1
|
Lew-Levy S, Reckin R, Kissler SM, Pretelli I, Boyette AH, Crittenden AN, Hagen RV, Haas R, Kramer KL, Koster J, O'Brien MJ, Sonoda K, Surovell TA, Stieglitz J, Tucker B, Lavi N, Ellis-Davies K, Davis HE. Socioecology shapes child and adolescent time allocation in twelve hunter-gatherer and mixed-subsistence forager societies. Sci Rep 2022; 12:8054. [PMID: 35577896 PMCID: PMC9110336 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-022-12217-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/13/2021] [Accepted: 05/06/2022] [Indexed: 11/09/2022] Open
Abstract
A key issue distinguishing prominent evolutionary models of human life history is whether prolonged childhood evolved to facilitate learning in a skill- and strength-intensive foraging niche requiring high levels of cooperation. Considering the diversity of environments humans inhabit, children's activities should also reflect local social and ecological opportunities and constraints. To better understand our species' developmental plasticity, the present paper compiled a time allocation dataset for children and adolescents from twelve hunter-gatherer and mixed-subsistence forager societies (n = 690; 3-18 years; 52% girls). We investigated how environmental factors, local ecological risk, and men and women's relative energetic contributions were associated with cross-cultural variation in child and adolescent time allocation to childcare, food production, domestic work, and play. Annual precipitation, annual mean temperature, and net primary productivity were not strongly associated with child and adolescent activity budgets. Increased risk of encounters with dangerous animals and dehydration negatively predicted time allocation to childcare and domestic work, but not food production. Gender differences in child and adolescent activity budgets were stronger in societies where men made greater direct contributions to food production than women. We interpret these findings as suggesting that children and their caregivers adjust their activities to facilitate the early acquisition of knowledge which helps children safely cooperate with adults in a range of social and ecological environments. These findings compel us to consider how childhood may have also evolved to facilitate flexible participation in productive activities in early life.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sheina Lew-Levy
- Department of Human Behavior, Ecology and Culture, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Deutscher Pl. 6, 04103, Leipzig, Germany.
- Department of Comparative Cultural Psychology, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Deutscher Pl. 6, 04103, Leipzig, Germany.
| | - Rachel Reckin
- Department of Archaeology, University of Cambridge, Downing Street, Cambridge, CV2 3DZ, UK
| | - Stephen M Kissler
- Department of Immunology and Infectious Diseases, Harvard T.H. Chan School of Public Health, 665 Huntington Ave, Boston, MA, 02115, USA
| | - Ilaria Pretelli
- Department of Human Behavior, Ecology and Culture, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Deutscher Pl. 6, 04103, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Adam H Boyette
- Department of Human Behavior, Ecology and Culture, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Deutscher Pl. 6, 04103, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Alyssa N Crittenden
- Department of Anthropology, University of Nevada, Las Vegas, 4505 S. Maryland Pkwy., Las Vegas, NV, 89154, USA
| | - Renée V Hagen
- Department of Anthropology, University of California, Los Angeles, 375 Portola Plaza, Los Angeles, CA, 90095, USA
| | - Randall Haas
- Department of Anthropology, Wayne State University, 656 W. Kirby St., 3037 FAB, Detroit, MI, 48202, USA
| | - Karen L Kramer
- Department of Anthropology, University of Utah, 260 Central Campus Drive, Suite 4553, Salt Lake City, UT, 84112, USA
| | - Jeremy Koster
- Department of Anthropology, University of Cincinnati, 481 Braunstein Hall, PO Box 210380, Cincinnati, OH, 45221-0380, USA
| | - Matthew J O'Brien
- Department of Anthropology, California State University, Chico, 400 W. First St., Chico, CA, 95929-0400, USA
| | - Koji Sonoda
- Faculty of Humanities and Social Sciences, Niigata University, 8050 Ikarashi 2-no-cho, Nishi-ku, Niigata, 950-2181, Japan
| | - Todd A Surovell
- Department of Anthropology, University of Wyoming, 12th and Lewis Streets, Laramie, WY, 8207, USA
| | - Jonathan Stieglitz
- Institute for Advanced Study in Toulouse, Université Toulouse 1 Capitole, 1 Esplanade de l'Université, 31080, Toulouse Cedex 06, France
| | - Bram Tucker
- Department of Anthropology, University of Georgia, 250 Baldwin Hall, Athens, GA, 30602, USA
| | - Noa Lavi
- Department of Anthropology, University of Haifa, Abba Khoushy Ave 199, Mount Carmel, 3498838, Haifa, Israel
- Department of Anthropology, University College London, 14 Taviton Street, London, WC1H 0BW, UK
| | - Kate Ellis-Davies
- Department of Psychology, Swansea University, Singleton Park, Sketty, Swansea, SA2 8PP, UK
| | - Helen E Davis
- Department of Human Evolutionary Biology, Harvard, 11 Divinity Avenue, Cambridge, MA, 02138, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Wang Y, Qian M, Nabbijohn AN, Wen F, Fu G, Zuo B, VanderLaan DP. Culture influences the development of children's gender-related peer preferences: Evidence from China and Thailand. Dev Sci 2021; 25:e13221. [PMID: 34942036 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13221] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/16/2020] [Revised: 12/09/2021] [Accepted: 12/13/2021] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
Current understanding of how culture relates to the development of children's gender-related peer preferences is limited. To investigate the role of societal acceptance of gender nonconformity, this study compared children from China and Thailand. Unlike China and other cultures where the conceptualization of gender as binary is broadly accepted, individuals who identify as a nonbinary "third" sex/gender have been highly visible and tolerated in Thai society for at least several decades. Chinese and Thai 4- to 9-year-olds (N = 458) viewed vignettes of four hypothetical peers who varied on gender (i.e., boy vs. girl) and gender-typed toy play behavior (i.e., masculine vs. feminine), and were asked to give a friendship preference rating for each peer. Chinese, compared with Thai, children evidenced gender-related peer preferences that emerged earlier, remained more stable across age groups, and were relatively more biased against gender-nonconforming behavior. The only cultural similarity was in children's preference for peers who were of the same gender and/or displayed same-gender-typed behavior. Thus, while preference for peers who are of the same gender and/or display same-gender-typed behavior is common among children across cultures, the developmental onset and course of these preferences vary by culture. Moreover, societal acceptance of gender nonconformity might be key to limiting children's bias against gender-nonconforming peers.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yang Wang
- Central China Normal University, Wuhan, Hubei, China
| | - Miao Qian
- Department of Psychology, University of Detroit Mercy, Michigan, USA
| | - A Natisha Nabbijohn
- Department of Psychology, University of Toronto Mississauga, Mississauga, Ontario, Canada.,Department of Psychology, University of Guelph, Guelph, Ontario, Canada
| | - Fangfang Wen
- Central China Normal University, Wuhan, Hubei, China
| | - Genyue Fu
- School of Education, Hangzhou Normal University, Hangzhou, Zhejiang, China
| | - Bin Zuo
- Central China Normal University, Wuhan, Hubei, China
| | - Doug P VanderLaan
- Department of Psychology, University of Toronto Mississauga, Mississauga, Ontario, Canada.,Child and Youth Psychiatry, Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Davis JTM, Hines M. The Magnitude of Children's Gender-Related Toy Interests Has Remained Stable Over 50 Years of Research. ARCHIVES OF SEXUAL BEHAVIOR 2021; 50:749-751. [PMID: 33811280 DOI: 10.1007/s10508-021-01989-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/10/2021] [Revised: 03/15/2021] [Accepted: 03/15/2021] [Indexed: 06/12/2023]
Affiliation(s)
- Jac T M Davis
- Gender Development Research Centre, University of Cambridge, Free School Lane, Cambridge, CB2 3RQ, UK.
| | - Melissa Hines
- Gender Development Research Centre, University of Cambridge, Free School Lane, Cambridge, CB2 3RQ, UK
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Kung KTF. Preschool Gender-Typed Play Behavior Predicts Adolescent Gender-Typed Occupational Interests: A 10-Year Longitudinal Study. ARCHIVES OF SEXUAL BEHAVIOR 2021; 50:843-851. [PMID: 33751288 PMCID: PMC8035105 DOI: 10.1007/s10508-021-01976-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/08/2018] [Revised: 02/26/2021] [Accepted: 03/02/2021] [Indexed: 06/12/2023]
Abstract
There are significant gender differences in both play behavior and occupational interests. Play has been regarded as an important medium for development of skills and personal characteristics. Play may also influence subsequent preferences through social and cognitive processes involved in gender development. The present study investigated the association between gender-typed play behavior in early childhood and gender-typed occupational interests in early adolescence. Participants were drawn from a British longitudinal population study, the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. Participants were recruited based on their parent-reported gender-typed play behavior assessed at age 3.5 years. There were 66 masculine boys and 61 masculine girls, 82 feminine boys and 69 feminine girls, and 55 randomly selected control boys and 67 randomly selected control girls. At age 13 years, the participants were administered a questionnaire assessing their interest in gender-typed occupations. It was found that masculine children showed significantly more interest in male-typical occupations than did control or feminine children. Compared with control children, feminine children had marginally significantly lower interest in male-typical jobs. Masculine children also had significantly lower interest in female-typical jobs than did control or feminine children. The associations were not moderated by gender and were observed after taking into account sociodemographic background, parental occupations, and academic performance. The degree of gender-typed play shown by preschoolers can predict their occupational interests 10 years later following transition into adolescence. Childhood gender-typed play has occupational implications that transcend developmental stages.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Karson T F Kung
- Department of Psychology, University of Hong Kong, Pokfulam, Hong Kong.
- Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK.
- School of Psychology, University of Kent, Canterbury, UK.
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Wong WI, Tsui WBP, Siu TSC. Empathic accuracy of young boys and girls in ongoing parent-child interactions: Performance and (mis)perception. J Exp Child Psychol 2020; 203:105042. [PMID: 33302130 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2020.105042] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/04/2019] [Revised: 10/20/2020] [Accepted: 10/31/2020] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
Understanding others accurately is crucial in relationships and learning. Research shows that adults face challenges in empathic accuracy, that is, the ability to read the content of others' moment-to-moment mental states during interactions. Although young children possess some empathic understanding, the extent of their empathic accuracy is uncharted. Using a new SSP, 106 Chinese children aged 60 to 80 months (M = 70 months) were assessed on their ability to infer the mental states of adults in ongoing parent-child interactions. Replicating and extending extant findings on adults and adolescents, the children's inferences were found to be, at least computationally on a scale of .00 to 1.00, more often inaccurate than accurate regardless of the gender of the targets or participants (overall accuracy rate = .28). However, both the children and their primary caregivers overestimated the children's performance. In addition, although the primary caregivers expected girls to outperform boys, no gender difference in empathic accuracy was found when controlling for verbal fluency. Drawing on the findings of this first-ever application of the empathic accuracy paradigm in young children, the implications of empathic accuracy performance and misperceptions about such accuracy are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Wang Ivy Wong
- Department of Psychology, University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong; Gender Studies Program, Faculty of Social Science, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong; Department of Psychology, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong.
| | | | - Tik-Sze Carrey Siu
- Department of Early Childhood Education, The Education University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Spinner L, Cameron L, Ferguson HJ. Children's and parents' looking preferences to gender-typed objects: Evidence from eye tracking. J Exp Child Psychol 2020; 199:104892. [PMID: 32682100 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2020.104892] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/01/2019] [Revised: 05/05/2020] [Accepted: 05/06/2020] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
Abstract
Differences between children's and parents' implicit and explicit gender stereotypes were investigated in two experiments. For the first time, the visual world paradigm compared parents' and 7-8-year-old children's looking preferences toward masculine- and feminine-typed objects stereotypically associated with a story character's gender. In Experiment 1 participants listened to sentences that included a verb that inferred intentional action with an object (e.g., "Lilly/Alexander will play with the toy"), and in Experiment 2 the verb was replaced with a neutral verb (e.g., "Lilly/Alexander will trip over the toy"). A questionnaire assessed participants' explicit gender stereotype endorsement (and knowledge [Experiment 2]) of children's toys. Results revealed that parents and children displayed similar implicit stereotypes, but different explicit stereotypes, to one another. In Experiment 1, both children and parents displayed looking preferences toward the masculine-typed object when the story character was male and looking preferences toward the feminine-typed object when the character was female. No gender effects were found with a neutral verb in Experiment 2, reinforcing the impact of gender stereotypes on implicit processing and showing that the effects are not simply driven by gender stereotypic name-object associations. In the explicit measure, parents did not endorse the gender stereotypes related to toys but rather appeared to be egalitarian, whereas children's responses were gender stereotypic.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lauren Spinner
- School of Psychology, Keynes College, University of Kent, Canterbury, Kent CT2 7NP, UK.
| | - Lindsey Cameron
- School of Psychology, Keynes College, University of Kent, Canterbury, Kent CT2 7NP, UK
| | - Heather J Ferguson
- School of Psychology, Keynes College, University of Kent, Canterbury, Kent CT2 7NP, UK
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
King RA, Scott KE, Renno MP, Shutts K. Counterstereotyping can change children's thinking about boys' and girls' toy preferences. J Exp Child Psychol 2020; 191:104753. [PMID: 31841820 PMCID: PMC11370625 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2019.104753] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/21/2019] [Revised: 10/16/2019] [Accepted: 11/01/2019] [Indexed: 12/17/2022]
Abstract
Children think that peers prefer gender-stereotypical toys over gender-counterstereotypical toys. These beliefs can limit children's exploration of gender-counterstereotypical behaviors and prevent the development of broad skills and interests. The current research tested interventions to combat gender-based stereotyping about toys among children aged 4 to 7 years (N = 373). Across four experiments featuring seven different intervention versions, participants saw videos where a teacher provided counterstereotypical messages about toy preferences (e.g., "boys like dolls," "girls like trucks"). The phrasing of the messages (e.g., generic vs. demonstrative) and accompanying photographs (e.g., images of many children vs. one child) varied across experiments. In all intervention conditions, participants made more counterstereotypical (and fewer stereotypical) predictions about peers' toy preferences after viewing intervention videos; differences in the phrasing of the intervention message (e.g., "boys like dolls" vs. "this kid likes dolls") had little effect on participants' predictions. In Experiment 4, an intervention condition containing generic phrasing and gender noun labels (e.g., "boys like dolls") changed children's selection of toys for peers. This research provides promise for counterstereotyping as an impactful and easily implementable intervention strategy.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rachel Ann King
- University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI 53706, USA; Cornell University, Ithaca, NY 14853, USA.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
8
|
Zhang H, Stapleton CM, Hsueh Y, Cohen R. Three pathways from anger dysregulation to lower social status among chinese boys. INTERPERSONA: AN INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL ON PERSONAL RELATIONSHIPS 2019. [DOI: 10.5964/ijpr.v13i2.380] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/20/2022] Open
Abstract
During middle childhood, Chinese boys are particularly at risk to develop both externalizing (e.g., overt aggression) and internalizing behavioral problems (e.g., social withdrawal). A possible contributor to these problems is that boys cannot regulate their anger very well. Inability to manage anger may cause a particular social challenge for Chinese boys. Open expression of anger may be prohibited by prevailing Chinese cultural norms, because it emphasizes individuality over harmony. But anger is a socially disengaging emotion which works against social harmony. This situation requires Chinese boys to manage and express anger appropriately in social interactions. Based on the hierarchical model of social relationships and the three trends of human interactions, this study examined three pathways—aggression, social withdrawal, and sociability-leadership—that lead from Chinese boys’ anger dysregulation to their lower social status among peers at school. Participants of this study were 267 boys in Grades 3-6 from an elementary school in urban China. A self-report questionnaire of anger dysregulation was used to evaluate how often Chinese boys express their anger in dysregulated ways (e.g., attacking things or people). Peer nominations were used to measure children’s overt aggression (moving against peers), social withdrawal (moving away from peers), and sociability-leadership (moving toward peers). Social status was assessed by a sociometric measure which evaluates the degree to which children were liked by their classmates. Results showed that boys’ anger dysregulation was negatively associated with their social status. Moreover, aggression, social withdrawal, and social skills fully mediated this association. This study enriches our understanding of the mechanisms linking anger dysregulation to lower social status and provides practical implications to help Chinese boys improve social and emotional functioning in middle childhood.
Collapse
|
9
|
Kent C, Cordier R, Joosten A, Wilkes-Gillan S, Bundy A. Can I join in? Multiple case study investigation of play performance generalisation for children with autism spectrum disorder from dyad to triad. Aust Occup Ther J 2019; 67:199-209. [PMID: 31823379 DOI: 10.1111/1440-1630.12635] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 11/16/2019] [Indexed: 01/04/2023]
Abstract
INTRODUCTION Children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) have difficulties with play, social interaction with peers and generalisation of intervention outcomes. The Ultimate Guide to Play, Language and Friendship (PLF) has demonstrated effectiveness in improving play performance of children with ASD and their typically developing (TD) peers. The aim of this investigation was to examine the changes in play performance when an additional TD child is added to an existing dyad of a child with ASD and a TD playmate to inform future delivery and adaptations of the intervention. METHODS Participants in this multiple case study design were five children with ASD and their TD peer who completed a dyad intervention as part of a randomised control trial investigation of the PLF and an additional TD peer who joined the play dyad. A trained occupational therapist delivered an adapted version of the PLF to the triad over four clinic sessions. An independent rater scored each child (N = 15) on The Test of Playfulness at pre- and post-triad intervention. Line graphs were used to examine case data and compare to dyad play performance and patterns of interaction. RESULTS Four of the five children with ASD generalised their play performance from the dyad to the triad social environment. However, the triad intervention did not demonstrate improvements in play performance. The play performance scores for the children with ASD and their TD peers were variable and demonstrated changes in their play pattern from the dyad to the triad. CONCLUSION This investigation delivered preliminary evidence of play performance generalisation from a dyad to a triad with TD peers for children with ASD. Careful consideration of characteristics of all playmates is recommended for delivering the intervention to support play performance of children with ASD.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Cally Kent
- School of Occupational Therapy and Social Work, Curtin University, Perth, WA, USA
| | - Reinie Cordier
- School of Occupational Therapy and Social Work, Curtin University, Perth, WA, USA.,School Department of Special Needs Education, Faculty of Educational Sciences, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
| | - Annette Joosten
- School of Allied Health, Australian Catholic University, Melbourne, Vic., Australia
| | | | - Anita Bundy
- Department of Occupational Therapy, Colorado State University, Fort Collins, CO, USA
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Hukkelberg S, Ogden T. What is social competence? An investigation into the concept among children with antisocial behaviours. EMOTIONAL AND BEHAVIOURAL DIFFICULTIES 2019. [DOI: 10.1080/13632752.2019.1687168] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Silje Hukkelberg
- NORCE Norwegian Research Centre, Norwegian Center for Child Behavioral Development (NUBU), Majorstuen, Norway
| | - Terje Ogden
- NORCE Norwegian Research Centre, Norwegian Center for Child Behavioral Development (NUBU), Majorstuen, Norway
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Lew-Levy S, Boyette AH, Crittenden AN, Hewlett BS, Lamb ME. Gender-Typed and Gender-Segregated Play Among Tanzanian Hadza and Congolese BaYaka Hunter-Gatherer Children and Adolescents. Child Dev 2019; 91:1284-1301. [PMID: 31553073 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.13306] [Citation(s) in RCA: 31] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
Few data exist on gender-typed and gender-segregated play in hunter-gatherer societies, despite their unique demographic and cultural features which may influence children's gendered play. Using naturalistic observations of Hadza (N = 46, 41% female) and BaYaka (N = 65, 48% female) hunter-gatherer 3- to 18-year-olds from Tanzania and the Republic of Congo, we showed that access to playmates was negatively associated with playing in mixed-gender groups. Young boys did not engage in more rough-and-tumble play than girls, but adolescent boys participated in this type of play more than adolescent girls. Children were also more likely to participate in work-themed play which conformed to gender norms within their society. Findings are discussed within the context of gendered division of labor, child autonomy, and demography.
Collapse
|
12
|
Wong WI, Yeung SP. Early Gender Differences in Spatial and Social Skills and Their Relations to Play and Parental Socialization in Children from Hong Kong. ARCHIVES OF SEXUAL BEHAVIOR 2019; 48:1589-1602. [PMID: 31123945 DOI: 10.1007/s10508-019-1415-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/23/2017] [Revised: 01/31/2019] [Accepted: 02/02/2019] [Indexed: 05/26/2023]
Abstract
Children's play preferences are highly gender-typed. At the same time, much research revolves around spatial and social skills that sometimes show male and female advantages, respectively. There is evidence that play with masculine toys is associated with better spatial skills and emerging evidence suggests that play with feminine toys is associated with better social skills. However, several research gaps limit current knowledge on these aspects of gender development. First, the study of childhood gender development has been largely Eurocentric; second, the link between gender-typed play and social skills development is not well supported. We tested 644 5-year-old Hong Kong Chinese children on five gender-typed skills, play preferences, and parental gender socialization. The pattern of gender differences was remarkably similar to those in the West. Boys preferred masculine toys more than girls and were better at mental transformation and targeting accuracy, while girls preferred feminine (and neutral) toys more than boys and were better at empathy and were less aggressive, although there was no significant gender difference in comforting skill. There was little evidence that these gender differences varied with socioeconomic status (parental income and education). Play correlated with some outcomes in expected ways. This is in contrast to parents' gender socialization, which showed some expected differences by child gender but minimally correlated with children's skills. These findings shed light on the generalizability of current knowledge on early gender differences and may facilitate gender developmental research outside the West. Although the study did not test the direction of effects, they substantiate the growing discourse on gender-typed play as an important learning mechanism.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Wang Ivy Wong
- Department of Psychology, University of Hong Kong, Pokfulam, Hong Kong.
- Gender Studies Program, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Sino Building, Shatin, N.T., Hong Kong.
| | - Sui Ping Yeung
- Department of Psychology, University of Hong Kong, Pokfulam, Hong Kong
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Macalister HE, Ethridge K. The effect of gendered presentation on children's food choice. Appetite 2018; 135:28-32. [PMID: 30583009 DOI: 10.1016/j.appet.2018.12.039] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/04/2018] [Revised: 12/20/2018] [Accepted: 12/21/2018] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
Abstract
Children at the peak age of cognitive gender rigidity (age 3-6 years) demonstrate preference for gender-typed activities, toys, and peers. This study explores whether this preference extends to gender-typed food. A total of 212 Virginia preschool and elementary school children performed a card sort of food images and chose between snacks with gender-consistent and gender-inconsistent packaging. Chi-squared analyses revealed that children were more likely to choose a snack in gender-consistent packaging, even when a tastier snack in gender-inconsistent packaging was available. Further, children consistently sorted pink-frosted cupcakes for girls and blue-frosted cupcakes for boys; and a subset of children who imposed gender onto other foods did so consistently (e.g., hamburgers for boys). Consistent with other evidence of children's gender rigidity, these findings support cognitive developmental theories of gender and add a gender-based explanation of children's food choice.
Collapse
|
14
|
|
15
|
Weisgram ES, Bruun ST. Predictors of Gender-Typed Toy Purchases by Prospective Parents and Mothers: The Roles of Childhood Experiences and Gender Attitudes. SEX ROLES 2018. [DOI: 10.1007/s11199-018-0928-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/29/2022]
|
16
|
Spinner L, Cameron L, Calogero R. Peer Toy Play as a Gateway to Children's Gender Flexibility: The Effect of (Counter)Stereotypic Portrayals of Peers in Children's Magazines. SEX ROLES 2018; 79:314-328. [PMID: 30147223 PMCID: PMC6096659 DOI: 10.1007/s11199-017-0883-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 33] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
Abstract
Extensive evidence has documented the gender stereotypic content of children’s media, and media is recognized as an important socializing agent for young children. Yet, the precise impact of children’s media on the endorsement of gender-typed attitudes and behaviors has received less scholarly attention. We investigated the impact of stereotypic and counter-stereotypic peers pictured in children’s magazines on children’s gender flexibility around toy play and preferences, playmate choice, and social exclusion behavior (n = 82, age 4–7 years-old). British children were randomly assigned to view a picture of a peer-age boy and girl in a magazine playing with either a gender stereotypic or counter-stereotypic toy. In the stereotypic condition, the pictured girl was shown with a toy pony and the pictured boy was shown with a toy car; these toys were reversed in the counter-stereotypic condition. Results revealed significantly greater gender flexibility around toy play and playmate choices among children in the counter-stereotypic condition compared to the stereotypic condition, and boys in the stereotypic condition were more accepting of gender-based exclusion than were girls. However, there was no difference in children’s own toy preferences between the stereotypic and counter-stereotypic condition, with children preferring more gender-typed toys overall. Implications of the findings for media, education, and parenting practices are discussed, and the potential for counter-stereotypic media portrayals of toy play to shape the gender socialization of young children is explored.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lauren Spinner
- 1School of Psychology, University of Kent, Canterbury, Kent CT2 7NP UK
| | - Lindsey Cameron
- 1School of Psychology, University of Kent, Canterbury, Kent CT2 7NP UK
| | - Rachel Calogero
- 2Department of Psychology, University of Western Ontario, London, ON Canada
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Kollmayer M, Schultes MT, Schober B, Hodosi T, Spiel C. Parents' Judgments about the Desirability of Toys for Their Children: Associations with Gender Role Attitudes, Gender-typing of Toys, and Demographics. SEX ROLES 2018; 79:329-341. [PMID: 30147224 PMCID: PMC6096664 DOI: 10.1007/s11199-017-0882-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 30] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
Parental toy selection and responses to toy play are important factors in children's gender socialization. Reinforcing play with same-gender-typed toys guides children's activities and limits their action repertoires in accordance with gender stereotypes. A survey of 324 Austrian parents of three- to six-year-old children was conducted to investigate parents' judgments about the desirability of different types of toys for their children and how these judgements relate to parents' gender-typing of toys, gender role attitudes, and demographics (age, education, gender). Results show that parents rated same-gender-typed and gender-neutral toys as more desirable for their children than cross-gender-typed toys. The traditionalism of parents' gender role attitudes was not associated with their desirability judgments of same-gender-typed toys, but was negatively related to their desirability judgments of cross-gender-typed toys. This indicates that egalitarian parents permit a greater range of interests and behaviors in their children than traditional parents do. Younger parents, parents with lower educational levels, and fathers reported more traditional gender role attitudes than did older parents, parents with higher educational levels, and mothers. However, no differences based on age, educational level or gender were found in parents' judgments of toy desirability. The present study demonstrates that parents' judgments about the desirability of toys for their children do not accurately reflect their gender role attitudes. This finding highlights the importance of simultaneously investigating different aspects of parents' gender-related attitudes in order to gain a better understanding of parental transmission of gender stereotypes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marlene Kollmayer
- 1Department of Applied Psychology: Work, Education and Economy, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
| | - Marie-Therese Schultes
- 2Gillings School of Global Public Health, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, NC USA
| | - Barbara Schober
- 1Department of Applied Psychology: Work, Education and Economy, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
| | - Tanja Hodosi
- 1Department of Applied Psychology: Work, Education and Economy, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
| | - Christiane Spiel
- 1Department of Applied Psychology: Work, Education and Economy, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Yeung SP, Wong WI. Gender Labels on Gender-Neutral Colors: Do they Affect Children’s Color Preferences and Play Performance? SEX ROLES 2018. [DOI: 10.1007/s11199-017-0875-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
|
19
|
Todd BK, Fischer RA, Di Costa S, Roestorf A, Harbour K, Hardiman P, Barry JA. Sex differences in children's toy preferences: A systematic review, meta-regression, and meta-analysis. INFANT AND CHILD DEVELOPMENT 2017. [DOI: 10.1002/icd.2064] [Citation(s) in RCA: 91] [Impact Index Per Article: 13.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Brenda K. Todd
- Department of Psychology; City, University of London; London UK
| | | | - Steven Di Costa
- Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience; University College London; London UK
| | - Amanda Roestorf
- Department of Psychology; City, University of London; London UK
| | | | - Paul Hardiman
- Institute of Women's Health; University College London; London UK
| | - John A. Barry
- Institute of Women's Health; University College London; London UK
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Farr RH, Bruun ST, Doss KM, Patterson CJ. Children’s Gender-Typed Behavior from Early to Middle Childhood in Adoptive Families with Lesbian, Gay, and Heterosexual Parents. SEX ROLES 2017. [DOI: 10.1007/s11199-017-0812-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
|
21
|
|
22
|
Dinella LM, Weisgram ES, Fulcher M. Children's Gender-Typed Toy Interests: Does Propulsion Matter? ARCHIVES OF SEXUAL BEHAVIOR 2017; 46:1295-1305. [PMID: 27896564 DOI: 10.1007/s10508-016-0901-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/16/2015] [Revised: 11/09/2016] [Accepted: 11/10/2016] [Indexed: 06/06/2023]
Abstract
Children's toy play is at the foundation of child development. However, gender differentiation in early play experiences may result in gender differences in cognitive abilities, social interactions, and vocational choices. We investigated gender-typing of toys and toys' propulsive properties (e.g., wheels, forward motion) as possible factors impacting children's toy interests, perceptions of other children's interests, and children's actual toy choices during free play. In Studies 1 and 2, 82 preschool children (42 boys, 40 girls; mean age = 4.90 years) were asked to report their interest and perceptions of other children's interests in toys. In Study 1, masculine, feminine, and neutral toys with and without propulsive properties were presented. Children reported greater interest in gender-typed toys and neutral toys compared to cross-gender-typed toys. In Study 2, unfamiliar, neutral toys with and without propulsive properties were presented. Propulsive properties did not affect children's interest across both studies. Study 3 was an observational study that assessed toy preferences among 42 preschool children (21 males, 21 females, mean age = 4.49 years) during a play session with masculine, feminine, and neutral toys with and without propulsive properties. Gender-typed toy preferences were less apparent than expected, with children showing high interest in neutral toys, and girls playing with a wide variety of masculine, feminine, and neutral toys. Gender differences in interest for toys with propulsion properties were not evident. Overall, gender differences in children's interest in toys as a function of propulsion properties were not found in the three experiments within this study.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lisa M Dinella
- Department of Psychology, Monmouth University, West Long Branch, NJ, 07764, USA.
| | - Erica S Weisgram
- Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin-Stevens Point, Stevens Point, WI, USA
| | - Megan Fulcher
- Department of Psychology, Washington and Lee University, Lexington, VA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Dinella LM. Halloween Costume Choices: Reflections of Gender Development in Early Childhood. The Journal of Genetic Psychology 2017; 178:165-178. [PMID: 28402183 DOI: 10.1080/00221325.2017.1295223] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
Abstract
The author examined whether preschoolers' Halloween costume choices reflect their gender development. The sample consisted of 110 (53 boys, 57 girls) infant through preschool-aged participants, and 1 parent of each child. Both observational methodologies and parent-report surveys were used to assess the gender-typed nature of children's Halloween costumes, information about the Halloween costume choice process, and about the children's gender development. Boys' costumes were more masculine and girls' costumes were more feminine. Younger children's costumes were consistently less gender typed than the older children's costumes were. Children whose parents chose their Halloween costumes for them had Halloween costumes that were less gender typed than did children who were involved in the Halloween costume decision-making process. Moreover, children's gender-typed play and desire to wear gender-stereotyped clothes were related to the gender stereotyped nature of their Halloween costume. Unexpectedly, gender typicality, a dimension of gender identity, was not related to children's Halloween costume choices. Overall, the findings support that children's Halloween costume choice is an indicator of children's gender development processes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lisa M Dinella
- a Department of Psychology , Monmouth University , West Long Branch, NJ , USA
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Kollmayer M, Schober B, Spiel C. Gender stereotypes in education: Development, consequences, and interventions. EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2016. [DOI: 10.1080/17405629.2016.1193483] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Marlene Kollmayer
- Faculty of Psychology, Department of Applied Psychology: Work, Education, Economy, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
| | - Barbara Schober
- Faculty of Psychology, Department of Applied Psychology: Work, Education, Economy, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
| | - Christiane Spiel
- Faculty of Psychology, Department of Applied Psychology: Work, Education, Economy, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
| |
Collapse
|