1
|
Zolotareva AD, Chernetsov NS. Celestial Orientation in Birds. BIOL BULL+ 2021. [DOI: 10.1134/s1062359021090259] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
|
2
|
Foveal shape, ultrastructure and photoreceptor composition in yellow-legged gull, Larus michahellis (Naumann, 1840). ZOOMORPHOLOGY 2021. [DOI: 10.1007/s00435-020-00512-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/13/2022]
|
3
|
Bringmann A. Structure and function of the bird fovea. Anat Histol Embryol 2019; 48:177-200. [DOI: 10.1111/ahe.12432] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/05/2018] [Revised: 01/07/2019] [Accepted: 01/15/2019] [Indexed: 12/30/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Andreas Bringmann
- Department of Ophthalmology and Eye Hospital, Medical Faculty University of Leipzig Leipzig Germany
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Delius JD, Delius JAM, Lee JM. Symmetry recognition by pigeons: Generalized or not? PLoS One 2017; 12:e0187541. [PMID: 29121110 PMCID: PMC5679541 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0187541] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/15/2017] [Accepted: 10/20/2017] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
This note looks into the reasons why earlier reports may have arrived at differing conclusions about pigeons’ capacity to categorize bilaterally symmetric and asymmetric visual patterns. Attention is drawn to pigeons’ comparatively superior visual flicker resolution and superior visual linear acuity by reporting results of two ad-hoc experiments. This circumstance turns out to constrain conclusions drawn by earlier symmetry–asymmetry studies that used computer-generated patterns displayed on cathode ray tube monitors as these suffered from pictorial distortions. Additionally one of the studies involved patterns of inconsistent symmetry at global and local levels. A smaller-scale experiment using slide-projected unequivocal symmetric and asymmetric patterns yielded results compatible with the supposition that pigeons are capable of a symmetry–asymmetry categorization. The possibility that an artfactual cue may have inadvertently accentuated this capability in an earlier own experiment is considered.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Juan D. Delius
- Allgemeine Psychologie, Universität Konstanz, Konstanz, Germany
- * E-mail:
| | - Julia A. M. Delius
- Center for Lifespan Psychology, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany
| | - Jennifer M. Lee
- Allgemeine Psychologie, Universität Konstanz, Konstanz, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Melgar J, Lind O, Muheim R. No response to linear polarization cues in operant conditioning experiments with zebra finches. J Exp Biol 2015; 218:2049-54. [DOI: 10.1242/jeb.122309] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/15/2015] [Accepted: 04/27/2015] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
Many animals can use the polarization of light in various behavioural contexts. Birds are well known to use information from the skylight polarization pattern for orientation and compass calibration. Still, there are few controlled studies of polarization vision in birds, and the majority of them have not been successful in convincingly demonstrating polarization vision. We used a two-alternative forced choice conditioning approach to assess linear polarization vision in male zebra finches in the “visible” spectral range (wavelengths>400 nm). The birds were trained to discriminate colour, brightness, and polarization stimuli presented on either one of two LCD-screens. All birds were able to discriminate the colour and brightness stimuli, but they were unable to discriminate the polarization stimuli. Our results suggest that in the behavioural context studied here, zebra finches are not able to discriminate polarized light stimuli.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Julian Melgar
- Lund Vision Group, Department of Biology, Lund University, Biology Building B, Sölvegatan 35, SE-223 62 Lund, Sweden
| | - Olle Lind
- Lund Vision Group, Department of Biology, Lund University, Biology Building B, Sölvegatan 35, SE-223 62 Lund, Sweden
- Cognitive Science, Department of Philosophy, Lund University, LUX, Helgonavägen 3, SE-223 62 Lund, Sweden
| | - Rachel Muheim
- Lund Vision Group, Department of Biology, Lund University, Biology Building B, Sölvegatan 35, SE-223 62 Lund, Sweden
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
|
7
|
Muheim R. Behavioural and physiological mechanisms of polarized light sensitivity in birds. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2011; 366:763-71. [PMID: 21282180 DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0196] [Citation(s) in RCA: 51] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Polarized light (PL) sensitivity is relatively well studied in a large number of invertebrates and some fish species, but in most other vertebrate classes, including birds, the behavioural and physiological mechanism of PL sensitivity remains one of the big mysteries in sensory biology. Many organisms use the skylight polarization pattern as part of a sun compass for orientation, navigation and in spatial orientation tasks. In birds, the available evidence for an involvement of the skylight polarization pattern in sun-compass orientation is very weak. Instead, cue-conflict and cue-calibration experiments have shown that the skylight polarization pattern near the horizon at sunrise and sunset provides birds with a seasonally and latitudinally independent compass calibration reference. Despite convincing evidence that birds use PL cues for orientation, direct experimental evidence for PL sensitivity is still lacking. Avian double cones have been proposed as putative PL receptors, but detailed anatomical and physiological evidence will be needed to conclusively describe the avian PL receptor. Intriguing parallels between the functional and physiological properties of PL reception and light-dependent magnetoreception could point to a common receptor system.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rachel Muheim
- Department of Biology, Lund University, Lund 223 62, Sweden.
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Mäthger LM, Lohmann KJ, Limpus CJ, Fritsches KA. An unsuccessful attempt to elicit orientation responses to linearly polarized light in hatchling loggerhead sea turtles (Caretta caretta). Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2011; 366:757-62. [PMID: 21282179 PMCID: PMC3049016 DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0212] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Sea turtles undertake long migrations in the open ocean, during which they rely at least partly on magnetic cues for navigation. In principle, sensitivity to polarized light might be an additional sensory capability that aids navigation. Furthermore, polarization sensitivity has been linked to ultraviolet (UV) light perception which is present in sea turtles. Here, we tested the ability of hatchling loggerheads (Caretta caretta) to maintain a swimming direction in the presence of broad-spectrum polarized light. At the start of each trial, hatchling turtles, with their magnetic sense temporarily impaired by magnets, successfully established a steady course towards a light-emitting diode (LED) light source while the polarized light field was present. When the LED was removed, however, hatchlings failed to maintain a steady swimming direction, even though the polarized light field remained. Our results have failed to provide evidence for polarized light perception in young sea turtles and suggest that alternative cues guide the initial migration offshore.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lydia M. Mäthger
- Sensory Neurobiology Group, School of Biomedical Sciences, University of Queensland, Brisbane, QLD 4072, Australia
- Marine Biological Laboratory, Marine Resources Center, Woods Hole, MA 02543, USA
| | - Kenneth J. Lohmann
- Department of Biology, CB no. 3280, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, NC 27599, USA
| | - Colin J. Limpus
- Department of Environment and Resource Management, Brisbane, QLD 4002, Australia
| | - Kerstin A. Fritsches
- Sensory Neurobiology Group, School of Biomedical Sciences, University of Queensland, Brisbane, QLD 4072, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Mäthger LM, Shashar N, Hanlon RT. Do cephalopods communicate using polarized light reflections from their skin? J Exp Biol 2009; 212:2133-40. [DOI: 10.1242/jeb.020800] [Citation(s) in RCA: 66] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
SUMMARY
Cephalopods (squid, cuttlefish and octopus) are probably best known for their ability to change color and pattern for camouflage and communication. This is made possible by their complex skin, which contains pigmented chromatophore organs and structural light reflectors (iridophores and leucophores). Iridophores create colorful and linearly polarized reflective patterns. Equally interesting, the photoreceptors of cephalopod eyes are arranged in a way to give these animals the ability to detect the linear polarization of incoming light. The capacity to detect polarized light may have a variety of functions, such as prey detection, navigation, orientation and contrast enhancement. Because the skin of cephalopods can produce polarized reflective patterns, it has been postulated that cephalopods could communicate intraspecifically through this visual system. The term `hidden' or`private' communication channel has been given to this concept because many cephalopod predators may not be able to see their polarized reflective patterns. We review the evidence for polarization vision as well as polarization signaling in some cephalopod species and provide examples that tend to support the notion – currently unproven – that some cephalopods communicate using polarized light signals.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lydia M. Mäthger
- Marine Resources Center, Marine Biological Laboratory, Woods Hole, MA 02543,USA
| | - Nadav Shashar
- Department of Life Sciences, Eilat Campus, Ben Gurion University, Beer Sheva,84105, Israel
| | - Roger T. Hanlon
- Marine Resources Center, Marine Biological Laboratory, Woods Hole, MA 02543,USA
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Ramsden SD, Anderson L, Mussi M, Kamermans M, Hawryshyn CW. Retinal processing and opponent mechanisms mediating ultraviolet polarization sensitivity in rainbow trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss). ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2008; 211:1376-85. [PMID: 18424671 DOI: 10.1242/jeb.015941] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
A number of teleost fishes have photoreceptor mechanisms to detect linearly polarized light. We studied the neuronal mechanism underlying this ability. It was found that a polarized signal could be detected in rainbow trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss) both in the electroretinogram (ERG) and in the compound action potential (CAP) measured in the optic nerve, indicating a strong retinal contribution to the processing of polarized light. The CAP recordings showed a W-shaped sensitivity curve, with a peak at 0 degrees , 90 degrees and 180 degrees , consistent with processes for both vertical and horizontal orientation. By contrast, the ERG recordings reveal a more complex pattern. In addition to the peaks at 0 degrees , 90 degrees and 180 degrees , two additional peaks appeared at 45 degrees and 135 degrees . This result suggests a specialized contribution of the outer retina in the processing of polarized light. The spectral sensitivity of the mechanisms responsible for these intermediate peaks was studied using chromatic adaptation. Here we show that long wavelength-sensitive (LWS) cone mechanism adaptation shifted the intermediate peaks towards 90 degrees , whereas ultraviolet-sensitive (UVS) cone mechanism adaptation shifted the peaks away from 90 degrees towards either 0 degrees or 180 degrees . These results provide further confirmation that the 90 degrees peak is dominated by the LWS cone mechanism and the 0 degrees and 180 degrees peaks are dominated by the UVS cone mechanism. In addition, a pharmacological approach was used to examine the retinal neural mechanisms underlying polarization sensitivity. The effect of blocking negative feedback from horizontal cells to cones on the ERG was studied by making intraocular injections of low doses of cobalt, known to block this feedback pathway. It was found that the intermediate peaks seen in the ERG polarization sensitivity curves were eliminated after application of cobalt, suggesting that these peaks are due to outer retinal inhibition derived from feedback of horizontal cells onto cones. A simple computational model was developed to evaluate these results. The model consists of opponent and non-opponent processing elements for the two polarization detectors. This model provides a first approximation analysis suggesting that opponent processing occurs in the outer retina for polarization vision. Although it seems that polarization vision uses a slightly more complicated coding scheme than colour vision, the results presented in this paper suggest that opponent and non-opponent channels process polarization information.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Samuel D Ramsden
- Department of Biology, University of Victoria, PO Box 3020 STN CSC, Victoria, British Columbia, V8W 3N5, Canada
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
11
|
Jones MP, Pierce KE, Ward D. Avian Vision: A Review of Form and Function with Special Consideration to Birds of Prey. J Exot Pet Med 2007. [DOI: 10.1053/j.jepm.2007.03.012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 144] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/11/2022]
|
12
|
Abstract
Most processes or forms of learning have been treated almost as special creations, each as an independent process unrelated to others. This review offers an evolutionary cladogram linking nearly one hundred forms of learning and showing the paths through which they evolved. Many processes have multiple forms. There are at least five imprinting processes, eleven varieties of Pavlovian conditioning, ten of instrumental conditioning, and eight forms of mimicry and imitation. Song learning evolved independently in at least six groups of animals, and movement imitation in three (great apes, cetaceans and psittacine birds). The cladogram also involves at least eight new processes: abstract concept formation, percussive mimicry, cross-modal imitation, apo-conditioning, hybrid conditioning, proto-pantomime, prosodic mimicry, and image-mediated learning. At least eight of the processes evolved from more than one source. Multiple sources are of course consistent with modern evolutionary theory, as seen in some obligate symbionts, and gene-swapping organisms. Song learning is believed to have evolved from two processes: auditory imprinting and skill learning. Many single words evolved from three sources: vocal mimicry, discrimination learning, and abstract concept formation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Bruce R Moore
- Department of Psychology, Dalhousie University, Halifax, NS, Canada B3H 4J1.
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Hegedüs R, Åkesson S, Wehner R, Horváth G. Could Vikings have navigated under foggy and cloudy conditions by skylight polarization? On the atmospheric optical prerequisites of polarimetric Viking navigation under foggy and cloudy skies. Proc Math Phys Eng Sci 2007. [DOI: 10.1098/rspa.2007.1811] [Citation(s) in RCA: 62] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
In sunshine, the Vikings navigated on the open sea using sundials. According to a widespread hypothesis, when the Sun was occluded by fog or clouds the Vikings might have navigated by skylight polarization detected with an enigmatic birefringent crystal (sunstone). There are two atmospheric optical prerequisites for this alleged polarimetric Viking navigation under foggy/cloudy skies: (1) the degree of linear polarization
p
of skylight should be high enough and (2) at a given Sun position, the pattern of the angle of polarization
α
of the foggy/cloudy sky should be similar to that of the clear sky. Until now, these prerequisites have not been investigated. Using full-sky imaging polarimetry, we measured the
p
- and
α
-patterns of Arctic foggy and cloudy skies when the Sun was invisible. These patterns were compared with the polarization patterns of clear Arctic skies. We show here that although prerequisite (2) is always fulfilled under both foggy and cloudy conditions, if the fog layer is illuminated by direct sunlight, prerequisite (1) is usually satisfied only for cloudy skies. In sunlit fog, the Vikings could have navigated by polarization only, if
p
of light from the foggy sky was sufficiently high.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ramón Hegedüs
- Biooptics Laboratory, Department of Biological Physics, Physical Institute, Eötvös University1117 Budapest, Pázmány sétány 1, Hungary
| | - Susanne Åkesson
- Department of Animal Ecology, Lund UniversityEcology Building, 223 62 Lund, Sweden
| | - Rüdiger Wehner
- Zoological Institute, University of ZürichWinterthurerstrasse 190, 8057 Zürich, Switzerland
| | - Gábor Horváth
- Biooptics Laboratory, Department of Biological Physics, Physical Institute, Eötvös University1117 Budapest, Pázmány sétány 1, Hungary
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Leask MJ. A physicochemical mechanism for magnetic field detection by migratory birds and homing pigeons. Nature 2005; 267:144-5. [PMID: 16073421 DOI: 10.1038/267144a0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 150] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/23/1976] [Accepted: 03/16/1977] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
Abstract
Migratory birds and homing pigeons can apparently obtain directional information from the Earth's magnetic field. The effect is difficult to detect, and discussion of the possible process of magnetic field detection by birds seems so far to have foundered on the simple fact that the orientational effect of the Earth's magnetic field on a single electron spin associated with a molecule of animal tissue would be of the order 10(-8) eV--almost certainly too small to be detectable biologically. Here I direct attention to a process which would overcome this basic problem, and which also seems to provide an explanation of all the main features of published data. It is a mechanism in principle only, however, and is discussed here in no more detail than is necessary to clarify the basic ideas and to provide a basis for further investigation.
Collapse
|
15
|
Greenwood VJ, Smith EL, Church SC, Partridge JC. Behavioural investigation of polarisation sensitivity in the Japanese quail (Coturnix coturnix japonica) and the European starling (Sturnus vulgaris). J Exp Biol 2003; 206:3201-10. [PMID: 12909701 DOI: 10.1242/jeb.00537] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
Many animals have sensitivity to the e-vector of linearly polarised light, which may assist in visually mediated behaviours such as navigation, signalling and foraging. However, it is still controversial as to whether birds possess polarisation sensitivity. Several studies have found that altering the polarisation patterns of the broad visual field surrounding birds alters their intended migratory orientation. However, electrophysiological tests have failed to elicit evidence for polarisation sensitivity in birds, and the mechanism by which birds might perceive polarised light is unknown. In this experiment, we trained Japanese quail and European starlings to discriminate stimuli differing in their polarisation pattern. Although both quail and starlings were able to discriminate stimuli in which the stimulus sub-components either differed or had the same radiant intensity (the control task), they were unable to discriminate stimuli in which the e-vector orientations of the stimulus sub-components either differed by 90 degrees or had the same angle of polarisation. The birds' successful performance on the control task, but failure to complete the polarisation task, demonstrated that they had all the necessary cognitive abilities to make the discrimination except sensitivity to angle of polarisation. We conclude that quail and starlings are unable to use polarisation cues in this foraging task.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Verity J Greenwood
- Ecology of Vision Research Laboratory, School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
16
|
Parkyn DC, Austin JD, Hawryshyn CW. Acquisition of polarized-light orientation in salmonids under laboratory conditions. Anim Behav 2003. [DOI: 10.1006/anbe.2003.2136] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
|
17
|
Abstract
The spectral sensitivities of avian retinal photoreceptors are examined with respect to microspectrophotometric measurements of single cells, spectrophotometric measurements of extracted or in vitro regenerated visual pigments, and molecular genetic analyses of visual pigment opsin protein sequences. Bird species from diverse orders are compared in relation to their evolution, their habitats and the multiplicity of visual tasks they must perform. Birds have five different types of visual pigment and seven different types of photoreceptor-rods, double (uneven twin) cones and four types of single cone. The spectral locations of the wavelengths of maximum absorbance (lambda(max)) of the different visual pigments, and the spectral transmittance characteristics of the intraocular spectral filters (cone oil droplets) that also determine photoreceptor spectral sensitivity, vary according to both habitat and phylogenetic relatedness. The primary influence on avian retinal design appears to be the range of wavelengths available for vision, regardless of whether that range is determined by the spectral distribution of the natural illumination or the spectral transmittance of the ocular media (cornea, aqueous humour, lens, vitreous humour). Nevertheless, other variations in spectral sensitivity exist that reflect the variability and complexity of avian visual ecology.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- N S Hart
- Vision, Touch and Hearing Research Centre, Department of Physiology and Pharmacology, The University of Queensland, Brisbane 4072, Australia.
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
|
19
|
Freake MJ. Evidence for orientation using the e-vector direction of polarised light in the sleepy lizard tiliqua rugosa. J Exp Biol 1999; 202 (Pt 9):1159-66. [PMID: 10101113 DOI: 10.1242/jeb.202.9.1159] [Citation(s) in RCA: 28] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
Adult sleepy lizards (Tiliqua rugosa) were trained to orient in a predictable direction under natural sky light in outdoor pens. When tested under clear skies in the late afternoon, without a view of the sun, the lizards exhibited a symmetrical bimodal pattern of orientation with respect to the trained axis. Since the e-vector of polarised light provides an axial rather than a polar cue, the bimodal orientation exhibited by the lizards is consistent with the use of a celestial compass based on sky polarisation patterns. To confirm that the lizards could orient with respect to a polarisation pattern, lizards were trained in indoor pens to orient in a predictable direction under a linearly polarised light source. When tested in a circular arena illuminated by another polarised light source, the lizards used the e-vector direction of the polarised light source to orient along the trained axis. There was no evidence that the lizards were using any room-specific cues or brightness patterns to orient in the training direction. These results support the hypothesis that the lizards can use the e-vector direction of polarised light in the form of a sky polarisation compass.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- MJ Freake
- School of Biological Sciences, The Flinders University of South Australia, GPO Box 2100, Adelaide, SA 5001, Australia. mfreake@bio. indiana.edu
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Wiltschko R, Wiltschko W. Das Orientierungssystem der Vögel I. Kompaßmechanismen. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 1999. [DOI: 10.1007/bf02462086] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
|
21
|
Tove´e MJ. Reply from M.J. Tovee. Trends Ecol Evol 1996; 11:130. [DOI: 10.1016/0169-5347(96)91626-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
|
22
|
|
23
|
Able KP, Able MA. Daytime calibration of magnetic orientation in a migratory bird requires a view of skylight polarization. Nature 1993. [DOI: 10.1038/364523a0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 71] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
|
24
|
Abstract
Our present understanding of orientation behaviour in birds is based on a broad array of observational, experimental and analytical (statistical) techniques, which are briefly reviewed here. As an extremely productive model the homing behaviour of pigeons has allowed especially diverse experimental manipulations documenting the involvement of magnetic, visual and olfactory cues in orientation. Work with migratory birds has profited greatly from the design of several kinds of orientation cages, now commonly used, and from hand-rearing test birds under controlled conditions. Free-flying birds, especially on long-distance migration, are still least amenable to study, but radio transmitter technology is providing important new opportunities in this respect. In general, the most valuable studies have been those involving the ontogenetic development of orientation, and those combining several methods of investigation. Some suggestions for further experiments are made.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- A J Helbig
- Institut für Pharmazeutische Biologie, Universität Heidelberg, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
Depolarization of natural skylight disrupts orientation of an avian nocturnal migrant. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 1990. [DOI: 10.1007/bf01939958] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
|
26
|
Coemans MA, Vos Hzn JJ, Nuboer JF. No evidence for polarization sensitivity in the pigeon. THE SCIENCE OF NATURE - NATURWISSENSCHAFTEN 1990; 77:138-42. [PMID: 2342581 DOI: 10.1007/bf01134480] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/31/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- M A Coemans
- Laboratory of Comparative Physiology, Utrecht University
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
27
|
Sunset, skylight polarization and the migratory orientation of yellow-rumped warblers, Dendroica coronata. Anim Behav 1988. [DOI: 10.1016/s0003-3472(88)80116-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 42] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
|
28
|
Phillips JB, Waldvogel JA. Celestial polarized light patterns as a calibration reference for sun compass of homing pigeons. J Theor Biol 1988. [DOI: 10.1016/s0022-5193(88)80120-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 37] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
|
29
|
Waldvogel JA, Phillips JB, Brown AI. Changes in the short-term deflector loft effect are linked to the sun compass of homing pigeons. Anim Behav 1988. [DOI: 10.1016/s0003-3472(88)80258-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
|
30
|
|
31
|
Hawryshyn CW, McFarland WN. Cone photoreceptor mechanisms and the detection of polarized light in fish. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 1987. [DOI: 10.1007/bf00615079] [Citation(s) in RCA: 72] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
|
32
|
Presti DE. Avian Navigation, Geomagnetic Field Sensitivity, and Biogenic Magnetite. TOPICS IN GEOBIOLOGY 1985. [DOI: 10.1007/978-1-4613-0313-8_22] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/18/2023]
|
33
|
Young SR, Martin GR. Optics of retinal oil droplets: a model of light collection and polarization detection in the avian retina. Vision Res 1984; 24:129-37. [PMID: 6710875 DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(84)90098-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 69] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/21/2023]
Abstract
A wave optical model was used to analyse the scattering properties of avian retinal oil droplets. Computations for the near field region showed that oil droplets perform significant light collection in cone photoreceptors and so enhance outer segment photon capture rates. Scattering by the oil droplet of the principal cone of a double cone pair, combined with accessory cone dichroic absorption under conditions of transverse illumination, may mediate avian polarization sensitivity.
Collapse
|
34
|
|
35
|
|
36
|
Moore FR. Sunset and the orientation of a nocturnal bird migrant: A mirror experiment. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 1982. [DOI: 10.1007/bf00300176] [Citation(s) in RCA: 33] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
|
37
|
|
38
|
|
39
|
Quine DB, Kreithen ML. Frequency shift discrimination: Can homing pigeons locate infrasounds by Doppler shifts? J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 1981. [DOI: 10.1007/bf01342661] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
|
40
|
|
41
|
Wiltschko R. Die Sonnenorientierung der Vögel. II. Entwicklung des Sonnenkompaß und sein Stellenwert im Orientierungssystem. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 1981. [DOI: 10.1007/bf01643440] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
|
42
|
A wavelength discrimination function for the hummingbirdArchilochus alexandri. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 1981. [DOI: 10.1007/bf00606073] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
|
43
|
|
44
|
Moore FR. Solar cues in the migratory orientation of the Savannah sparrow, Passerculus sandwichensis. Anim Behav 1980. [DOI: 10.1016/s0003-3472(80)80129-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 35] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
|
45
|
Emmerton J, Delhis JD. Wavelength discrimination in the ?visible? and ultraviolet spectrum by pigeons. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 1980. [DOI: 10.1007/bf00611877] [Citation(s) in RCA: 66] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
|
46
|
Auburn JS, Taylor DH. Polarized light perception and orientation in larval bullfrogs Rana catesbeiana. Anim Behav 1979. [DOI: 10.1016/0003-3472(79)90003-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
|
47
|
Kreithen ML, Quine DB. Infrasound detection by the homing pigeon: A behavioral audiogram. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 1979. [DOI: 10.1007/bf00679906] [Citation(s) in RCA: 97] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/21/2022]
|
48
|
|
49
|
Sensory Mechanisms Related to Homing in Pigeons. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 1978. [DOI: 10.1007/978-3-662-11147-5_3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register]
|
50
|
|