1
|
Robinson DA. Neurophysiology, pathology and models of rapid eye movements. PROGRESS IN BRAIN RESEARCH 2022; 267:287-317. [PMID: 35074059 DOI: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2021.10.014] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
Abstract
This chapter discusses the premotor neural mechanisms that control horizontal saccadic eye movements. Oculomotoneurons carry a pulse-step signal that underlies the pulse-step force driving the overdamped plant. The pulse and step are both generated by a common signal, arising from medium-lead burst neurons in the pons. Their burst signal encodes saccadic eye velocity, while the number of spikes in the burst relates to the saccade amplitude. The step component, which encodes the eye position, is obtained by neural integration of the burst. Several oculomotor neural disorders can be explained by impairments in the binocular push-pull organization of this pulse-step mechanism. Plasticity of the pulse-step control, e.g., in response to muscle weakening, is mediated by cerebellar vermis and flocculus. Saccadic offset may be controlled, either by active braking, or by an exponential slide signal. The neurophysiology is summarized by a quantitative model, in which the firing rate of burst neurons is controlled by a dynamic negative feedback loop that carries the instantaneous eye position signal from the neural integrator. This signal is compared with a desired eye-position command in the head from higher centers, and the resulting dynamic motor error drives the high-gain burst cells. Instability of the system is prevented by the mutual inhibitory interaction between burst cells and omnipause neurons. The model explains many features of normal saccades, but also accounts for pathologies and abnormalities like dynamic overshoots and saccade oscillations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- David A Robinson
- Late Professor of Ophthalmology, Biomedical Engineering and Neuroscience, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, United States
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Abstract
Most modern video eye trackers deliver binocular data. Many researchers take the average of the left and right eye signals (the version signal) to decrease the variable error (precision) up to a factor of \documentclass[12pt]{minimal}
\usepackage{amsmath}
\usepackage{wasysym}
\usepackage{amsfonts}
\usepackage{amssymb}
\usepackage{amsbsy}
\usepackage{mathrsfs}
\usepackage{upgreek}
\setlength{\oddsidemargin}{-69pt}
\begin{document}$\sqrt {2}$\end{document}2. What happens to the systematic error (accuracy) if the left and right eye signals are averaged? To determine the systematic error, we conducted a calibration validation in two experiments (n= 79 and n = 64). The systematic error was computed for the left eye, right eye, and version signals separately. In respectively 29.5 and 25.8% of the participants, the systematic error of a single eye signal was lower than that of the version signal at the cost of a higher variable error. If a small variable error is desirable, and the difference between the left and the right eye is not the topic of study, one should average position data from the left and the right eye (in other words, use the version signal). If a small systematic error is desirable, one should use the signal (from left eye, right eye or version) that delivers the best accuracy. In the latter case, this may cause worse precision than that of the version signal.
Collapse
|
3
|
The effect of distractors on saccades and adaptation of saccades in strabismus. Vision Res 2011; 51:2405-24. [PMID: 22037361 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2011.09.017] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/20/2011] [Revised: 09/27/2011] [Accepted: 09/28/2011] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
This paper reports two experiments to determine the contribution of the suppressing eye to the generation of saccadic eye movements in constant strabismus. Eye movements were recorded using a Skalar infra-red recorder. Experiment 1 tested six participants with constant strabismus, pathological suppression and no clinically demonstrable binocular single vision (BSV). We explored the effect of visual distractors presented monocularly (to either the fixing eye or the strabismic eye) and binocularly, on saccade latency and accuracy. Saccade latency significantly increased when distractors were presented to the strabismic eye compared to the no distractor condition. In all participants the effect on latency, with distractors presented to the strabismic eye, was maximum when distractors were presented towards the location of the anatomical fovea. Saccade accuracy was reduced with ipsilateral distractors to the target when presented binocularly or monocularly to the fixing eye but not affected by distractors presented to the strabismic eye. Experiment 2 investigated fast disconjugate saccade adaptations in six participants with constant strabismus, pathological suppression and no clinically demonstrable BSV and for comparison 8 with normal bifoveal BSV. Saccade disconjugacy was induced using an electronic feedback system in which the calibrated eye movement position signal could be scaled by a factor (the feedback gain) to move the target visible to one eye during binocular viewing. In all BSV participants and 3 of 6 participants with constant strabismus, saccadic adaptation occurred rapidly such that under conditions of visual feedback saccades became increasingly disconjugate. These disconjugacies persisted when normal viewing conditions were restored. The presence of an adaptive mechanism to adjust the binocular co-ordination of saccades in the presence of constant strabismus with suppression and no clinically demonstrable BSV has been demonstrated. Mechanisms that might explain such results are discussed.
Collapse
|
4
|
Davis-López de Carrizosa MA, Morado-Díaz CJ, Miller JM, de la Cruz RR, Pastor AM. Dual encoding of muscle tension and eye position by abducens motoneurons. J Neurosci 2011; 31:2271-9. [PMID: 21307263 PMCID: PMC3074277 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.5416-10.2011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 32] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/14/2010] [Revised: 11/09/2010] [Accepted: 12/09/2010] [Indexed: 01/08/2023] Open
Abstract
Extraocular muscle tension associated with spontaneous eye movements has a pulse-slide-step profile similar to that of motoneuron firing rate. Existing models only relate motoneuron firing to eye position, velocity and acceleration. We measured and quantitatively compared lateral rectus muscle force and eye position with the firing of abducens motoneurons in the cat to determine fundamental encoding correlations. During fixations (step), muscle force increased exponentially with eccentric eye position, consistent with a model of estimate ensemble motor innervation based on neuronal sensitivities and recruitment order. Moreover, firing rate in all motoneurons tested was better related to eye position than to muscle tension during fixations. In contrast, during the postsaccadic slide phase, the time constant of firing rate decay was closely related to that of muscle force decay, suggesting that all motoneurons encode muscle tension as well. Discharge characteristics of abducens motoneurons formed overlapping clusters of phasic and tonic motoneurons, thus, tonic units recruited earlier and had a larger slide signal. We conclude that the slide signal is a discharge characteristic of the motoneuron that controls muscle tension during the postsaccadic phase and that motoneurons are specialized for both tension and position-related properties. The organization of signal content in the pool of abducens motoneurons from the very phasic to the very tonic units is possibly a result of the differential trophic background received from distinct types of muscle fibers.
Collapse
|
5
|
Bucci M, Kapoula Z, Bernotas M, Zamfirescu F. Role of attention and eye preference in the binocular coordination of saccades in strabismus. Neuroophthalmology 2009. [DOI: 10.1076/noph.22.2.115.3736] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/03/2022] Open
|
6
|
Jaschinski W, Švede A, Jainta S. Relation between fixation disparity and the asymmetry between convergent and divergent disparity step responses. Vision Res 2008; 48:253-63. [DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2007.11.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/05/2007] [Revised: 11/09/2007] [Accepted: 11/09/2007] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
|
7
|
Kloke WB, Jaschinski W. Individual differences in the asymmetry of binocular saccades, analysed with mixed-effects models. Biol Psychol 2006; 73:220-6. [PMID: 16740356 DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.04.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/16/2005] [Revised: 04/03/2006] [Accepted: 04/03/2006] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
We analysed the reliability of individual differences in parameters of binocular saccadic eye movements. During saccades between isovergent targets, the movement of the right and left eye are not exactly symmetrical (conjugate). Typically, the abducting eye has a shorter latency and reaches a higher velocity, so that a transient divergence occurs during the saccade. For the asymmetry in latency and for the maximum of transient divergence, we applied statistical mixed-effects models in a repeated-measures design with 39 subjects and found that the variability between subjects was much larger than the variability from Sessions 1 to 2 (about 8 days later). The retest correlations were 0.54 for the latency difference, and 0.82 for the transient divergence maximum. We conclude that significant individual differences exist in the asymmetry of binocular saccades and that these can be observed with about 20 saccadic trials per subject.
Collapse
|
8
|
Hopp JJ, Fuchs AF. The characteristics and neuronal substrate of saccadic eye movement plasticity. Prog Neurobiol 2004; 72:27-53. [PMID: 15019175 DOI: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2003.12.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 243] [Impact Index Per Article: 12.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/20/2003] [Accepted: 12/01/2003] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
Abstract
Saccadic eye movements are shifts in the direction of gaze that rapidly and accurately aim the fovea at targets of interest. Saccades are so brief that visual feedback cannot guide them to their targets. Therefore, the saccadic motor command must be accurately specified in advance of the movement and continually modified to compensate for growth, injury, and aging, which otherwise would produce dysmetric saccades. When a persistent dysmetria occurs in subjects with muscle weakness or neural damage or is induced in normal primates by the surreptitious jumping of a target forward or backward as a saccade is made to acquire the target, saccadic amplitude changes to reduce the dysmetria. Adaptation of saccadic amplitude or direction occurs gradually and is retained in the dark, thus representing true motor plasticity. Saccadic adaptation is more rapid in humans than in monkeys, usually is incomplete in both species, and is slower and less robust for amplitude increases than decreases. Adaptation appears to be motor rather than sensory. In humans, adaptation of saccades that would seem to require more sensory-motor processing does not transfer to saccades that seem to require less, suggesting the existence of distributed adaptation loci. In monkeys, however, transfer from more simple to more complex saccades is robust, suggesting a common adaptation site. Neurophysiological data from both species indicate that the oculomotor cerebellum is crucial for saccadic adaptation. This review shows that the precise, voluntary behaviors known as saccadic eye movements provide an alternative to simple reflexes for the study of the neuronal basis of motor learning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- J Johanna Hopp
- Department of Physiology and Biophysics, National Primate Research Center, University of Washington, Box 357290, 1959 NE Pacific St. HSB G-424, Seattle, WA 98195, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
9
|
Bucci MP, Gomes M, Paris S, Kapoula Z. Disconjugate oculomotor learning caused by feeble image-size inequality: differences between secondary and tertiary positions. Vision Res 2001; 41:625-37. [PMID: 11226507 DOI: 10.1016/s0042-6989(00)00293-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
In order to examine the minimum value of image-size inequality capable of inducing lasting disconjugacy of the amplitude of saccades, six normal emmetropic subjects were exposed for 16 min to 2% image size inequality. Subjects were seated at 1 m in front of a screen where a random-dot pattern was projected and made saccades of 7.5 and 15 deg along the horizontal and vertical principal meridians and to tertiary positions in the upper and lower field. During the training period, compensatory disconjugacy of the amplitude of the saccades occurred for the principal horizontal and vertical meridians; such increased disconjugacy persisted after training, suggesting learning. In contrast, for horizontal saccades to or from tertiary positions made in the upper and lower field, no consistent changes in the disconjugacy occurred, either during training or after the training condition. In an additional experiment, three subjects read sequences of words with the 2% magnifier in front of their dominant eye: in such a task, horizontal saccades to or from tertiary positions at the upper or lower field showed appropriate and lasting disconjugacy for two of the three subjects. We conclude that even a 2% image size inequality stimulates oculomotor learning, leading to persistent disconjugacy of saccades. The small disparity created by the image-size inequality is thus compensated by the oculomotor system rather than tolerated by the sensory system (e.g. by enlarging the Panum's area).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- M P Bucci
- Laboratoire de Physiologie de la Perception et de l'Action, UMR 9950, CNRS-Collège de France, 11, Place Marcelin Berthelot, 75005, Paris, France.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
10
|
Kapoula Z, Bucci MP, Eggert T, Garraud L. Impairment of the binocular coordination of saccades in strabismus. Vision Res 1997; 37:2757-66. [PMID: 9373674 DOI: 10.1016/s0042-6989(97)00064-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 39] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/05/2023]
Abstract
To examine the link between binocular vision and binocular coordination of saccades we studied subjects with convergent strabismus since childhood with mild or no amblyopia: three subjects had small squint (< 10 prism D) and preserved peripheral binocular visual function with gross stereopsis; four subjects had larger squint (18-35 prism D) and no detectable stereopsis. A standard paradigm was used to elicit horizontal saccades; binocular recordings were made with the IRIS device. For subjects with small strabismus, saccades were disconjugate (unequal between the two eyes) typically by 1 deg. Subjects with larger strabismus exhibited even larger and more variable disconjugacy (typically 1.8 deg). Post-saccadic eye drift was consistently divergent in subjects with small strabismus and tended to reduce the convergent squint angle. In contrast, in subjects with large strabismus drift was convergent. The impairment of the binocular control of saccades is attributed to the deficiency of disconjugate oculomotor adaptive capabilities necessary to compensate for the natural asymmetries or changes in the two oculomotor plants; such deficiency would be more severe in subjects with large strabismus who have neither central nor peripheral binocular vision.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Z Kapoula
- Laboratoire de Physiologie de la Perception et de l'Action, UMR 9950, CNRS-Collège de France, Paris, France.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
11
|
Inchingolo P, Accardo A, Da Pozzo S, Pensiero S, Perissutti P. Cyclopean and disconjugate adaptive recovery from post-saccadic drift in strabismic children before and after surgery. Vision Res 1996; 36:2897-913. [PMID: 8917792 DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(96)00007-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/03/2023]
Abstract
Post-saccadic drift has been analyzed in strabismic children 11-18 yr old, before and after surgery. Before surgery all the subjects had a large multi-component post-saccadic drift. A disconjugate compensation of the drift was not active, but an optimized cyclopean compensation occurred, aimed at minimizing the drift size in both eyes, either with binocular or monocular vision. The drift was directed toward the static offset of the eyes. One week after surgery the drift increased in most cases but the cyclopean compensation still occurred. One week later, a partial disconjugate compensation decreased the drift to levels lower than before surgery. In one subject surgery produced very big three-component drift, exceeding 20 deg. One year after the drift was decreased to < 1 deg. A mathematical model is presented accounting for most of the results.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- P Inchingolo
- Dipartmento di Elettrotecnica, Elettronica ed Informatica Università di Trieste, Italy.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
12
|
Abstract
In five normal subjects, we analyzed uncalled for torsion (blips) during and after horizontal and vertical saccades. Torsion was defined as movement out of Listing's plane. During horizontal saccades in downward gaze the abducting eye extorted and the adducting eye intorted. The direction of the blips reversed in upward gaze. Peak torsional amplitudes (up to 1-2 deg) were always reached during saccades; drifts back to Listing's plane outlasted the saccades. Torsion of the extorting eye was larger than that of the intorting eye, producing a transient positive cyclovergence. Torsion and cyclovergence evoked by vertical saccades were also stereotyped in each eye, but showed idiosyncratic differences among subjects. We conclude that Listing's law is violated during saccades. Transient saccade-evoked torsion might reflect properties of the three-dimensional velocity-to-position integrator and/or the ocular plant.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- D Straumann
- Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins Hospital, Baltimore, MD 21287, USA
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
13
|
Bruno P, Inchingolo P, van der Steen J. Unequal saccades produced by aniseikonic patterns: a model approach. Vision Res 1995; 35:3473-92. [PMID: 8560813 DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(95)00157-a] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/31/2023]
Abstract
This study addresses a possible mechanism for fast disconjugate adaptation of binocular horizontal saccades. Disconjugacy of binocular saccades was elicited by two dichoptically presented, identical but aniseikonic, random checkerboard patterns. Adaptation was achieved with the patterns at far distance (144 cm). In this condition, which requires a relatively small (8%) size difference of the saccades, a short learning period was mandatory for the binocular saccades to become disconjugate. The saccadic modifications were superimposed on an idiosyncratic pattern of intra-saccadic yoking. A model of saccadic signal generation is described, that has been used to separate the contributions on saccadic disconjugacy provided by modification of visual inputs processing, which alters the motor-system inputs, and by modification of the control system: the adaptation. We identified three major components of the saccadic command (two phasic and one tonic) that contribute and in a specific way to the saccadic yoking and disconjugacy. The model analysis proposes that separate control mechanisms exist operating on these phasic and tonic signals. We show that the saccadic system can generate the vergence component shown by our aniseikonic saccades. We discuss a distributed-parallel implementation of the saccadic system able to provide both the conjugate and disconjugate components of control.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- P Bruno
- Dipartimento di Elettrotecnica, Elettronica ed Informatica, University of Trieste, Italy
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
14
|
Abstract
In recent years the conjugacy of the saccadic eye movements has been studied extensively in adult humans, while little investigation has been carried out in children. We studied the characteristics of binocular saccades in school-age children, finding significant differences with adults, particularly in their dynamics and binocular coordination. The largest deviations were found in the youngest children. An incomplete optimization of the saccadic waveform and a poor disconjugate compensation of the mechanical asymmetries of the plants are hypothesized to explain the results.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- F Fioravanti
- Dipartimento di Elettrotecnia, Elettronica ed Informatica, University of Trieste, Italy
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
15
|
Lemij HG, Collewijn H. Nonconjugate adaptation of human saccades to anisometropic spectacles: meridian-specificity. Vision Res 1992; 32:453-64. [PMID: 1604832 DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(92)90237-d] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/27/2022]
Abstract
Recently it has been demonstrated that saccades become different in size in the two eyes if a subject is adapted to anisometropic spectacles, which provide visual images of different magnitude to the two eyes. These nonconjugate adaptations adequately meet the requirements of those spectacles and, once acquired, they persist (with some reduction) even during monocular viewing. We now demonstrate that such nonconjugate adaptations of saccades can be meridian-specific, if there is a pressure for such meridian-specificity. This pressure was provided by means of a cylindrical spectacle-lens. Adaptations along a vertical, horizontal or oblique meridian did not transfer to the orthogonal meridian. These results demonstrate a capability of saccadic adaptation to deal with calibration problems restricted not only to one eye, but even to one specific plane of muscular action. Our results also suggest that the meridian-specific adaptations of oblique saccades take place at a stage before the decomposition of motor commands into separate horizontal and vertical components. The meridian-specific nonconjugacies were also expressed in smooth-pursuit eye movements. Post-saccadic drift adapted only along the horizontal meridian.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- H G Lemij
- Department of Physiology I, Faculty of Medicine, Erasmus University Rotterdam, The Netherlands
| | | |
Collapse
|
16
|
Abstract
Previous research has shown that the smooth pursuit system in early infancy is quite immature. Infants' tracking of a single, small target moving at velocities greater than 10 deg/sec is almost entirely saccadic until the end of the second postnatal month. The emergence of smooth pursuit is characterized by low gain (less than 0.5) and frequent saccadic intrusions. To provide a quantitative description of pursuit to relatively slow target velocities, 10 infants ranging in age from 7 to 11 weeks viewed a 2 deg target that was stepped 5 to 10 deg from screen center and then ramped back to screen center and 10 deg beyond at a constant velocity of 3, 6 or 12 deg/sec. Smooth pursuit was observed even in the youngest infant whose segments of pursuit between saccades were up to 5 sec in duration. At the slowest target velocity, mean pursuit gain across infants was 0.50, while at 6 and 12 deg/sec mean pursuit gain was 0.25 and 0.11. This systematic decrease in pursuit gain with increasing target velocity implies that pursuit velocity was invariant across the three target velocities. These findings suggest that smooth pursuit can be generated consistently by the end of the second postnatal month, but that it is slow and uncalibrated to the velocity of the target.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- S L Shea
- Center for Visual Science, University of Rochester, NY 14627
| | | |
Collapse
|
17
|
Collewijn H, Erkelens CJ, Steinman RM. Binocular co-ordination of human horizontal saccadic eye movements. J Physiol 1988; 404:157-82. [PMID: 3253429 PMCID: PMC1190820 DOI: 10.1113/jphysiol.1988.sp017284] [Citation(s) in RCA: 294] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/04/2023] Open
Abstract
1. The binocular co-ordination of human horizontal saccades was analysed for the first time systematically over the full oculomotor range with a precise and accurate scleral sensor coil technique. Effects of amplitude (1.25-80 deg), direction (adduction vs. abduction and centrifugal vs. centripetal) and eccentricity (symmetrical about primary or between primary and secondary positions) were systematically investigated in three subjects). 2. To minimize extraneous effects of stimulus presentation on the programming of saccades, subjects were instructed to voluntarily change their gaze between two continuously visible targets. These were positioned on an iso-vergence locus, and thus contained no stimulus for disjunctive eye movements. 3. Under these conditions the amplitudes of the primary saccades of the two eyes were remarkably accurate; undershooting of the target by about 0.5 deg (independent of amplitude in the range 10-70 deg) was typical. This finding contrasts with the undershooting by about 10% described in the literature as characteristic for other stimulus conditions. 4. Saccadic peak velocities saturated at a mean asymptotic level of 502 +/- 32 (S.D.) deg/s for saccades of 40 deg and larger. The duration was linearly related to amplitude for saccades up to 50 deg; for saccades of larger sizes the duration increased progressively more steeply. Skewness values (acceleration time as a fraction of total saccadic duration) decreased from about 0.45 for saccades up to 10 deg to about 0.20 for saccades of 50 deg and larger. 5. Binocular saccades showed an abduction-adduction asymmetry and were not well yoked dynamically. The saccades of the abducting eye consistently had a larger size, a higher peak velocity, a shorter duration and were more skewed than the concomitant adducting saccades of the fellow eye. As a result, the eyes diverged transiently by as much as 3 deg during horizontal saccades. 6. Saccades also showed a marked centrifugal-centripetal asymmetry. Peak velocities of saccades towards the primary position were about 10% higher than peak velocities of corresponding centrifugal saccades. 7. These directional asymmetries were the main source of variability in the pool of saccades. In comparison, intra- and intersubject variability was minor in our sample. 8. Post-saccadic drift consisted of a vergence and a version component. The vergence component of this drift was a continuation of the vergence movement occurring during saccades. The version component, generally smaller than the vergence component, was directed towards the target position.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 400 WORDS)
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- H Collewijn
- Department of Physiology I, Erasmus University Rotterdam, The Netherlands
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
18
|
Abstract
1. The binocular co-ordination of human vertical saccades was analysed systematically over the full oculomotor range, with a precise and accurate scleral sensor coil technique. Effects of amplitude (1.25-70 deg), direction (upward vs. downward and centripetal vs. centrifugal), as well as position (upper or lower sector of vertical oculomotor range), were investigated systematically in three subjects. 2. All saccades were made voluntarily between continuously presented pairs of targets, which subtended equal angles of target vergence. 3. Vertical saccades were less accurate than horizontal saccades (as described by Collewijn, Erkelens & Steinman, 1988). For target distances between 10 and 70 deg, upward saccades undershot the target by about 10%, whereas downward saccades tended to overshoot the target. Downward saccades were about 1.5 deg larger than upward saccades between the same targets. 4. Peak velocities continued to increase monotonically with saccadic amplitude up to 513 +/- 27 (S.D.) deg/s for 70 deg saccades; a distinct asymptotic level was not reached. 5. Velocity profiles of upward and downward saccades, made symmetrically about the primary (straight-ahead) position, were very similar for amplitudes up to 30 deg. At larger amplitudes, velocity profiles of upward saccades remained single peaked, whereas those of downward saccades invariably developed a second velocity peak. 6. Parameters of upward saccades depended heavily on the position of the eye. In the upper oculomotor range such saccades had lower maximum speeds, longer durations, and were more skewed than similar saccades in the lower oculomotor range (below primary). Downward saccades were almost independent of eye position. 7. Vertical eye movements during vertical saccades were virtually identical in the two eyes. In contrast, disjunctive horizontal components were systematically present. Upward saccades, at all amplitudes, were associated with diverging eye movements. Converging eye movements occurred during downward saccades. These systematic effects suggest that the vergence subsystem is not turned off during saccades. 8. These changes in vergence were followed by converging horizontal post-saccadic drift after upward saccades, and in diverging horizontal drift after downward saccades.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 400 WORDS)
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- H Collewijn
- Department of Physiology I, Erasmus University Rotterdam, The Netherlands
| | | | | |
Collapse
|