1
|
Alexander RG, Macknik SL, Martinez-Conde S. What the Neuroscience and Psychology of Magic Reveal about Misinformation. PUBLICATIONS 2022; 10:33. [PMID: 36275197 PMCID: PMC9583043 DOI: 10.3390/publications10040033] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 09/07/2024] Open
Abstract
When we believe misinformation, we have succumbed to an illusion: our perception or interpretation of the world does not match reality. We often trust misinformation for reasons that are unrelated to an objective, critical interpretation of the available data: Key facts go unnoticed or unreported. Overwhelming information prevents the formulation of alternative explanations. Statements become more believable every time they are repeated. Events are reframed or given "spin" to mislead audiences. In magic shows, illusionists apply similar techniques to convince spectators that false and even seemingly impossible events have happened. Yet, many magicians are "honest liars," asking audiences to suspend their disbelief only during the performance, for the sole purpose of entertainment. Magic misdirection has been studied in the lab for over a century. Psychological research has sought to understand magic from a scientific perspective and to apply the tools of magic to the understanding of cognitive and perceptual processes. More recently, neuroscientific investigations have also explored the relationship between magic illusions and their underlying brain mechanisms. We propose that the insights gained from such studies can be applied to understanding the prevalence and success of misinformation. Here, we review some of the common factors in how people experience magic during a performance and are subject to misinformation in their daily lives. Considering these factors will be important in reducing misinformation and encouraging critical thinking in society.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Robert G. Alexander
- Departments of Ophthalmology, Neurology, and Physiology & Pharmacology, State University of New York Downstate Health Sciences University, Brooklyn, NY 11203, USA
| | - Stephen L. Macknik
- Departments of Ophthalmology, Neurology, and Physiology & Pharmacology, State University of New York Downstate Health Sciences University, Brooklyn, NY 11203, USA
| | - Susana Martinez-Conde
- Departments of Ophthalmology, Neurology, and Physiology & Pharmacology, State University of New York Downstate Health Sciences University, Brooklyn, NY 11203, USA
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Ortega J, Montañes P, Barnhart A, Kuhn G. Differential Effects of Experience and Information Cues on Metacognitive Judgments About Others' Change Detection Abilities. Iperception 2021; 12:20416695211039242. [PMID: 34471513 PMCID: PMC8404646 DOI: 10.1177/20416695211039242] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/11/2021] [Accepted: 07/27/2021] [Indexed: 11/15/2022] Open
Abstract
This study explored the interaction between visual metacognitive judgments about others and cues related to the workings of System 1 and System 2. We examined how intrinsic cues (i.e., saliency of a visual change) and experience cues (i.e., detection/blindness) affect people's predictions about others' change detection abilities. In Experiment 1, 60 participants were instructed to notice a subtle and a salient visual change in a magic trick that exploits change blindness, after which they estimated the probability that others would detect the change. In Experiment 2, 80 participants watched either the subtle or the salient version of the trick and they were asked to provide predictions for the experienced change. In Experiment 1, participants predicted that others would detect the salient change more easily than the subtle change, which was consistent with the actual detection reported in Experiment 2. In Experiment 2, participants' personal experience (i.e., whether they detected the change) biased their predictions. Moreover, there was a significant difference between their predictions and offline predictions from Experiment 1. Interestingly, change blindness led to lower predictions. These findings point to joint contributions of experience and information cues on metacognitive judgments about other people's change detection abilities.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jeniffer Ortega
- Department of Psychology, Goldsmiths University of London, London, United Kingdom
| | - Patricia Montañes
- Department of Psychology, Goldsmiths University of London, London, United Kingdom
| | - Anthony Barnhart
- Department of Psychology, Goldsmiths University of London, London, United Kingdom
| | - Gustav Kuhn
- Department of Psychology, Goldsmiths University of London, London, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Camí J, Gomez-Marin A, Martínez LM. On the cognitive bases of illusionism. PeerJ 2020; 8:e9712. [PMID: 32904334 PMCID: PMC7453929 DOI: 10.7717/peerj.9712] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/31/2020] [Accepted: 07/23/2020] [Indexed: 12/16/2022] Open
Abstract
Cognitive scientists have paid very little attention to magic as a distinctly human activity capable of creating situations that are considered impossible because they violate expectations and conclude with the apparent transgression of well-established cognitive and natural laws. This illusory experience of the "impossible" entails a very particular cognitive dissonance that is followed by a subjective and complex "magical experience". Here, from a perspective inspired by visual neuroscience and ecological cognition, we propose a set of seven fundamental cognitive phenomena (from attention and perception to memory and decision-making) plus a previous pre-sensory stage that magicians interfere with during the presentation of their effects. By doing so, and using as an example the deconstruction of a classic trick, we show how magic offers novel and powerful insights to study human cognition. Furthermore, live magic performances afford to do so in tasks that are more ecological and context-dependent than those usually exploited in artificial laboratory settings. We thus believe that some of the mysteries of how the brain works may be trapped in the split realities present in every magic effect.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jordi Camí
- Universitat Pompeu Fabra, Barcelona, Spain
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
4
|
Morchain P, Lacroix A, Guillot S. Représentation de la magie chez des enfants de 4 à 9 ans. ENFANCE 2019. [DOI: 10.3917/enf2.194.0487] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/14/2022]
|
5
|
Kuhn G, Olson JA, Raz A. Editorial: The Psychology of Magic and the Magic of Psychology. Front Psychol 2016; 7:1358. [PMID: 27695427 PMCID: PMC5025437 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01358] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/13/2016] [Accepted: 08/25/2016] [Indexed: 11/20/2022] Open
Affiliation(s)
- Gustav Kuhn
- Department of Psychology, Goldsmiths, University of London London, UK
| | - Jay A Olson
- Cognitive Neuroscience Laboratory, Psychiatry Department, McGill University Montreal, QC, Canada
| | - Amir Raz
- Cognitive Neuroscience Laboratory, Psychiatry Department, McGill University Montreal, QC, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Hergovich A, Oberfichtner B. Magic and Misdirection: The Influence of Social Cues on the Allocation of Visual Attention While Watching a Cups-and-Balls Routine. Front Psychol 2016; 7:761. [PMID: 27303327 PMCID: PMC4885884 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00761] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/20/2016] [Accepted: 05/06/2016] [Indexed: 11/29/2022] Open
Abstract
In recent years, a body of research that regards the scientific study of magic performances as a promising method of investigating psychological phenomena in an ecologically valid setting has emerged. Seemingly contradictory findings concerning the ability of social cues to strengthen a magic trick’s effectiveness have been published. In this experiment, an effort was made to disentangle the unique influence of different social and physical triggers of attentional misdirection on observers’ overt and covert attention. The ability of 120 participants to detect the mechanism of a cups-and-balls trick was assessed, and their visual fixations were recorded using an eye-tracker while they were watching the routine. All the investigated techniques of misdirection, including sole usage of social cues, were shown to increase the probability of missing the trick mechanism. Depending on the technique of misdirection used, very different gaze patterns were observed. A combination of social and physical techniques of misdirection influenced participants’ overt attention most effectively.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Andreas Hergovich
- Department of Applied Psychology: Work, Education and Economy, Faculty of Psychology, University of Vienna Vienna, Austria
| | - Bernhard Oberfichtner
- Department of Applied Psychology: Work, Education and Economy, Faculty of Psychology, University of Vienna Vienna, Austria
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Influencing choice without awareness. Conscious Cogn 2015; 37:225-36. [DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2015.01.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/18/2014] [Revised: 12/30/2014] [Accepted: 01/06/2015] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
|
8
|
Thomas C, Didierjean A, Maquestiaux F, Gygax P. Does Magic Offer a Cryptozoology Ground for Psychology? REVIEW OF GENERAL PSYCHOLOGY 2015. [DOI: 10.1037/gpr0000041] [Citation(s) in RCA: 32] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Cyril Thomas
- Laboratoire de Psychologie, Université de Franche-Comté
| | | | | | - Pascal Gygax
- Département de Psychologie, Université de Fribourg
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Olson JA, Demacheva I, Raz A. Explanations of a magic trick across the life span. Front Psychol 2015; 6:219. [PMID: 25798117 PMCID: PMC4351568 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00219] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/16/2014] [Accepted: 02/12/2015] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Studying how children and adults explain magic tricks can reveal developmental differences in cognition. We showed 167 children (aged 4–13 years) a video of a magician making a pen vanish and asked them to explain the trick. Although most tried to explain the secret, none of them correctly identified it. The younger children provided more supernatural interpretations and more often took the magician's actions at face value. Combined with a similar study of adults (N = 1008), we found that both young children and older adults were particularly overconfident in their explanations of the trick. Our methodology demonstrates the feasibility of using magic to study cognitive development across the life span.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jay A Olson
- Cognitive Neuroscience Laboratory, Psychiatry Department, McGill University Montreal, QC, Canada
| | - Irina Demacheva
- Cognitive Neuroscience Laboratory, Psychiatry Department, McGill University Montreal, QC, Canada
| | - Amir Raz
- Cognitive Neuroscience Laboratory, Psychiatry Department, McGill University Montreal, QC, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Revealing ontological commitments by magic. Cognition 2015; 136:43-8. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.019] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/22/2012] [Revised: 10/30/2014] [Accepted: 10/31/2014] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
|
11
|
Rensink RA, Kuhn G. A framework for using magic to study the mind. Front Psychol 2015; 5:1508. [PMID: 25698983 PMCID: PMC4313584 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01508] [Citation(s) in RCA: 54] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/27/2014] [Accepted: 12/07/2014] [Indexed: 12/04/2022] Open
Abstract
Over the centuries, magicians have developed extensive knowledge about the manipulation of the human mind—knowledge that has been largely ignored by psychology. It has recently been argued that this knowledge could help improve our understanding of human cognition and consciousness. But how might this be done? And how much could it ultimately contribute to the exploration of the human mind? We propose here a framework outlining how knowledge about magic can be used to help us understand the human mind. Various approaches—both old and new—are surveyed, in terms of four different levels. The first focuses on the methods in magic, using these to suggest new approaches to existing issues in psychology. The second focuses on the effects that magic can produce, such as the sense of wonder induced by seeing an apparently impossible event. Third is the consideration of magic tricks—methods and effects together—as phenomena of scientific interest in their own right. Finally, there is the organization of knowledge about magic into an informative whole, including the possibility of a science centered around the experience of wonder.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ronald A Rensink
- Departments of Psychology and Computer Science, University of British Columbia Vancouver, BC, Canada
| | - Gustav Kuhn
- Department of Psychology, Goldsmiths University of London London, UK
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Kuhn G, Caffaratti HA, Teszka R, Rensink RA. A psychologically-based taxonomy of misdirection. Front Psychol 2014; 5:1392. [PMID: 25538648 PMCID: PMC4260479 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01392] [Citation(s) in RCA: 45] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/04/2014] [Accepted: 11/13/2014] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
Magicians use misdirection to prevent you from realizing the methods used to create a magical effect, thereby allowing you to experience an apparently impossible event. Magicians have acquired much knowledge about misdirection, and have suggested several taxonomies of misdirection. These describe many of the fundamental principles in misdirection, focusing on how misdirection is achieved by magicians. In this article we review the strengths and weaknesses of past taxonomies, and argue that a more natural way of making sense of misdirection is to focus on the perceptual and cognitive mechanisms involved. Our psychologically-based taxonomy has three basic categories, corresponding to the types of psychological mechanisms affected: perception, memory, and reasoning. Each of these categories is then divided into subcategories based on the mechanisms that control these effects. This new taxonomy can help organize magicians' knowledge of misdirection in a meaningful way, and facilitate the dialog between magicians and scientists.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gustav Kuhn
- Department of Psychology, Goldsmiths, University of London London, UK
| | - Hugo A Caffaratti
- Centre for Systems Neuroscience, University of Leicester Leicester, UK
| | - Robert Teszka
- Department of Psychology, Goldsmiths, University of London London, UK
| | - Ronald A Rensink
- Departments of Computer Science and Psychology, University of British Columbia Vancouver, BC, Canada
| |
Collapse
|