1
|
Chiyohara S, Furukawa JI, Noda T, Morimoto J, Imamizu H. Proprioceptive short-term memory in passive motor learning. Sci Rep 2023; 13:20826. [PMID: 38012253 PMCID: PMC10682388 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-023-48101-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/24/2023] [Accepted: 11/22/2023] [Indexed: 11/29/2023] Open
Abstract
A physical trainer often physically guides a learner's limbs to teach an ideal movement, giving the learner proprioceptive information about the movement to be reproduced later. This instruction requires the learner to perceive kinesthetic information and store the instructed information temporarily. Therefore, (1) proprioceptive acuity to accurately perceive the taught kinesthetics and (2) short-term memory to store the perceived information are two critical functions for reproducing the taught movement. While the importance of proprioceptive acuity and short-term memory has been suggested for active motor learning, little is known about passive motor learning. Twenty-one healthy adults (mean age 25.6 years, range 19-38 years) participated in this study to investigate whether individual learning efficiency in passively guided learning is related to these two functions. Consequently, learning efficiency was significantly associated with short-term memory capacity. In particular, individuals who could recall older sensory stimuli showed better learning efficiency. However, no significant relationship was observed between learning efficiency and proprioceptive acuity. A causal graph model found a direct influence of memory on learning and an indirect effect of proprioceptive acuity on learning via memory. Our findings suggest the importance of a learner's short-term memory for effective passive motor learning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Shinya Chiyohara
- Brain Information Communication Research Laboratory Group, Advanced Telecommunications Research Institute International (ATR), Keihanna Science City, Kyoto, 619-0288, Japan
| | - Jun-Ichiro Furukawa
- Brain Information Communication Research Laboratory Group, Advanced Telecommunications Research Institute International (ATR), Keihanna Science City, Kyoto, 619-0288, Japan
- Man-Machine Collaboration Research Team, Guardian Robot Project, RIKEN, Kyoto, Japan
| | - Tomoyuki Noda
- Brain Information Communication Research Laboratory Group, Advanced Telecommunications Research Institute International (ATR), Keihanna Science City, Kyoto, 619-0288, Japan
| | - Jun Morimoto
- Brain Information Communication Research Laboratory Group, Advanced Telecommunications Research Institute International (ATR), Keihanna Science City, Kyoto, 619-0288, Japan.
- Man-Machine Collaboration Research Team, Guardian Robot Project, RIKEN, Kyoto, Japan.
- Graduate School of Informatics, Kyoto University, Kyoto, Japan.
| | - Hiroshi Imamizu
- Brain Information Communication Research Laboratory Group, Advanced Telecommunications Research Institute International (ATR), Keihanna Science City, Kyoto, 619-0288, Japan
- Department of Psychology, Graduate School of Humanities and Sociology, The University of Tokyo, Hongo 7-3-1, Bunkyo-Ku, Tokyo, 113-0033, Japan
- Research Into Artifacts, Center for Engineering, School of Engineering, The University of Tokyo, Hongo 7-3-1, Bunkyo-Ku, Tokyo, 113-8656, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Heled E, Israeli R, Margalit D. Working memory development in different modalities in children and young adults. J Exp Child Psychol 2022; 220:105422. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2022.105422] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/05/2021] [Revised: 02/08/2022] [Accepted: 03/02/2022] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
|
3
|
Erp JBFV, Paul KI, Mioch T. Tactile Working Memory Capacity of Users Who Are Blind in an Electronic Travel Aid Application with a Vibration Belt. ACM TRANSACTIONS ON ACCESSIBLE COMPUTING 2020. [DOI: 10.1145/3372273] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
Abstract
Electronic travel aids (ETAs) can increase the safety and comfort of pedestrians who have a visual impairment by displaying obstacles through a vibrotactile navigation belt. Building a complete picture of relevant obstacles and finding a safe route requires ETA users to integrate vibrotactile cues over time and space in their tactile working memory. Previous research suggests that the sense of touch exhibits a working memory that has characteristics similar to vision and audition. However, the capacity of the tactile working memory and the effects of secondary tasks are still under-researched. We investigated tactile working memory capacity of 14 adolescent participants who are blind in an immediate, whole report recall test. Participants received trials consisting of one to five vibration patterns presented sequentially at different locations on their torso representing obstacles with a direction (vibration location) and distance (vibration pattern). Recall performance was assessed under four conditions: baseline and with distracting background sounds and/or while walking with the long cane. Both walking and ignoring distracting sounds are relevant for everyday use of an ETA and were expected to decrease memory performance. We calculated the 75% correct scores for two memory performance measures: the number of items in a trial (numerosity), and item location and pattern correct. In the baseline condition, the scores were close to ceiling (i.e., 5 items). However, in the presence of distracting sounds and while walking, the scores were reduced to 3.2 items for numerosity and 1.6 items for location and identity correct. We recommend using 2 items as the maximum tactile working memory load in an applied setting unless users are trained and/or can adopt their strategy without unacceptable costs, such as reducing their walking speed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Katja I. Paul
- University of Groningen and Max-Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany
| | | |
Collapse
|
4
|
Lauzon AP, Russo FA, Harris LR. The influence of rhythm on detection of auditory and vibrotactile asynchrony. Exp Brain Res 2020; 238:825-832. [PMID: 32130431 PMCID: PMC7181424 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-019-05720-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/08/2019] [Accepted: 12/30/2019] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
Abstract
The perception of an event is strongly influenced by the context in which it occurs. Here, we examined the effect of a rhythmic context on detection of asynchrony in both the auditory and vibrotactile modalities. Using the method of constant stimuli and a two-alternative forced choice (2AFC), participants were presented with pairs of pure tones played either simultaneously or with various levels of stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA). Target stimuli in both modalities were nested within either: (i) a regularly occurring, predictable rhythm (ii) an irregular, unpredictable rhythm, or (iii) no rhythm at all. Vibrotactile asynchrony detection had higher thresholds and showed greater variability than auditory asynchrony detection in general. Asynchrony detection thresholds for auditory targets but not vibrotactile targets were significantly reduced when the target stimulus was embedded in a regular rhythm as compared to no rhythm. Embedding within an irregular rhythm produced no such improvement. The observed modality asymmetries are interpreted with regard to the superior temporal resolution of the auditory system and specialized brain circuitry supporting auditory-motor coupling.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Andrew P Lauzon
- Department of Psychology, York University, 4700 Keele St, Toronto, ON, M3J 1P3, Canada.
- Centre for Vision Research, York University, Toronto, ON, Canada.
| | - Frank A Russo
- Department of Psychology, Ryerson University, Toronto, ON, Canada
| | - Laurence R Harris
- Department of Psychology, York University, 4700 Keele St, Toronto, ON, M3J 1P3, Canada
- Centre for Vision Research, York University, Toronto, ON, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Diffusion modeling of interference and decay in auditory short-term memory. Exp Brain Res 2019; 237:1899-1905. [DOI: 10.1007/s00221-019-05533-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/01/2018] [Accepted: 03/27/2019] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
|
6
|
Sasangohar F, Donmez B, Easty AC, Trbovich PL. Effects of Nested Interruptions on Task Resumption: A Laboratory Study With Intensive Care Nurses. HUMAN FACTORS 2017; 59:628-639. [PMID: 28128985 DOI: 10.1177/0018720816689513] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/06/2023]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE Interruptions to secondary tasks resulting in multiple tasks to resume may tax working memory. The objective of this research is to study such interruptions experienced by intensive care unit (ICU) nurses. BACKGROUND ICU nurses are frequently interrupted, resulting in a switch from primary to secondary tasks. In two recent studies, we observed that some of these secondary tasks also get interrupted, resulting in multiple tasks that have to be resumed, a phenomenon we refer to as nested interruptions. Although completing multiple secondary tasks in a serial fashion during an interruption period can create context-switching costs, we hypothesize that nested interruptions tax the working memory even more than just performing multiple secondary tasks sequentially because the nurse would have to encode in working memory the resumption goals for both the primary and the interrupted secondary tasks. METHOD We conducted a laboratory study with 30 ICU nurses, who performed an electronic order-entry task under three interruption conditions: (a) baseline-no secondary task during the interruption period; (2) serial-performance of two tasks one after the other during the interruption period; and (3) nested-performance of two tasks during the interruption period, one of which was also interrupted. RESULTS Nested interruptions resulted in significantly longer primary-task resumption lag and less accurate task resumption compared with both the serial interruption and baseline conditions. CONCLUSION The nested nature of interruptions adds to the resumption lag and diminishes resumption accuracy by likely populating the working memory with goals associated with interrupted secondary tasks.
Collapse
|
7
|
Abstract
Studies of interference in working and short-term memory suggest that irrelevant information may overwrite the contents of memory or intrude into memory. While some previous studies have reported greater interference when irrelevant information is similar to the contents of memory than when it is dissimilar, other studies have reported greater interference for dissimilar distractors than for similar distractors. In the present study, we find the latter effect in a paradigm that uses auditory tones as stimuli. We suggest that the effects of distractor similarity to memory contents are mediated by the type of information held in memory, particularly the complexity or simplicity of information.
Collapse
|
8
|
Inferring the depth of 3-D objects from tactile spatial information. Atten Percept Psychophys 2015; 77:1411-22. [PMID: 25762304 DOI: 10.3758/s13414-015-0878-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Four psychophysical experiments were conducted to examine the relation between tactile spatial information and the estimated depth of partially touched 3-D objects. Human participants touched unseen, tactile grating patterns with their hand while keeping the hand shape flat. Experiment 1, by means of a production task, showed that the estimated depth of the concave part below the touched grating was well correlated with the separation between the elements of the grating, but not with the overall size of the grating, nor with the local structure of the touched parts. Experiments 2 and 3, by means of a haptic working memory task, showed that the remembered depth of a target surface was biased toward the estimated bottom position of a tactile grating distractor. Experiment 4, by means of a discrimination task, revealed that tactile grating patterns influenced speeded judgments about visual 3-D shapes. These results suggest that the haptic system uses heuristics based on spatial information to infer the depth of an untouched part of a 3-D object.
Collapse
|
9
|
Simulating stimulus- and TMS-induced interference in short-term memory using a model of prefrontal cortex. BMC Neurosci 2014. [PMCID: PMC4125045 DOI: 10.1186/1471-2202-15-s1-p141] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/10/2022] Open
|
10
|
Bancroft TD, Hogeveen J, Hockley WE, Servos P. TMS-induced neural noise in sensory cortex interferes with short-term memory storage in prefrontal cortex. Front Comput Neurosci 2014; 8:23. [PMID: 24634653 PMCID: PMC3942793 DOI: 10.3389/fncom.2014.00023] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/15/2013] [Accepted: 02/10/2014] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
In a previous study, Harris et al. (2002) found disruption of vibrotactile short-term memory after applying single-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) to primary somatosensory cortex (SI) early in the maintenance period, and suggested that this demonstrated a role for SI in vibrotactile memory storage. While such a role is compatible with recent suggestions that sensory cortex is the storage substrate for working memory, it stands in contrast to a relatively large body of evidence from human EEG and single-cell recording in primates that instead points to prefrontal cortex as the storage substrate for vibrotactile memory. In the present study, we use computational methods to demonstrate how Harris et al.'s results can be reproduced by TMS-induced activity in sensory cortex and subsequent feedforward interference with memory traces stored in prefrontal cortex, thereby reconciling discordant findings in the tactile memory literature.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tyler D Bancroft
- Department of Psychology, Wilfrid Laurier University Waterloo, ON, Canada
| | - Jeremy Hogeveen
- Department of Psychology, Wilfrid Laurier University Waterloo, ON, Canada
| | - William E Hockley
- Department of Psychology, Wilfrid Laurier University Waterloo, ON, Canada
| | - Philip Servos
- Department of Psychology, Wilfrid Laurier University Waterloo, ON, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Irrelevant sensory stimuli interfere with working memory storage: Evidence from a computational model of prefrontal neurons. COGNITIVE AFFECTIVE & BEHAVIORAL NEUROSCIENCE 2012; 13:23-34. [DOI: 10.3758/s13415-012-0131-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
|
12
|
Fernandes AM, Albuquerque PB. Tactual perception: a review of experimental variables and procedures. Cogn Process 2012; 13:285-301. [PMID: 22669262 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-012-0443-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/01/2012] [Accepted: 05/18/2012] [Indexed: 01/05/2023]
Abstract
This paper reviews the literature on tactual perception. Throughout this review, we will highlight some of the most relevant aspects in the touch literature: type of stimuli; type of participants; type of tactile exploration; and finally, the interaction between touch and other senses. Regarding type of stimuli, we will analyse studies with abstract stimuli such as vibrations, with two- and three-dimensional stimuli, and also concrete stimuli, considering the relation between familiar and unfamiliar stimuli and the haptic perception of faces. Under the "type of participants" topic, we separated studies with blind participants, studies with children and adults, and also performed an overview of sex differences in performance. The type of tactile exploration is explored considering conditions of active and passive touch, the relevance of movement in touch and the relation between haptic exploration and time. Finally, interactions between touch and vision, touch and smell and touch and taste are explored in the last topic. The review ends with an overall conclusion on the state of the art for the tactual perception literature. With this work, we intend to present an organised overview of the main variables in touch experiments, compiling aspects reported in the tactual literature, and attempting to provide both a summary of previous findings, and a guide to the design of future works on tactual perception and memory, through a presentation of implications from previous studies.
Collapse
|
13
|
Abstract
Previous studies of delayed-match-to-sample (DMTS) frequency discrimination in animals and humans have succeeded in delineating the neural signature of frequency processing in somatosensory working memory (WM). During retention of vibrotactile frequencies, stimulus-dependent single-cell and population activity in prefrontal cortex was found to reflect the task-relevant memory content, whereas increases in occipital alpha activity signaled the disengagement of areas not relevant for the tactile task. Here, we recorded EEG from human participants to determine the extent to which these mechanisms can be generalized to frequency retention in the visual and auditory domains. Subjects performed analogous variants of a DMTS frequency discrimination task, with the frequency information presented either visually, auditorily, or by vibrotactile stimulation. Examining oscillatory EEG activity during frequency retention, we found characteristic topographical distributions of alpha power over visual, auditory, and somatosensory cortices, indicating systematic patterns of inhibition and engagement of early sensory areas, depending on stimulus modality. The task-relevant frequency information, in contrast, was found to be represented in right prefrontal cortex, independent of presentation mode. In each of the three modality conditions, parametric modulations of prefrontal upper beta activity (20-30 Hz) emerged, in a very similar manner as recently found in vibrotactile tasks. Together, the findings corroborate a view of parametric WM as supramodal internal scaling of abstract quantity information and suggest strong relevance of previous evidence from vibrotactile work for a more general framework of quantity processing in human working memory.
Collapse
|
14
|
Bancroft TD, Hockley WE, Servos P. Can vibrotactile working memory store multiple items? Neurosci Lett 2012; 514:31-4. [PMID: 22387156 DOI: 10.1016/j.neulet.2012.02.044] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/06/2011] [Revised: 01/10/2012] [Accepted: 02/14/2012] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
Vibrotactile working memory is increasing in popularity as a model system to test theories of working memory. Notably, however, we know little about vibrotactile working memory capacity. While most other domains of working memory are able to store multiple items (for example, the seven-plus-or-minus-two capacity of verbal memory [17]), previous examinations of vibrotactile working memory suggest that stored items may suffer from high levels of interference in the form of overwriting or representation-based interference [2,4], potentially limiting capacity and also limiting our ability to draw comparisons between vibrotactile working memory and other forms of working memory. In the present study, we use a two-item delayed match-to-sample paradigm to demonstrate that subjects are able to store multiple items in vibrotactile working memory, suggesting that interference does not catastrophically limit capacity, and strengthening our ability to compare vibrotactile working memory to other working memory tasks.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tyler D Bancroft
- Department of Psychology, Wilfrid Laurier University, Waterloo, ON, Canada
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
15
|
Abstract
The nature of interference in working memory has been a subject of discussion for decades. It has previously been argued that irrelevant stimuli can interfere with working memory by being encoded into memory. Previous findings have suggested that irrelevant sensory activity can interfere with the storage of information in tactile working memory. More recently, it has been suggested that this type of interference may operate through the overwriting of stored information by interfering sensory stimuli, even when participants are instructed to ignore such stimuli. Such a mechanism of interference is consistent with previous theoretical proposals. In the present study, we use a computational diffusion model to demonstrate that previous empirical findings are best explained by the encoding of irrelevant sensory information and subsequent interference.
Collapse
|
16
|
Bancroft TD, Hockley WE, Servos P. Vibrotactile working memory as a model paradigm for psychology, neuroscience, and computational modeling. Front Hum Neurosci 2011; 5:162. [PMID: 22163217 PMCID: PMC3234499 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00162] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/06/2011] [Accepted: 11/21/2011] [Indexed: 12/03/2022] Open
Affiliation(s)
- Tyler D Bancroft
- Department of Psychology, Wilfrid Laurier University Waterloo, ON, Canada
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
17
|
Bancroft TD, Servos P, Hockley WE. Mechanisms of interference in vibrotactile working memory. PLoS One 2011; 6:e22518. [PMID: 21818331 PMCID: PMC3144235 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0022518] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/31/2011] [Accepted: 06/27/2011] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
In previous studies of interference in vibrotactile working memory, subjects were presented with an interfering distractor stimulus during the delay period between the target and probe stimuli in a delayed match-to-sample task. The accuracy of same/different decisions indicated feature overwriting was the mechanism of interference. However, the distractor was presented late in the delay period, and the distractor may have interfered with the decision-making process, rather than the maintenance of stored information. The present study varies the timing of distractor onset, (either early, in the middle, or late in the delay period), and demonstrates both overwriting and non-overwriting forms of interference.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tyler D. Bancroft
- Department of Psychology, Wilfrid Laurier University, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada
| | - Philip Servos
- Department of Psychology, Wilfrid Laurier University, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada
- * E-mail:
| | - William E. Hockley
- Department of Psychology, Wilfrid Laurier University, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada
| |
Collapse
|