1
|
Thibodeaux JJ, Taylor PM, Bacotti JK, Morris SL. Evaluating feedback frequency preference and its relation to task performance. J Appl Behav Anal 2025; 58:232-242. [PMID: 39722543 DOI: 10.1002/jaba.2932] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/12/2024] [Accepted: 12/08/2024] [Indexed: 12/28/2024]
Abstract
Many researchers have evaluated how characteristics of feedback may influence trainee performance, but relatively little attention has been allocated to directly assessing trainee preference for feedback characteristics and its relation to performance. Thus, the primary purpose of this study was to use a within-subject experimental design to directly assess trainee preference for the frequency of feedback and its relation to task performance. A secondary objective was to evaluate how trainee preferences varied across specific task components based on component complexity. Thirty-five undergraduate students completed two arbitrary tasks and were given the opportunity to request feedback after each component of the task. For 85.71% of our participants, an inverse relation was observed between preference for feedback frequency and task performance. Participants requested feedback less often as performance improved. Feedback preferences also varied with the complexity of each component of the task. Implications for training, supervision, and feedback practices are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Janea J Thibodeaux
- Department of Psychology, Louisiana State University, Baton Rouge, LA, USA
| | - Pierce M Taylor
- Department of Psychology, Louisiana State University, Baton Rouge, LA, USA
| | - Janelle K Bacotti
- Department of Psychology, University of Miami, Coral Gables, FL, USA
| | - Samuel L Morris
- Department of Psychology, Louisiana State University, Baton Rouge, LA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Godinez ES, Brand D, Miguel CF, Penrod B. A further investigation regarding the efficacy of and preference for positive and corrective feedback. J Appl Behav Anal 2024; 57:1070-1081. [PMID: 38894697 DOI: 10.1002/jaba.1096] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/23/2023] [Accepted: 05/21/2024] [Indexed: 06/21/2024]
Abstract
Although feedback is a widely used intervention for improving performance, it is unclear what characteristics individuals prefer and what is necessary for it to be effective. The purpose of this study was to systematically extend Simonian and Brand (2022) by addressing the limitations of the study and adding a best-treatment phase. During an acquisition phase, participants received either positive, corrective, or no feedback upon task completion. Nine of 10 participants mastered the task that was associated with corrective feedback, and one participant mastered the task with no feedback. Eight participants completed a preference phase in which they were provided a choice of either positive or corrective feedback when learning to play a novel game. Half of the eight participants showed a preference for corrective feedback, and the remaining participants had mixed preferences. Overall, corrective feedback was more efficacious and more preferred than positive feedback.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Erik S Godinez
- Psychology Department, California State University, Sacramento, CA, USA
| | - Denys Brand
- Psychology Department, California State University, Sacramento, CA, USA
| | - Caio F Miguel
- Psychology Department, California State University, Sacramento, CA, USA
| | - Becky Penrod
- Psychology Department, California State University, Sacramento, CA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Auten EM, Van Camp C, Ferguson AB. A review of the concurrent-chains arrangement to assess intervention choice: 2018-2023. J Appl Behav Anal 2024; 57:319-330. [PMID: 38299638 DOI: 10.1002/jaba.1059] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/11/2023] [Accepted: 01/12/2024] [Indexed: 02/02/2024]
Abstract
The purpose of this review is to summarize recent literature on the use of concurrent-chains arrangements in the assessment of preference for interventions (or intervention components) in the applied literature. The types of interventions and participants are described briefly, and procedural variations, ethical considerations, and recommendations for future research are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Emma M Auten
- Children's Specialized Hospital-Rutgers University Center for Autism Research, Education, and Services (CSH-RUCARES), Somerset, NJ, USA
| | - Carole Van Camp
- Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina Wilmington, Wilmington, NC, USA
| | - Allie B Ferguson
- Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina Wilmington, Wilmington, NC, USA
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Briggs AM, Zohr SJ, Harvey OB. Training individuals to implement discrete-trial teaching procedures using behavioral skills training: A scoping review with implications for practice and research. J Appl Behav Anal 2024; 57:86-103. [PMID: 37772639 DOI: 10.1002/jaba.1024] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/13/2023] [Accepted: 09/01/2023] [Indexed: 09/30/2023]
Abstract
Behavioral skills training (BST) is an evidence-based approach for training individuals to implement discrete-trial teaching procedures. Despite the effectiveness of this approach, implementing BST can be time and resource intensive, which may interfere with a clinical organization's adoption of this training format. We conducted a scoping review of studies using BST components for training discrete-trial teaching procedures in peer-reviewed articles between 1977 and 2021. We identified 51 studies in 46 publications involving 354 participants. We coded descriptive data on (a) participant characteristics, (b) study characteristics, (c) training conditions (including instructions, modeling, rehearsal, and feedback), and (d) training outcomes. The results indicated that studies have primarily attempted to improve the efficacy and efficiency of BST by modifying or omitting common training components. We provide best-practice considerations for using BST to teach discrete-trial teaching procedures and offer a research agenda to guide future investigation in this area.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Adam M Briggs
- Department of Psychology, Eastern Michigan University, Ypsilanti, MI, USA
| | - Samantha J Zohr
- Department of Psychology, Eastern Michigan University, Ypsilanti, MI, USA
| | - Olivia B Harvey
- Department of Psychology, Eastern Michigan University, Ypsilanti, MI, USA
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Slanzi CM, Sellers T. Paying for Supervision: Barriers, Solutions, and Opportunities. Behav Anal Pract 2023; 16:363-373. [PMID: 37187853 PMCID: PMC10170001 DOI: 10.1007/s40617-022-00727-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 06/15/2022] [Indexed: 11/25/2022] Open
Abstract
In some situations, those requiring supervision to meet Behavior Analyst Certification Board supervised fieldwork requirements, maintain certification, or get assistance with a difficult case or ethical dilemma may need to contract with a qualified supervisor and pay them directly. Although it is not considered to be a multiple relationship, the financial component does carry an inherent conflict of interest, which can create barriers to effective and appropriate supervision. In this article we propose a list of barriers that may arise in this particular supervisory relationship and potential solutions to manage each one, with a particular focus on supervised independent fieldwork. We also discuss unique learning opportunities that may arise from this situation that may be beneficial to both the trainee and supervisor.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Crystal M. Slanzi
- Department of Social and Behavioral Sciences, Temple University, 1700 N Broad Street, Philadelphia, PA 19121 USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
6
|
Johnson DA, Johnson CM, Dave P. Performance Feedback in Organizations: Understanding the Functions, Forms, and Important Features. JOURNAL OF ORGANIZATIONAL BEHAVIOR MANAGEMENT 2022. [DOI: 10.1080/01608061.2022.2089436] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Douglas A. Johnson
- Western Michigan University, Department of Psychology, Kalamazoo, Michigan, USA
| | - C. Merle Johnson
- Central Michigan University, Department of Psychology, Mount Pleasant, Michigan, USA
| | - Priyanka Dave
- Western Michigan University, Department of Psychology, Kalamazoo, Michigan, USA
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Simonian MJ, Brand D. Assessing the efficacy of and preference for positive and corrective feedback. J Appl Behav Anal 2022; 55:727-745. [PMID: 35297040 DOI: 10.1002/jaba.911] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/23/2021] [Revised: 01/23/2022] [Accepted: 01/24/2022] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
Abstract
Feedback is an effective strategy for improving performance and consists of multiple characteristics. One characteristic that can influence feedback efficacy is its nature (whether feedback is positive or corrective) and little is known about the conditions under which individuals may prefer corrective over positive feedback. Thus, the purpose of this study was to assess the efficacy of and preference for positive and corrective feedback during the acquisition of novel tasks. In the first phase, participants received either positive, corrective, or no feedback across three novel tasks. Participants only mastered the task in which they received corrective feedback. In the second phase, participants chose to receive either positive or corrective feedback after completing trials of the previous phase's control task. All participants chose to receive corrective feedback more frequently than positive feedback. We discuss the implications of the results for feedback delivery in the workplace and provide suggestions for future research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Denys Brand
- Department of Psychology, California State University, Sacramento
| |
Collapse
|