1
|
Abstract
Recent studies have suggested that temporal dynamics rather than symmetrical motion-direction contribute to mirror-symmetry perception. Here we investigate temporal aspects of symmetry perception and implicitly, its temporal flexibility and limitations, by examining how symmetrical pattern elements are combined over time. Stimuli were dynamic dot-patterns consisting of either an on-going alternation of two images (sustained stimulus presentation) or just two images each presented once (transient stimulus presentation) containing different amounts of symmetry about the vertical axis. We varied the duration of the two images under five temporal-arrangement conditions: (a) whole patterns in which a symmetric pattern alternated with a noise pattern; (b) delayed halves—the halves of the symmetric and noise patterns were presented with temporal delay; (c) matched-pairs—two alternating images each containing equal amounts of symmetrical matched-pairs; (d) delayed matched-pairs—the same as arrangement (c), but with matched-pairs presented with delay; and (e) static—both images presented simultaneously as one. We found increased sensitivity in sustained compared to transient stimulus presentations and with synchronous compared to delayed matched-pairs stimuli. For the delayed conditions, sensitivity decreased gradually with longer image durations (>60 ms), prominently for the transient stimulus presentations. We conclude that spatial correlations across-the-symmetry-midline can be integrated over time (∼120 ms), and symmetry mechanisms can tolerate temporal delays between symmetric dot-pairs of up to ∼60 ms.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rebecca J Sharman
- University of Stirling, Department of Psychology, Stirling, Scotland, UK
| | | | - Elena Gheorghiu
- University of Stirling, Department of Psychology, Stirling, Scotland, UK
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
The effects of the binocular disparity differences between targets and maskers on visual search. Atten Percept Psychophys 2016; 79:459-472. [PMID: 27943121 DOI: 10.3758/s13414-016-1252-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
A visual search for targets is facilitated when the target objects are on a different depth plane than other masking objects cluttering the scene. The ability of observers to determine whether one of four letters presented stereoscopically at four symmetrically located positions on the fixation plane differed from the other three was assessed when the target letters were masked by other randomly positioned and oriented letters appearing on the same depth plane as the target letters, or in front, or behind it. Three additional control maskers, derived from the letter maskers, were also presented on the same three depth planes: (1) random-phase maskers (same spectral amplitude composition as the letter masker but with the phase spectrum randomized); (2) random-pixel maskers (the locations of the letter maskers' pixel amplitudes were randomized); (3) letter-fragment maskers (the same letters as in the letter masker but broken up into fragments). Performance improved with target duration when the target-letter plane was in front of the letter-masker plane, but not when the target letters were on the same plane as the masker, or behind it. A comparison of the results for the four different kinds of maskers indicated that maskers consisting of recognizable objects (letters or letter fragments) interfere more with search and comparison judgments than do visual noise maskers having the same spatial frequency profile and contrast. In addition, performance was poorer for letter maskers than for letter-masker fragments, suggesting that the letter maskers interfered more with performance than the letter-fragment maskers because of the lexical activity they elicit.
Collapse
|
3
|
Sachtler WLB, Gillam B. The Stereoscopic Sliver: A Comparison of Duration Thresholds for Fully Stereoscopic and Unmatched Versions. Perception 2016; 36:135-44. [PMID: 17357711 DOI: 10.1068/p5625] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
Abstract
Just as positional disparities of image features seen with both eyes provide depth information, the presence of an area visible to one eye but not the other within a binocularly viewed scene can indicate an occlusion at a depth discontinuity,. The close geometrical association between these two kinds of cues suggests they may both be exploited by stereopsis. To investigate this, we developed a novel binocular stimulus entirely lacking in classical disparity that contains an unmatched vertical sliver which elicits a warping of the surrounding surface to accommodate a depth discontinuity. We measured depth-discrimination performance at a range of stimulus durations, correcting for variations in stimulus visibility, to characterise the decline of the efficacy of the depth signal with limited integration time. Results show a close correspondence of performance for similar stimuli with unmatched features and classical binocular disparity across a sixtyfold range of viewing durations, supporting the notion of a close association between the two types of cues in human stereopsis. Control experiments excluded simple eye-of-origin cues and long-range false matches as explanatory factors.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- W L Ben Sachtler
- School of Psychology, University of New South Wales, Sydney, NSW 2052, Australia.
| | | |
Collapse
|
4
|
Arnold DH, Lipp OV. Discrepant integration times for upright and inverted faces. Perception 2012; 40:989-99. [PMID: 22132513 DOI: 10.1068/p6955] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/17/2022]
Abstract
Judgments of upright faces tend to be more rapid than judgments of inverted faces. This is consistent with encoding at different rates via discrepant mechanisms, or via a common mechanism that is more sensitive to upright input. However, to the best of our knowledge no previous study of facial coding speed has tried to equate sensitivity across the characteristics under investigation (eg emotional expression, facial gender, or facial orientation). Consequently we cannot tell whether different decision speeds result from mechanisms that accrue information at different rates, or because facial images can differ in the amount of information they make available. To address this, we examined temporal integration times, the times across which information is accrued toward a perceptual decision. We examined facial gender and emotional expressions. We first identified image pairs that could be differentiated on 80% of trials with protracted presentations (1 s). We then presented these images at a range of brief durations to determine how rapidly performance plateaued, which is indicative of integration time. For upright faces gender was associated with a protracted integration relative to expression judgments. This difference was eliminated by inversion, with both gender and expression judgments associated with a common, rapid, integration time. Overall, our data suggest that upright facial gender and expression are encoded via distinct processes and that inversion does not just result in impaired sensitivity. Rather, inversion caused gender judgments, which had been associated with a protracted integration, to become associated with a more rapid process.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Derek H Arnold
- School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, St Lucia, Queensland 4072, Australia.
| | | |
Collapse
|
5
|
Arnold DH, Wilcock P. Cortical processing and perceived timing. Proc Biol Sci 2007; 274:2331-6. [PMID: 17623637 PMCID: PMC2211513 DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2007.0576] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/20/2007] [Revised: 06/13/2007] [Accepted: 06/14/2007] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
As of yet, it is unclear how we determine relative perceived timing. One controversial suggestion is that timing perception might be related to when analyses are completed in the cortex of the brain. An alternate proposal suggests that perceived timing is instead related to the point in time at which cortical analyses commence. Accordingly, timing illusions should not occur owing to cortical analyses, but they could occur if there were differential delays between signals reaching cortex. Resolution of this controversy therefore requires that the contributions of cortical processing be isolated from the influence of subcortical activity. Here, we have done this by using binocular disparity changes, which are known to be detected via analyses that originate in cortex. We find that observers require longer stimulus exposures to detect small, relative to larger, disparity changes; observers are slower to react to smaller disparity changes and observers misperceive smaller disparity changes as being perceptually delayed. Interestingly, disparity magnitude influenced perceived timing more dramatically than it did stimulus change detection. Our data therefore suggest that perceived timing is both influenced by cortical processing and is shaped by sensory analyses subsequent to those that are minimally necessary for stimulus change perception.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Derek H Arnold
- School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, St Lucia, Queensland 4072, Australia.
| | | |
Collapse
|
6
|
Hess RF, Wilcox LM. Stereo dynamics are not scale-dependent. Vision Res 2006; 46:1911-23. [PMID: 16469355 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2005.12.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/15/2005] [Revised: 11/17/2005] [Accepted: 12/01/2005] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
Abstract
The experiments reported here focus on the temporal dynamics of stereopsis in an effort to shed light on how low level mechanisms might contribute to the execution of coarse-to-fine processing in the human stereo system. Because previous studies have used a variety of stimuli and configurations, we assess the effect of exposure duration on stereo thresholds using band-limited Gabor patches for a range of stimulus configurations. In preliminary studies, we found that the best stereo sensitivity-spatial frequency relationship was obtained when using configurations in which the size and target-reference spacing were consistent with spatially scaled stimuli. Sub-optimal stereo sensitivity as a function of spatial frequency was observed when the size and separation were fixed. Further, we found that the temporal properties of stereopsis were consistently sustained in nature irrespective of the stimulus spatial frequency content. This latter finding suggests that if coarse-to-fine stereo processing does occur it does not follow as a consequence of the dynamics of low-level disparity transduction.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Robert F Hess
- McGill Vision Research, Department of Ophthalmology, McGill University, Montreal, Que., Canada.
| | | |
Collapse
|
7
|
Becker S, Bowd C, Shorter S, King K, Patterson R. Occlusion contributes to temporal processing differences between crossed and uncrossed stereopsis in random-dot displays. Vision Res 1999; 39:331-9. [PMID: 10326139 DOI: 10.1016/s0042-6989(98)00110-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
Stereoscopic depth discrimination was investigated in crossed and uncrossed directions using stimuli defined by binocular disparity differences embedded in dynamic random-dot stereograms. Across three experiments, fixation was directed to a point on the display screen (which placed crossed stimuli in front of and uncrossed stimuli behind, the background dots of the stereogram), to a point in front of the display screen (which placed both crossed and uncrossed stimuli in front of the background dots), and to a point behind the display screen (which placed both crossed and uncrossed stimuli behind the background dots). Results showed that depth discrimination was always good when the stimuli appeared in front of the background dots of the stereogram, whereas discrimination was always poor when the stimuli appeared behind the background dots. These results suggest that differences between crossed and uncrossed stereopsis as reported in past research arose, in part, from effects related to occlusion.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- S Becker
- Department of Psychology, Washington State University, Pullman 99164-4820, USA
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
8
|
Abstract
We measured sensitivity to binocular correlation in dynamic random-dot stereograms that defined moving sinusoidal gratings-in-depth. At a range of spatial frequencies and drift rates we established sensitivity by adding Gaussian distributed disparity noise to the modulation of disparity that defined a cyclopean grating, and finding the noise amplitude that rendered the grating just detectable. This permitted correlation thresholds to be measured at a range of suprathreshold disparity amplitudes. Spatial requirements for binocular correlation depend little on temporal frequency, and vice versa. This suggests that binocular correlation mechanisms can be characterized by independent spatial and temporal sensitivity functions. The temporal frequency function has a low pass characteristic. Sensitivity declines above about 1 c/sec, reaching its limit at 4-8 c/sec. The spatial characteristic depends greatly on the amplitude of disparity modulation, changing from band pass at low amplitude to low pass at high amplitude. The maximum resolvable spatial frequency is 4-6 c/deg, but declines sharply for relatively high amplitudes. The interaction between amplitude and spatial frequency cannot be explained by fixed high or low limits on detectable disparity gradients.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- M J Lankheet
- Center for Visual Science, University of Rochester, NY 14627, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
9
|
Patterson R, Cayko R, Short GL, Flanagan R, Moe L, Taylor E, Day P. Temporal integration differences between crossed and uncrossed stereoscopic mechanisms. PERCEPTION & PSYCHOPHYSICS 1995; 57:891-7. [PMID: 7651812 DOI: 10.3758/bf03206803] [Citation(s) in RCA: 31] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/26/2023]
Abstract
In this study, we investigated temporal integration of disparity information for crossed and uncrossed stereopsis. Across three experiments, exposure duration thresholds were measured for stereoscopic stimuli created from dynamic random-dot stereograms. In Experiment 1, an investigation of disparity detection showed that detection thresholds were equal for the crossed and uncrossed directions. In Experiment 2, an examination of duration limits for depth perception showed that critical durations were lower, and depth more veridical, for crossed depth than for uncrossed depth. In Experiment 3, an investigation of depth discrimination revealed that discrimination thresholds were lower for crossed depth than for uncrossed depth. These results suggest that crossed and uncrossed mechanisms differ in terms of their temporal integration properties at processing levels involving the computation and discrimination of depth.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- R Patterson
- Department of Psychology, Washington State University, Pullman 99164-4820, USA
| | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
10
|
Tyler CW, Kontsevich LL. Mechanisms of stereoscopic processing: stereoattention and surface perception in depth reconstruction. Perception 1995; 24:127-53. [PMID: 7617422 DOI: 10.1068/p240127] [Citation(s) in RCA: 58] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/26/2023]
Abstract
Consideration of the range of phenomena from studies of human stereopsis suggests that a five-stage model is required to provide a complete account of the processes involved, within which any stereoattention mechanism must operate. The information from the disparity field of the optical projections to the two eyes (stage 1) goes to a set of parallel Keplerian arrays of disparity detectors, each array selective for a different spatiotemporal property of the visual images (stage 2). Global interactions produce a cyclopean depth image that is cleaned of the spurious ghost images in the Keplerian arrays (stage 3) and that may then be processed for its (hypercyclopean) from elements (stage 4). Finally, there must be a stage of integration of the stereoscopic depth cues with monocular and kinesthetic depth cues to form the overall map of perceived distance (stage 5). The fact that multiple cyclopean surfaces may be perceived as transparent implies that the stereoscopic system is not limited by a singular-surface constraint. However, it is unclear whether multiple surfaces can be seen simultaneously or whether only one surface is seen at a time by a selective-attention process, with the others perceived as a purely inchoate (qualitative) depth impression. New experiments on cueing of ambiguous stereocorrugations by singular flat planes suggest that selective stereoattention is a powerful mechanism. In fact, the results show that attention can be focused not just in horopteral planes but in a variety of depth configurations. Moreover, this attention focus may act as a tracking mechanism to allow perception of smooth cyclopean stereomotion, which has a frequency response up to approximately 5 Hz (in contrast to the approximately 15 Hz limit for detecting planar disparity shifts as jerky appearance and disappearance effects). Finally, the spatial limits of stereosurface reconstruction are explored with cyclopean targets to show some interesting asymmetries of the surface-wrapping process that may represent object-oriented constraints on depth reconstruction.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- C W Tyler
- Smith-Kettlewell Eye Research Institute, San Francisco, CA 94115, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
11
|
Uttal WR, Davis NS, Welke C. Stereoscopic perception with brief exposures. PERCEPTION & PSYCHOPHYSICS 1994; 56:599-604. [PMID: 7991356 DOI: 10.3758/bf03206955] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/28/2023]
Abstract
In this report we describe the results of an experiment in which we demonstrated that a powerful and compelling stereoscopic experience is elicited with very brief (< 1 msec) stimulus durations. The observers were highly successful in recognizing briefly flashed, stereoscopic, random-dot surfaces in the absence of monocular contours. The results are shown to be closely related to the range of depths for any stimulus form; however, the recognition thresholds were nonmonotonic as a function of disparity. Previous investigators have disagreed about the existence of a temporal threshold for stereopsis. We believe that prior findings suggesting that stereopsis cannot occur at short exposure durations are probably due to inadequate control of fixation disparity. Therefore, there is poor dichoptic image registration when a stereoscopic stimulus is presented. The present results also raise difficulties for any theory of stereopsis that requires eye movements.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- W R Uttal
- Department of IMSE, Arizona State University, Tempe 85287
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
12
|
Stevenson SB, Cormack LK, Schor CM. Hyperacuity, superresolution and gap resolution in human stereopsis. Vision Res 1989; 29:1597-605. [PMID: 2635483 DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(89)90141-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 40] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/01/2023]
Abstract
Different types of stereoscopic acuity were studied with tasks adapted from studies of visual direction acuity. Dynamic, random-element stereograms portraying multiple surfaces in depth and a temporal 2AFC procedure were used for all measurements. The three tasks required detection of a depth offset (Hyperacuity task), a depth-axis thickening (Superresolution task), and a depth-axis gap between surfaces (Gap Resolution task). Thresholds for the three tasks were on the order of 3 sec arc, 30 sec arc and 200 sec arc of retinal disparity, respectively. These results are comparable to those for the analogous visual direction tasks on which they were patterned, suggesting that the underlying judgments involved are similar. Results are used to estimate the intrinsic noise of horizontal disparity processing.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- S B Stevenson
- School of Optometry, University of California, Berkeley 94720
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
13
|
Dynamic stereo displays for research on the recovery of three-dimensional structure. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 1986. [DOI: 10.3758/bf03201424] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
|
14
|
White KD, Odom JV. Temporal integration in global stereopsis. PERCEPTION & PSYCHOPHYSICS 1985; 37:139-44. [PMID: 4011367 DOI: 10.3758/bf03202848] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/08/2023]
|
15
|
Shimojo S, Nakajima Y. Adaptation to the reversal of binocular depth cues: effects of wearing left-right reversing spectacles on stereoscopic depth perception. Perception 1981; 10:391-402. [PMID: 7335438 DOI: 10.1068/p100391] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/24/2023]
Abstract
The principle of stereopsis, that crossed disparity causes a convex perception and uncrossed disparity a concave one, has for a long time been considered to depend on a very rigid neural mechanism not affected by experience. Experiments are reported here which show that this relationship between disparity and perceived depth can be reversed by experience. An observer wore a pair of left-right reversing spectacles continuously for nine days. The spectacles also reversed the relation between the direction of perceived depth and the direction of binocular depth cues, ie disparity and vergence. For a period starting two days before wearing the spectacles and continuing until seventy-nine days after their removal the observer was examined with a haploscope and an electrooculograph. All the stereoscopic experiments were carried out without spectacles in order to examine some aftereffects of wearing spectacles. For the stereograms with linear contours not only the adaptive reversal of the relation between disparity and perceived depth, but also some abnormal depth perceptions and long-lasting aftereffects were found. For Julesz's random-dot stereograms, however, in which contours can be seen only after binocular combination, no adaptive change or reversal occurred. These results suggest that the process of stereopsis consists of two concurrent subprocesses.
Collapse
|
16
|
Harwerth RS, Boltz RL. Stereopsis in monkeys using random dot stereograms: the effect of viewing duration. Vision Res 1979; 19:985-91. [PMID: 119348 DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(79)90223-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 46] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/13/2022]
|
17
|
|
18
|
|
19
|
Michaels CF, Carello C, Shapiro B, Steitz C. An onset to onset rule for binocular integration in the Mach-Dvorak illusion. Vision Res 1977; 17:1107-13. [PMID: 595422 DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(77)90018-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/23/2022]
|
20
|
|
21
|
|