1
|
Anderson SR, Porrill J, Dean P. World Statistics Drive Learning of Cerebellar Internal Models in Adaptive Feedback Control: A Case Study Using the Optokinetic Reflex. Front Syst Neurosci 2020; 14:11. [PMID: 32269515 PMCID: PMC7111124 DOI: 10.3389/fnsys.2020.00011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/30/2019] [Accepted: 02/07/2020] [Indexed: 01/06/2023] Open
Abstract
The cerebellum is widely implicated in having an important role in adaptive motor control. Many of the computational studies on cerebellar motor control to date have focused on the associated architecture and learning algorithms in an effort to further understand cerebellar function. In this paper we switch focus to the signals driving cerebellar adaptation that arise through different motor behavior. To do this, we investigate computationally the contribution of the cerebellum to the optokinetic reflex (OKR), a visual feedback control scheme for image stabilization. We develop a computational model of the adaptation of the cerebellar response to the world velocity signals that excite the OKR (where world velocity signals are used to emulate head velocity signals when studying the OKR in head-fixed experimental laboratory conditions). The results show that the filter learnt by the cerebellar model is highly dependent on the power spectrum of the colored noise world velocity excitation signal. Thus, the key finding here is that the cerebellar filter is determined by the statistics of the OKR excitation signal.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sean R. Anderson
- Department of Automatic Control and Systems Engineering, University of Sheffield, Sheffield, United Kingdom
| | - John Porrill
- Department of Psychology, University of Sheffield, Sheffield, United Kingdom
| | - Paul Dean
- Department of Psychology, University of Sheffield, Sheffield, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Lappi O. Eye movements in the wild: Oculomotor control, gaze behavior & frames of reference. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2016; 69:49-68. [PMID: 27461913 DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.06.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 34] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/16/2015] [Revised: 05/14/2016] [Accepted: 06/08/2016] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
Abstract
Understanding the brain's capacity to encode complex visual information from a scene and to transform it into a coherent perception of 3D space and into well-coordinated motor commands are among the outstanding questions in the study of integrative brain function. Eye movement methodologies have allowed us to begin addressing these questions in increasingly naturalistic tasks, where eye and body movements are ubiquitous and, therefore, the applicability of most traditional neuroscience methods restricted. This review explores foundational issues in (1) how oculomotor and motor control in lab experiments extrapolates into more complex settings and (2) how real-world gaze behavior in turn decomposes into more elementary eye movement patterns. We review the received typology of oculomotor patterns in laboratory tasks, and how they map onto naturalistic gaze behavior (or not). We discuss the multiple coordinate systems needed to represent visual gaze strategies, how the choice of reference frame affects the description of eye movements, and the related but conceptually distinct issue of coordinate transformations between internal representations within the brain.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Otto Lappi
- Cognitive Science, Institute of Behavioural Sciences, PO BOX 9, 00014 University of Helsinki, Finland.
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Instruction dependent activation during optokinetic nystagmus (OKN) stimulation: An FMRI study at 3T. Brain Res 2010; 1336:10-21. [DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.04.017] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/07/2010] [Accepted: 04/08/2010] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
|
4
|
Burke MR, Barnes GR. Anticipatory eye movements evoked after active following versus passive observation of a predictable motion stimulus. Brain Res 2008; 1245:74-81. [PMID: 18930718 DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.09.073] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/15/2008] [Accepted: 09/16/2008] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
Abstract
We used passive and active following of a predictable smooth pursuit stimulus in order to establish if predictive eye movement responses are equivalent under both passive and active conditions. The smooth pursuit stimulus was presented in pairs that were either 'predictable' in which both presentations were matched in timing and velocity, or 'randomized' in which each presentation in the pair was varied in both timing and velocity. A visual cue signaled the type of response required from the subject; a green cue indicated the subject should follow both the target presentations (Go-Go), a pink cue indicated that the subject should passively observe the 1st target and follow the 2nd target (NoGo-Go), and finally a green cue with a black cross revealed a randomized (Rnd) trial in which the subject should follow both presentations. The results revealed better prediction in the Go-Go trials than in the NoGo-Go trials, as indicated by higher anticipatory velocity and earlier eye movement onset (latency). We conclude that velocity and timing information stored from passive observation of a moving target is diminished when compared to active following of the target. This study has significant consequences for understanding how visuomotor memory is generated, stored and subsequently released from short-term memory.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- M R Burke
- Institute of Psychological Sciences, University of Leeds, Leeds, LS29JT, UK.
| | | |
Collapse
|
5
|
Abadi RV, Howard IP, Ohmi M, Lee EE. The Effect of Central and Peripheral Field Stimulation on the Rise Time and Gain of Human Optokinetic Nystagmus. Perception 2005; 34:1015-24. [PMID: 16178157 DOI: 10.1068/p5251b] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
Abstract
We wished to examine the spatial (gain) and temporal (rise time) properties of human optokinetic nystagmus (OKN) as a function of stimulus velocity and field location. Stimuli were either M-scaled random dots or vertical stripes that moved at velocities between 20–80 deg s−1. Three field conditions were examined: full field; a 20 deg central field; and a 12.5 deg central-field mask. OKN gain was found to be significantly affected by stimulus velocity and stimulus location, with the higher stimulus velocities and the 12.5 deg central-field mask giving lower gains. Steady-state gains for all three field conditions were not found to be affected by prior adaptation to stationary or moving stimuli. The 63% rise time was found to be significantly affected by the stimulus velocity, whereas this was not the case for the 90% rise time. Neither rise time was found to be significantly affected by the field location. These results indicate that, although the effectiveness (gain) of peripheral retina is lower than that of the central retina during optokinetic stimulation, the peripheral retina has access to common mechanisms responsible for the fast component of OKN.
Collapse
|
6
|
Konen CS, Kleiser R, Seitz RJ, Bremmer F. An fMRI study of optokinetic nystagmus and smooth-pursuit eye movements in humans. Exp Brain Res 2005; 165:203-16. [PMID: 15864563 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-005-2289-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 100] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/28/2004] [Accepted: 01/20/2005] [Indexed: 12/16/2022]
Abstract
Both optokinetic nystagmus (OKN) and smooth-pursuit eye movements (SPEM) are subclasses of so-called slow eye movements. However, optokinetic responses are reflexive whereas smooth pursuit requires the voluntary tracking of a moving target. We used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to determine the neural basis of OKN and SPEM, and to uncover whether the two underlying neural systems overlap or are independent at the cortical level. The results showed a largely overlapping neural circuitry. A direct comparison between activity during the execution of OKN and SPEM yielded no oculomotor-related area exclusively dedicated to one or the other eye movement type. Furthermore, the performance of SPEM evoked a bilateral deactivation of the human equivalent of the parietoinsular vestibular cortex. This finding might indicate that the reciprocally inhibitory visual-vestibular interaction involves not only OKN but also SPEM, which are both linked with the encoding of object-motion and self-motion. Moreover, we could show differential activation patterns elicited by look-nystagmus and stare-nystagmus. Look-nystagmus is characterized by large amplitudes and low-frequency resetting eye movements rather resembling SPEM. Look-nystagmus evoked activity in cortical oculomotor centers. By contrast, stare-nystagmus is usually characterized as being more reflexive in nature and as showing smaller amplitudes and higher frequency resetting eye movements. Stare-nystagmus failed to elicit significant signal changes in the same regions as look-nystagmus/SPEM. Thus, less reflexive eye movements correlated with more pronounced signal intensity. Finally, on the basis of a general investigation of slow eye movements, we were interested in a cortical differentiation between subtypes of SPEM. We compared activity associated with predictable and unpredictable SPEM as indicated by appropriate visual cues. In general, predictable and unpredictable SPEM share the same neural network, yet information about the direction of an upcoming target movement reduced the cerebral activity level.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Christina S Konen
- Department of Neurophysics, Philipps-University Marburg, Renthof 7, 35032 Marburg, Germany.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
7
|
Seemungal BM, Faldon M, Revesz T, Lees AJ, Zee DS, Bronstein AM. Influence of target size on vertical gaze palsy in a pathologically proven case of progressive supranuclear palsy. Mov Disord 2003; 18:818-22. [PMID: 12815662 DOI: 10.1002/mds.10434] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/07/2022] Open
Abstract
We document a new oculomotor phenomenon in a patient with pathologically proven progressive supranuclear gaze palsy (PSP), namely that vertical gaze excursion improves with larger pursuit targets. We used computerised video-oculography during vertical smooth pursuit eye movements (SPEM) of circular targets of diameter 0.16 degrees and 16 degrees, sinusoidally oscillating at 0.08 Hz (peak-to-peak amplitude 49 degrees). Increasing target size improved vertical gaze excursion from 10 degrees to 25 degrees. There was no concomitant increase in slow phase eye velocity. The findings could be explained by a potentiation of the position control mechanism of pursuit by target size due to increased activation of brainstem pursuit-optokinetic pathways and to higher order attentional mechanisms. This observation may be useful in the clinical assessment of PSP patients with severe neck rigidity in whom the doll's head-eye manoeuvre cannot be performed by comparing the degree of vertical gaze palsy during smooth pursuit testing between at least two differently sized targets and observing whether there is a larger excursion in response to a large target such as a newspaper.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Barry M Seemungal
- The Academic Department of Neuro-Otology, Division of Neurosciences and Psychological Medicine, Imperial College Faculty of Medicine, Charing Cross Hospital, London, United Kingdom
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
8
|
Schweigart G, Maurer C, Mergner T. Combined action of smooth pursuit eye movements, optokinetic reflex and vestibulo-ocular reflex in macaque monkey during transient stimulation. Neurosci Lett 2003; 340:217-20. [PMID: 12672545 DOI: 10.1016/s0304-3940(03)00138-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
Abstract
The interaction of smooth pursuit eye movements, vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR) and optokinetic reflex (OKR) is still not well understood. We therefore measured in macaque monkeys horizontal eye movements using transient horizontal rotations of a visual target, of monkeys' heads and/or of an optokinetic background pattern (ten combinations; smoothed position ramps of 16 degrees ). With intermediate peak velocity of target motion (v(max)=12.8 degrees /s), pursuit held the eyes rather well on target, almost independent of concurrent vestibular or optokinetic stimuli (pursuit gain, 0.73-0.91). With v(max)=1.6 degrees /s, in contrast, pursuit gain became strongly modified by the optokinetic stimulus. With v(max)=51.2 degrees /s, pursuit gain became modified by vestibular stimulation. Although not intuitive, the experimental data can be explained by linear interaction (summation) of the neural driving signals for pursuit, VOR and OKR, as ascertained by simulations of a dynamic model.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- G Schweigart
- Neurocenter, University of Freiburg, Breisacher Strasse 64, 79106 Freiburg, Germany.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
9
|
Abstract
Subjects smoothly pursued a target moving horizontally at 15 deg/s. After pursuit for 1 s, the target jumped 3 deg ahead of the fovea. At the moment of the jump, target velocity became 0 and 'effective visual feedback' assumed a value of either 0 (target retinally stabilized), -0.2, -0.4, or -1.0 (target fixed in space). With 0 visual feedback the eye continued to move smoothly at a moderate velocity, an apparent response to target position relative to the fovea. When negative visual feedback was present eye velocity decreased. With -0.2 and -0.4 feedback, this decrease was not a simple exponential, but often consisted of an initial fast decrease followed by slower decrease. With -1.0 feedback, eye velocity quickly decreased in an approximately exponential manner, and stopped. We were able to simulate these pursuit responses using a simple model of the pursuit system. Key features of the model are: (a) a target-velocity channel whose output decreases with target offset from the fovea, and whose gain switches from high to low as pursuit velocity approaches zero; (b) a target-position channel with a saturation non-linearity at 1-3 deg; and (c) a positive feedback loop with gain of less than 1.0. All of these features are essential to simulate the pursuit responses, especially with visual feedback values of -0.2 and -0.4. Our results and model suggest that target position serves as an important stimulus in guiding smooth pursuit as pursuit velocity decreases, and especially during pursuit termination.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- J Pola
- Schnurmacher Institute for Vision Research, State University of New York, State College of Optometry, 33 West 42nd Street, New York, NY 10036, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
10
|
Abadi RV, Howard IP, Ohmi M. Gaze orientation during full-field and peripheral field passive optokinesis. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt 1999. [DOI: 10.1046/j.1475-1313.1999.00426.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
|
11
|
Abstract
Monkeys and humans are able to perform different types of slow eye movements. The analysis of the eye movement parameters, as well as the investigation of the neuronal activity underlying the execution of slow eye movements, offer an excellent opportunity to study higher brain functions such as motion processing, sensorimotor integration, and predictive mechanisms as well as neuronal plasticity and motor learning. As an example, since there exists a tight connection between the execution of slow eye movements and the processing of any kind of motion, these eye movements can be used as a biological, behavioural probe for the neuronal processing of motion. Global visual motion elicits optokinetic nystagmus, acting as a visual gaze stabilization system. The underlying neuronal substrate consists mainly of the cortico-pretecto-olivo-cerebellar pathway. Additionally, another gaze stabilization system depends on the vestibular input known as the vestibulo-ocular reflex. The interactions between the visual and vestibular stabilization system are essential to fulfil the plasticity of the vestibulo-ocular reflex representing a simple form of learning. Local visual motion is a necessary prerequisite for the execution of smooth pursuit eye movements which depend on the cortico-pontino-cerebellar pathway. In the wake of saccades, short-latency eye movements can be elicited by brief movements of the visual scene. Finally, eye movements directed to objects in different planes of depth consist of slow movements also. Although there is some overlap in the neuronal substrates underlying these different types of slow eye movements, there are brain areas whose activity can be associated exclusively with the execution of a special type of slow eye movement.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- U J Ilg
- Sektion für Visuelle Sensomotorik, Neurologische Universitätsklinik, Tübingen, Germany.
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Pola J, Wyatt HJ. Offset dynamics of human smooth pursuit eye movements: effects of target presence and subject attention. Vision Res 1997; 37:2579-95. [PMID: 9373690 DOI: 10.1016/s0042-6989(97)00058-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 46] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/05/2023]
Abstract
Subjects made smooth pursuit eye movements with a target moving horizontally at 15 deg/sec. At a specified location the target either: (1) suddenly vanished; or (2) jumped to the fovea with target retinal velocity and feedback becoming 0 (target stabilized at the fovea). In each type of trial, the subjects either: "looked" at the target, "pushed" the target, or "passively" gazed. When the target vanished, eye velocity decreased exponentially with a short time-constant (tau approximately 0.10 sec), regardless of whether the subjects were "looking," "pushing" or "passively" gazing. However, some subjects while "pushing" (using an imaginary target) did generate low velocity smooth movement (1-2.5 deg/sec) late in the offset. When the target was stabilized at the fovea, eye velocity also decreased, but with a relatively long time-constant (tau = 0.4-0.8 sec). The time-constant was the same with both "looking," and "pushing", but was shorter for some subjects with "passive" gazing (tau = 0.1-0.5 sec). These findings show that smooth pursuit offset is influenced by the presence of a target, but is relatively independent of attentional mode. All of the pursuit offset responses can be simulated using a model of the pursuit system with target velocity and position inputs, and an internal positive feedback loop enabled by target presence.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- J Pola
- Schnurmacher Institute for Vision Research, State University of New York, State College of Optometry, NY 10010, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
13
|
Schweigart G, Mergner T, Evdokimidis I, Morand S, Becker W. Gaze stabilization by optokinetic reflex (OKR) and vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR) during active head rotation in man. Vision Res 1997; 37:1643-52. [PMID: 9231230 DOI: 10.1016/s0042-6989(96)00315-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/04/2023]
Abstract
Vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR)-optokinetic reflex (OKR) interaction was studied in normal human subjects during active sine-like head movements in the horizontal plane for a variety of vestibular-optokinetic stimulus combinations (frequency range, 0.05-1.6 Hz). At low to mid frequencies (< 0.2 Hz) the eyes tended to be stabilized on the optokinetic pattern, independently of whether the head, the pattern, or both were rotated. At higher frequencies, the OKR gain was attenuated and, in each of the differing stimulus combinations, the eyes became increasingly stabilized in space. Qualitatively similar results were obtained when, for the same visual-vestibular combinations, the head was passively rotated at 0.05 and 0.8 Hz. The data could be simulated by a model which assumes a linear interaction of vestibular and optokinetic signals. It considers the OKR with its negative feedback loop of primordial importance for image stabilization on the retina and the VOR only as a useful addition which compensates for the limited bandwidth of the OKR during high frequency/velocity head rotations in a stationary visual environment.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- G Schweigart
- Neurological Clinic, Neurocenter, Freiburg, Germany
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
14
|
Schweigart G, Mergner T. Combined action of optokinetic reflex (OKR) and vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR) in macaque monkey during transient stimulation. Neurosci Lett 1995; 199:123-6. [PMID: 8584239 DOI: 10.1016/0304-3940(95)12037-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/31/2023]
Abstract
Interaction of vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR) and optokinetic reflex (OKR) was studied in macaque monkeys by recording horizontal eye movements during transient rotations of their heads and/or an optokinetic pattern in space. At low peak velocities of the stimuli (1.25 degrees/s, 10.0 degrees/s) the eyes were rather well stabilized on the optokinetic pattern, independently of whether the head, the pattern, or both were rotated. At higher velocities (40.0 degrees/s), the OKR gain was attenuated and, when combining vestibular and optokinetic stimuli, the eyes became increasingly stabilized in space. The data could be simulated by a computer model previously designed to describe VOR-OKR interaction during sinusoidal rotations. In this model eye stabilization primarily relies on the OKR, while the role of the VOR is to compensate for the limited bandwidth of the OKR.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- G Schweigart
- Ruhr-University Bochum, Department of Neurophysiology MA4, Germany
| | | |
Collapse
|
15
|
Wyatt HJ, Pola J, Lustgarten M, Aksionoff E. Optokinetic nystagmus (OKN) suppression by fixation of a stabilized target: the effect of OKN-stimulus predictability. Vision Res 1995; 35:2903-10. [PMID: 8533330 DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(95)00062-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/31/2023]
Abstract
In previous work, subjects looked at a target stabilized at the fovea, superimposed on a sinusiodally moving OKN stimulus. The stabilized target (no retinal-slip) suppressed OKN leaving residual eye movements that were often in counterphase with the OKN stimulus motion. In the present study we explored how this type of suppression of OKN is influenced by OKN stimulus predictability: OKN stimulus motion was either sinusoidal or a random walk of half-sinusoids. During fixation of a stabilized target with sinusoidal stimulus motion, OKN was suppressed leaving residual eye movement whose amplitude was typically less than OKN and with a phase lag of about 180 deg (roughly in counterphase with stimulus motion). With random-walk stimulus motion, the residual movement amplitude was even smaller, and at higher frequencies the phase lag decreased to become the same as for OKN. For both stimulus motions, OKN was suppressed when the target was present, but counterphase residual movements appear to depend on stimulus predictability.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- H J Wyatt
- Schnurmacher Institute for Vision Research, State University of New York College of Optometry, NY 10010, USA
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
16
|
Deno DC, Crandall WF, Sherman K, Keller EL. Characterization of prediction in the primate visual smooth pursuit system. Biosystems 1995; 34:107-28. [PMID: 7727693 DOI: 10.1016/0303-2647(94)01446-e] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/26/2023]
Abstract
To define predictive behavior and mechanisms in visual smooth pursuit, various target motions were presented to 2 monkeys. Target stimuli included: single sinusoids (1's), triangle waves (T's), sums of 4 nonharmonically related sinusoids (4's), bandpass limited white noise (B's), and wideband white noise (N's). Velocity error was least for 1's, greatest for N's, and intermediate for T's, 4's, and B's. For the bandlimited 4's and B's, monkeys demonstrated the greatest relative amplitude response at the highest frequencies. Predictive mechanisms are classified as short- and long-term, depending on how much past target motion information is employed. The T's and a modification of this stimulus pattern involving a random perturbation were used to test the hypothesis that prediction is based exclusively on short-term signal processing related to target position and its derivatives. The existence of long-term predictive mechanisms in monkey smooth pursuit was unequivocally demonstrated with the use of the latter stimulus.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- D C Deno
- Department of Electrical Engineering and Computer Sciences University of California, Berkeley 94720, USA
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
17
|
Abstract
The visual system interacts synergistically with the vestibular system. A normally functioning vestibulo-ocular reflex is necessary but not sufficient for optimum visual acuity during head motion. Studies of dynamic visual acuity, the acuity achieved during relative motion of visual targets or of the observer, indicate that motion of images on the retina markedly compromises vision. The vestibulo-ocular reflex normally provides a substantial measure of stabilization of the retina during head movements, but purely vestibular compensatory eye movements are not sufficiently precise for optimal vision under all circumstances. Other mechanisms, including visual tracking, motor preprogramming, prediction, and mental set, interact synergistically to optimize the gain (eye velocity divided by head velocity) of compensatory head movements. All of these mechanisms are limited in their capacity to produce effective visual-vestibular interaction at higher rotational frequencies and velocities. It is under these conditions that vestibular deficits give rise to symptoms of oscillopsia. Patients having vestibular lesions exploit mechanisms of visual-vestibular interaction to compensate by substitution for deficient vestibular function. Thus, for accurate topographic clinical diagnosis of vestibular lesions, testing conditions should isolate purely vestibular responses. This may be done by testing reflex eye movements during passively generated rotations in darkness, or perhaps by testing during other types of motion under conditions of extreme frequency and velocity sufficient to attenuate the effects of visual-vestibular interaction. This article reviews clinical tests of vestibular function in relation to synergistic interactions with vision.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- J L Demer
- Jules Stein Eye Institute, UCLA 90024-7002
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Smooth Pursuit Over a Structured Background: The Effects of Temporal and Spatial Changes in Target and Background. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 1994. [DOI: 10.1016/b978-0-444-81808-9.50016-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register]
|
19
|
Barnes GR. Visual-vestibular interaction in the control of head and eye movement: the role of visual feedback and predictive mechanisms. Prog Neurobiol 1993; 41:435-72. [PMID: 8210413 DOI: 10.1016/0301-0082(93)90026-o] [Citation(s) in RCA: 99] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Affiliation(s)
- G R Barnes
- MRC Human Movement and Balance Unit, Institute of Neurology, London, U.K
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Pola J, Wyatt HJ, Lustgarten M. Suppression of optokinesis by a stabilized target: effects of instruction and stimulus frequency. PERCEPTION & PSYCHOPHYSICS 1992; 52:186-200. [PMID: 1508626 DOI: 10.3758/bf03206772] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/27/2022]
Abstract
Subjects viewed a foveally stabilized target presented against a background field of dots moving sinusoidally. Several different modes of viewing the target were used (subjects were instructed to gaze, look, or hold), and the frequency of sinusoidal field motion was varied from 1/32 to 2 Hz. In line with previous findings, the presence of a stabilized target resulted in substantial suppression of optokinesis. The characteristics of this suppression (gain and phase of slow residual eye movements) were dependent on both the mode of viewing the target and the frequency of field motion. When subjects used an imaginary target, little suppression occurred. These findings provide an overall profile of dynamic characteristics of mechanisms involved in the suppression of optokinesis. They support the view that this suppression is significantly determined by the presence of a target against a moving background (even without retinal slip), and by the mode of attending to the target.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- J Pola
- Schnurmacher Institute for Vision Research, State University of New York, State College of Optometry, NY 10010
| | | | | |
Collapse
|