-term effect is explained by a speculation-as-misinformation assumption, whereas the long-term effect is explained by the use of a metastrategy.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- N Schreiber
- Psychologisches Institut IV, Westfälische Wilhelms-Universität Münster, Germany.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
228
|
Roebers CM, Moga N, Schneider W. The role of accuracy motivation on children's and adults' event recall. J Exp Child Psychol 2001; 78:313-29. [PMID: 11243692 DOI: 10.1006/jecp.2000.2577] [Citation(s) in RCA: 38] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
To examine the role of accuracy motivation in event recall, 6-, 7-, and 8-year-old children and adults were shown a short video about a conflict between two groups of children. Three weeks later, participants were asked a set of unbiased specific questions about the video. Following A. Koriat and M. Goldsmith's (1994) distinction of quantity- and quality-oriented memory assessments, and based on their model of strategic regulation of memory accuracy (1996), accuracy motivation was manipulated across three conditions. Participants were (a) forced to provide an answer to each question (low accuracy motivation), (b) initially instructed to withhold uncertain answers by saying "I don't know" (medium accuracy motivation), or (c) rewarded for every single correct answer (high accuracy motivation). When motivation for accuracy was high, children as young as 6 were to withhold uncertain answers to the benefit of accuracy. The expected quality-quantity trade-off emerged only for peripheral items but not for the central items. Participants who were forced to provide an answer gave more correct answers but also high numbers of incorrect answers than participants who had the option to answer "I don't know." The results are discussed in terms of the underlying model as well as in terms of forensic interviewing.
Collapse
|
229
|
Abstract
There has been unprecedented interest in recent years in questions pertaining to accuracy and distortion in memory. This interest, catalyzed in part by real-life problems, marks a significant departure from the quantity-oriented approach that has characterized much of traditional memory research. We outline a correspondence metaphor of memory underlying accuracy-oriented research, and show how the features of this metaphor are manifested across the disparate bodies of research reviewed here. These include work in the Gestalt tradition, spatial memory, memory for gist, schema theory, source monitoring, fluency misattributions, false recall and recognition, postevent misinformation, false memories, eyewitness research, and autobiographical memory. In examining the dynamics of memory accuracy, we highlight the importance of metacognitive monitoring and control processes. We end by discussing some of the methodological, theoretical, and metatheoretical issues inherent in accuracy-oriented research, attempting to prepare the groundwork for a more coherent psychology of memory accuracy.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- A Koriat
- Department of Psychology, University of Haifa, Israel.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
230
|
|
231
|
Roebers CM, Schneider W. Individual Differences in Children's Eyewitness Recall: The Influence of Intelligence and Shyness. APPLIED DEVELOPMENTAL SCIENCE 2001. [DOI: 10.1207/s1532480xads0501_2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 46] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/31/2022]
|
232
|
|