1
|
Sechrest ER, Chmelik K, Tan WD, Deng WT. Blue cone monochromacy and gene therapy. Vision Res 2023; 208:108221. [PMID: 37001420 PMCID: PMC10182257 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2023.108221] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/06/2023] [Revised: 03/14/2023] [Accepted: 03/15/2023] [Indexed: 03/30/2023]
Abstract
Blue cone monochromacy (BCM) is a congenital vision disorder characterized by complete loss or severely reduced long- and middle-wavelength cone function, caused by mutations in the OPN1LW/OPN1MW gene cluster on the X-chromosome. BCM patients typically suffer from poor visual acuity, severely impaired color discrimination, myopia, and nystagmus. In this review, we cover the genetic causes of BCM, clinical features of BCM patients, genetic testing, and clinical outcome measurements for future BCM clinical trials. However, our emphasis is on detailing the animal models for BCM and gene therapy using adeno-associated vectors (AAV). We describe two mouse models resembling the two most common causes of BCM, current progress in proof-of-concept studies to treat BCM with deletion mutations, the challenges we face, and future directions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Emily R Sechrest
- Department of Ophthalmology and Visual Sciences, West Virginia University, Morgantown, WV 26505, United States
| | - Kathryn Chmelik
- Department of Ophthalmology and Visual Sciences, West Virginia University, Morgantown, WV 26505, United States; Department of Biochemistry, West Virginia University, Morgantown, WV 26505, United States
| | - Wendy D Tan
- Department of Ophthalmology and Visual Sciences, West Virginia University, Morgantown, WV 26505, United States
| | - Wen-Tao Deng
- Department of Ophthalmology and Visual Sciences, West Virginia University, Morgantown, WV 26505, United States; Department of Biochemistry, West Virginia University, Morgantown, WV 26505, United States.
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Zlatkova MB, Racheva K, Totev T, Mihaylova M, Hristov I, Anderson RS. Resolution acuity and spatial summation of chromatic mechanisms in the peripheral retina. JOURNAL OF THE OPTICAL SOCIETY OF AMERICA. A, OPTICS, IMAGE SCIENCE, AND VISION 2021; 38:1003-1014. [PMID: 34263756 DOI: 10.1364/josaa.418073] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/18/2020] [Accepted: 05/28/2021] [Indexed: 06/13/2023]
Abstract
Green stimuli are more difficult to detect than red stimuli in the retinal periphery, as reported previously. We examined the spatial characteristics of chromatic mechanisms using stimuli, modulated from an achromatic background to each pole of the "red-green" cardinal axis in DKL space at 20 deg eccentricity. The "blue-yellow" cardinal axis was also studied for comparison. By measuring both grating discrimination at the resolution limit (resolution acuity) and spatial summation, assessed by the Michaelis-Menten function, we demonstrated a marked "red-green" asymmetry. The resolution acuity was worse and spatial summation more extended for "green" compared to "red" stimuli, while showing significant individual variations. Ricco's area was also measured, but not determined for "green" spots because of the poor small stimuli detection. These results cannot be explained by differences in L- and M-cone numerosity and/or spatial arrangement, but rather have postreceptoral origin, probably at the cortical level.
Collapse
|
3
|
Abstract
We as a couple spent 50 years working in visual psychophysics of color vision, temporal vision, and luminance adaptation. We sought collaborations with ophthalmologists, anatomists, physiologists, physicists, and psychologists, aiming to relate visual psychophysics to the underlying physiology of the primate retina. This review describes our journey and reflections in exploring the visual system.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Joel Pokorny
- Department of Ophthalmology and Visual Science, The University of Chicago, Chicago, Illinois 60637, USA;,
| | - Vivianne C. Smith
- Department of Ophthalmology and Visual Science, The University of Chicago, Chicago, Illinois 60637, USA;,
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Abstract
We have compared two explanations for poor peripheral binding. Binding is the ability to assign the correct features (e.g., color, direction of motion, orientation) to objects. Wu, Kanai, and Shimojo (Nature, 429(6989), 262, 2004) showed that subjects performed poorly on binding dot color with direction of motion in the periphery. Suzuki, Wolfe, Horowitz, and Noguchi (Vision Research, 82, 58-65, 2013) similarly showed that subjects had trouble binding color with line orientation in the periphery. These authors concluded that performance in the periphery was poor because binding is poor in the periphery. However, both studies used red and green stimuli. We tested an alternative hypothesis, that poor peripheral binding is in part due to poor peripheral red/green color vision. Eccentricity-dependent changes in visual processing cause peripheral red/green vision to be worse than foveal vision. In contrast, blue/yellow vision remains centrifugally more stable. We tested 9 subjects in a replication and extension of Suzuki and colleagues' line orientation judgment, in red and green, and in blue and yellow. There were three central conditions: (1) red (or blue) all horizontal, green (or yellow) all vertical; (2) red (or blue) all vertical, green (or yellow) all horizontal; or (3) random pairing of color and orientation. In both the red/green and the blue/yellow color schemes, peripheral performance was influenced by central line orientation, replicating Suzuki and colleagues. However, the effect with blue/yellow lines was smaller, indicating that poor peripheral "binding," as hypothesized by both Wu and colleagues and Suzuki and colleagues, is due in part to their use of red and green stimuli.
Collapse
|
5
|
Zhang F, Kurokawa K, Lassoued A, Crowell JA, Miller DT. Cone photoreceptor classification in the living human eye from photostimulation-induced phase dynamics. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2019; 116:7951-7956. [PMID: 30944223 PMCID: PMC6475411 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1816360116] [Citation(s) in RCA: 85] [Impact Index Per Article: 17.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Human color vision is achieved by mixing neural signals from cone photoreceptors sensitive to different wavelengths of light. The spatial arrangement and proportion of these spectral types in the retina set fundamental limits on color perception, and abnormal or missing types are responsible for color vision loss. Imaging provides the most direct and quantitative means to study these photoreceptor properties at the cellular scale in the living human retina, but remains challenging. Current methods rely on retinal densitometry to distinguish cone types, a prohibitively slow process. Here, we show that photostimulation-induced optical phase changes occur in cone cells and carry substantial information about spectral type, enabling cones to be differentiated with unprecedented accuracy and efficiency. Moreover, these phase dynamics arise from physiological activity occurring on dramatically different timescales (from milliseconds to seconds) inside the cone outer segment, thus exposing the phototransduction cascade and subsequent downstream effects. We captured these dynamics in cones of subjects with normal color vision and a deuteranope, and at different macular locations by: (i) marrying adaptive optics to phase-sensitive optical coherence tomography to avoid optical blurring of the eye, (ii) acquiring images at high speed that samples phase dynamics at up to 3 KHz, and (iii) localizing phase changes to the cone outer segment, where photoactivation occurs. Our method should have broad appeal for color vision applications in which the underlying neural processing of photoreceptors is sought and for investigations of retinal diseases that affect cone function.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Furu Zhang
- School of Optometry, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN 47405
| | | | - Ayoub Lassoued
- School of Optometry, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN 47405
| | - James A Crowell
- School of Optometry, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN 47405
| | - Donald T Miller
- School of Optometry, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN 47405
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Gunther KL, Downey CO. Influence of stimulus size on revealing non-cardinal color mechanisms. Vision Res 2016; 127:57-66. [PMID: 27470702 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2016.06.012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/26/2015] [Revised: 06/14/2016] [Accepted: 06/22/2016] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
Abstract
Multiple studies have shown that performance of subjects on a number of visual tasks is worse for non-cardinal than cardinal colors, especially in the red-green/luminance (RG/LUM) and tritan/luminance (TRIT/LUM) color planes. Inspired by neurophysiological evidence that suppressive surround input to receptive fields is particularly sensitive to luminance, we hypothesized that non-cardinal mechanisms in the RG/LUM and TRIT/LUM planes would be more sensitive to stimulus size than are isoluminant non-cardinal mechanisms. In Experiment 1 we tested 9-10 color-normal subjects in each of the three color planes (RG/TRIT, RG/LUM, and TRIT/LUM) on visual search at four bull's-eye dot sizes (0.5°/1°, 1°/2°, 2°/4°, and 3°/6° center/annulus dot diameter). This study yielded a significant main effect of dot size in each of the three color planes. In Experiment 2 we tested the same hypothesis using noise masking, at three stimulus sizes (3°, 6° and 9° diameter Gabors), again in all three color planes (5 subjects per color plane). This experiment yielded, in the RG/TRIT plane, a significant main effect of stimulus size; in the RG/LUM plane, significant evidence for non-cardinal mechanisms only for the 9° stimulus; but in the TRIT/LUM plane no evidence for non-cardinal mechanisms at any stimulus size. These results suggest that non-cardinal mechanisms, particularly in the RG/LUM color plane, are more sensitive to stimulus size than are non-cardinals in the RG/TRIT plane, supporting our hypothesis.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Karen L Gunther
- Department of Psychology, Wabash College, 301 West Wabash Avenue, Crawfordsville, IN 47933, USA.
| | - Colin O Downey
- Department of Psychology, Wabash College, 301 West Wabash Avenue, Crawfordsville, IN 47933, USA
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Gunther KL. Non-cardinal color mechanism strength differs across color planes but not across subjects. JOURNAL OF THE OPTICAL SOCIETY OF AMERICA. A, OPTICS, IMAGE SCIENCE, AND VISION 2014; 31:A293-A302. [PMID: 24695185 DOI: 10.1364/josaa.31.00a293] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/03/2023]
Abstract
This study tested two hypotheses: (1) that non-cardinal color mechanisms may be due to individual differences: some subjects have them (or have stronger ones), while other subjects do not; and (2) that non-cardinal mechanisms may be stronger in the isoluminant plane of color space than in the two planes with luminance. Five to six subjects per color plane were tested on three psychophysical paradigms: adaptation, noise masking, and plaid coherence. There were no consistent individual differences in non-cardinal mechanism strength across the three paradigms. In group-averaged data, non-cardinal mechanisms appear to be weaker in the two planes with luminance than in the isoluminant plane.
Collapse
|
8
|
Gunther KL. Non-cardinal color perception across the retina: easy for orange, hard for burgundy and sky blue. JOURNAL OF THE OPTICAL SOCIETY OF AMERICA. A, OPTICS, IMAGE SCIENCE, AND VISION 2014; 31:A274-A282. [PMID: 24695183 DOI: 10.1364/josaa.31.00a274] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/03/2023]
Abstract
Cardinal color performance (reddish, greenish, bluish, yellowish, black, and white) has been shown to decline in peripheral viewing. What about non-cardinal color performance (e.g., orange, burgundy, and sky blue)? In visual search, performance on non-cardinal colors matched that of the cardinal colors in the (L-M)/(S-(L+M)) (isoluminant) color plane (Experiment 1, n=10, to 30°; Experiment 2, n=3, to 50°). However, performance in the (L-M)/(L+M) and (S-(L+M))/(L+M) color planes was worse for non-cardinal colors, at all eccentricities, even in the fovea. The implications that these results have for the existence of non-cardinal mechanisms in each color plane are discussed.
Collapse
|
9
|
Danilova M, Chan C, Mollon J. Can spatial resolution reveal individual differences in the L:M cone ratio? Vision Res 2013; 78:26-38. [DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2012.12.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/09/2012] [Revised: 12/05/2012] [Accepted: 12/11/2012] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
|
10
|
Bókkon I, Vimal RLP, Wang C, Dai J, Salari V, Grass F, Antal I. Visible light induced ocular delayed bioluminescence as a possible origin of negative afterimage. JOURNAL OF PHOTOCHEMISTRY AND PHOTOBIOLOGY B-BIOLOGY 2011; 103:192-9. [PMID: 21463953 DOI: 10.1016/j.jphotobiol.2011.03.011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/14/2010] [Revised: 02/24/2011] [Accepted: 03/15/2011] [Indexed: 12/11/2022]
Abstract
The delayed luminescence of biological tissues is an ultraweak reemission of absorbed photons after exposure to external monochromatic or white light illumination. Recently, Wang, Bókkon, Dai and Antal (2011) [10] presented the first experimental proof of the existence of spontaneous ultraweak biophoton emission and visible light induced delayed ultraweak photon emission from in vitro freshly isolated rat's whole eye, lens, vitreous humor and retina. Here, we suggest that the photobiophysical source of negative afterimage can also occur within the eye by delayed bioluminescent photons. In other words, when we stare at a colored (or white) image for few seconds, external photons can induce excited electronic states within different parts of the eye that is followed by a delayed reemission of absorbed photons for several seconds. Finally, these reemitted photons can be absorbed by non-bleached photoreceptors that produce a negative afterimage. Although this suggests the photobiophysical source of negative afterimages is related retinal mechanisms, cortical neurons have also essential contribution in the interpretation and modulation of negative afterimages.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- I Bókkon
- Doctoral School of Pharmaceutical and Pharmacological Sciences, Semmelweis University, Budapest, Hungary.
| | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
11
|
Abstract
We report measurements of the absolute threshold of cone vision, which has been previously underestimated due to suboptimal conditions or overly strict subjective response criteria. We avoided these limitations by using optimized stimuli and experimental conditions while having subjects respond within a rating scale framework. Small (1' fwhm), brief (34 ms), monochromatic (550 nm) stimuli were foveally presented at multiple intensities in dark-adapted retina for 5 subjects. For comparison, 4 subjects underwent similar testing with rod-optimized stimuli. Cone absolute threshold, that is, the minimum light energy for which subjects were just able to detect a visual stimulus with any response criterion, was 203 ± 38 photons at the cornea, ~0.47 log unit lower than previously reported. Two-alternative forced-choice measurements in a subset of subjects yielded consistent results. Cone thresholds were less responsive to criterion changes than rod thresholds, suggesting a limit to the stimulus information recoverable from the cone mosaic in addition to the limit imposed by Poisson noise. Results were consistent with expectations for detection in the face of stimulus uncertainty. We discuss implications of these findings for modeling the first stages of human cone vision and interpreting psychophysical data acquired with adaptive optics at the spatial scale of the receptor mosaic.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Darren Koenig
- University of Houston College of Optometry, 4900 Calhoun Road, Houston, TX 77204, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
12
|
Wang C, Bókkon I, Dai J, Antal I. Spontaneous and visible light-induced ultraweak photon emission from rat eyes. Brain Res 2011; 1369:1-9. [DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.10.077] [Citation(s) in RCA: 38] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/18/2010] [Revised: 10/15/2010] [Accepted: 10/19/2010] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
|
13
|
Bókkon I, Vimal RLP. Retinal phosphenes and discrete dark noises in rods: A new biophysical framework. JOURNAL OF PHOTOCHEMISTRY AND PHOTOBIOLOGY B-BIOLOGY 2009; 96:255-9. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jphotobiol.2009.07.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/16/2009] [Revised: 06/28/2009] [Accepted: 07/02/2009] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
|
14
|
Jägle H, Heine J, Kurtenbach A. L:M-cone ratio estimates of the outer and inner retina and its impact on sex differences in ERG amplitudes. Doc Ophthalmol 2006; 113:105-13. [PMID: 17047907 DOI: 10.1007/s10633-006-9019-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/22/2006] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
Abstract
The relative number of L- and M-cones varies greatly between individuals. Differences in the relative number of L- and M-cones may also contribute to the sex difference in the ERG response amplitude reported several times. We therefore investigated the relative number of L- and M-cones and its impact on sex differences in ERG amplitudes. Multifocal ERG (mfERG) and multifocal oscillatory potentials (mfOP) combined with a cone silent substitution technique were used to investigate outer and inner retinal signals recorded from 7 female and 7 male trichromats. L:M ratios were estimated from peak amplitude as well as from area under curve (AUC) analysis. For mfERGs the L:M ratios estimates were independent of the method of analysis, while for mfOPs, differences were found which are possibly due to an artefact in the calculation of ratios for small responses. There was a tendency for lower L:M ratios in female than in male subjects for both analysis of mfERG and mfOP responses. The (L+M)-response amplitudes at both sites were slightly higher and the L:M ratios were lower for female than for male observers. Because the magnitude of the ERG amplitude differences is larger than could be explained by L:M-ratio and axial length differences, we conclude that it may be due to a direct effect of sex hormones on ion channel function.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Herbert Jägle
- Department of Pathophysiology of Vision & Neuro-Ophthalmology, University Eye Hospital, Schleichstr. 12-16, D-72076, Tübingen, Germany.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
15
|
Troup LJ, Pitts MA, Volbrecht VJ, Nerger JL. Effect of stimulus intensity on the sizes of chromatic perceptive fields. JOURNAL OF THE OPTICAL SOCIETY OF AMERICA. A, OPTICS, IMAGE SCIENCE, AND VISION 2005; 22:2137-42. [PMID: 16277283 DOI: 10.1364/josaa.22.002137] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/05/2023]
Abstract
The effects of intensity on chromatic perceptive field size were investigated along the horizontal meridian at 10 degrees temporal eccentricity by manipulating stimulus intensity from 0.3 to 3.3 log trolands. Following light adaptation, observers described the hue and saturation of monochromatic stimuli (440-660 nm, in 10 nm steps) for a series of test sizes (0.098-3 degrees) presented along the time period associated with the cone plateau of the dark-adaptation function. Perceptive field sizes of the four elemental hues (red, green, yellow, and blue) and the saturation component were estimated by three observers at each intensity level for each wavelength. In general, perceptive field sizes of blue and red are the smallest, and yellow and green are the largest. Furthermore, perceptive field sizes of all four hues decrease with increasing stimulus intensity, though the absolute change is largest for green and yellow. The decrease in size with increase in intensity cannot be completely explained in terms of saturation or rod signals and is likely, then, attributable to a cone-based mechanism.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lucy J Troup
- Department of Psychology, Colorado State University, Fort Collins 80523, USA.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
16
|
Osorio D, Smith AC, Vorobyev M, Buchanan-Smith HM. Detection of Fruit and the Selection of Primate Visual Pigments for Color Vision. Am Nat 2004; 164:696-708. [PMID: 29641923 DOI: 10.1086/425332] [Citation(s) in RCA: 159] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/03/2022]
Abstract
Primates have X chromosome genes for cone photopigments with sensitivity maxima from 535 to 562 nm. Old World monkeys and apes (catarrhines) and the New World (platyrrhine) genus Alouatta have separate genes for 535-nm (medium wavelength; M) and 562-nm (long wavelength; L) pigments. These pigments, together with a 425-nm (short wavelength) pigment, permit trichromatic color vision. Other platyrrhines and prosimians have a single X chromosome gene but often with alleles for two or three M/L photopigments. Consequently, heterozygote females are trichromats, but males and homozygote females are dichromats. The criteria that affect the evolution of M/L alleles and maintain genetic polymorphism remain a puzzle, but selection for finding food may be important. We compare different types of color vision for detecting more than 100 plant species consumed by tamarins (Saguinus spp.) in Peru. There is evidence that both frequency-dependent selection on homozygotes and heterozygote advantage favor M/L polymorphism and that trichromatic color vision is most advantageous in dim light. Also, whereas the 562-nm allele is present in all species, the occurrence of 535- to 556-nm alleles varies between species. This variation probably arises because trichromatic color vision favors widely separated pigments and equal frequencies of 535/543- and 562-nm alleles, whereas in dichromats, long-wavelength pigment alleles are fitter.
Collapse
|
17
|
Diller L, Packer OS, Verweij J, McMahon MJ, Williams DR, Dacey DM. L and M cone contributions to the midget and parasol ganglion cell receptive fields of macaque monkey retina. J Neurosci 2004; 24:1079-88. [PMID: 14762126 PMCID: PMC6793593 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.3828-03.2004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 73] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
Abstract
Analysis of cone inputs to primate parvocellular ganglion cells suggests that red-green spectral opponency results when connections segregate input from long wavelength (L) or middle wavelength (M) sensitive cones to receptive field centers and surrounds. However, selective circuitry is not an obvious retinal feature. Rather, cone receptive field surrounds and H1 horizontal cells get mixed L and M cone input, likely indiscriminately sampled from the randomly arranged cones of the photoreceptor mosaic. Red-green spectral opponency is consistent with random connections in central retina where the mixed cone ganglion cell surround is opposed by a single cone input to the receptive field center, but not in peripheral retina where centers get multiple cone inputs. The selective and random connection hypotheses might be reconciled if cone type selective circuitry existed in inner retina. If so, the segregation of L and M cone inputs to receptive field centers and surrounds would increase from horizontal to ganglion cell, and opponency would remain strong in peripheral retina. We measured the relative strengths of L and M cone inputs to H1 horizontal cells and parasol and midget ganglion cells by recording intracellular physiological responses from morphologically identified neurons in an in vitro preparation of the macaque monkey retina. The relative strength of L and M cone inputs to H1 and ganglion cells at the same locations matched closely. Peripheral midget cells were nonopponent. These results suggest that peripheral H1 and ganglion cells inherit their L and M cone inputs from the photoreceptor mosaic unmodified by selective circuitry.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lisa Diller
- Department of Biological Structure, University of Washington, Seattle, Washington 98195, USA
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
18
|
Gunther KL, Dobkins KR. Individual differences in chromatic (red/green) contrast sensitivity are constrained by the relative number of L- versus M-cones in the eye. Vision Res 2002; 42:1367-78. [PMID: 12044743 DOI: 10.1016/s0042-6989(02)00043-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 39] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
Many previous studies have shown that the relative number of long-wavelength-selective (L) versus medium-wavelength-selective (M) cones in the eye influences spectral sensitivity revealed perceptually. Here, we hypothesize that the L:M cone ratio should also influence red/green chromatic contrast sensitivity. To test this, in each subject we derived an estimate of L:M ratio based on her red/green equiluminance settings (obtained with heterochromatic flicker photometry), and measured both red/green chromatic and luminance contrast sensitivity at different spatial and temporal frequencies. Factor analysis was applied to the data in order to reveal covariance between conditions. As expected, chromatic and luminance contrast sensitivity were found to be independent of one another, and no relationship was observed between L:M ratio and luminance contrast sensitivity. However, a significant relationship was observed between L:M ratio and chromatic contrast sensitivity, wherein subjects possessing the most symmetrical L:M cone ratios (i.e., near 1:1) appear to possess the relatively greatest chromatic contrast sensitivity. This relationship can be accounted for by a simple model based on the notion of random L- and M-cone inputs to the center and surround receptive fields of chromatic (L-M) mechanisms.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Karen L Gunther
- Department of Psychology, University of California, San Diego 0109, La Jolla, CA 92093, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
19
|
Abstract
In this study, we have compared foveal SF discriminations for luminance and color-defined stimuli using two different tasks (criteria): in criterion-A, the discrimination is based on spatial (size of the stimuli) and/or spatial frequency; in criterion-B, it is based on apparent motion (contraction/expansion). We used high contrast (75%) spatially localized D6 stimuli and cosine gratings (0.25-9.5 cpd). The SF discrimination was measured by the method of constant stimuli with a two-interval forced-choice procedure. Data show that: (i) for criterion-A, the discrimination thresholds for color stimuli were lower than that for luminance stimuli at low SFs, but similar or higher at higher SFs; for criterion-B, the thresholds to chromatic stimuli were higher than that to achromatic stimuli for all SFs; (ii) SF discrimination was best at inter-stimulus-interval (ISI) of about 200 ms for color stimuli and at ISI of 0 ms for luminance stimuli; (iii) SF discrimination got better with stimulus duration and reached to plateau at 200 ms (or more) for color stimuli and at 67 ms (or more) for luminance stimuli; (iv) SF discrimination threshold (mean Delta(f)=0.19 octaves) is about one-tenth of the full bandwidth (mean=1.96 octaves) of SF tuned mechanisms and is in hyperacuity range; both (discrimination and hyperacuity) can be explained by the relative activities within a population of tuned mechanisms. We conclude that color and luminance SF discrimination thresholds have a different SF dependence. While color appears to perform better than luminance vision at low SFs, this effect is lost or even reversed at high SFs. Data imply that color and form interact, but color and motion are largely segregated (i.e. they weakly interact).
Collapse
|
20
|
Bouman MA. Spatiotemporal configuration dependent pairing of nerve events in dark-adapted human vision. JOURNAL OF THE OPTICAL SOCIETY OF AMERICA. A, OPTICS, IMAGE SCIENCE, AND VISION 2002; 19:241-253. [PMID: 11822587 DOI: 10.1364/josaa.19.000241] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/23/2023]
Abstract
In the model presented here, in the dark any single quantum absorption in a rod or cone produces a subliminal excitation. Subliminal excitations from both halves of a twin unit pair in the retina for the perception of light from the stimulus. A twin unit contains either two red or two green cones. The twin units are intertwined in triples of two red units and one green unit in a hexagon called a trion. P satellite rods surround each cone, P being approximately proportional to the square of eccentricity. A successful pairing for light perception represents-through the points of time and locations of the creation of its partners in the retina--a direction event with two possible polarities and with the orientation of the elongated shape of the twin unit. The polarity of the event depends on which of the two partners arrives first at the twin's pairing facility. Simultaneous events and successive events with the same polarity in adjacent units that are aligned along one of the three orientations of the hexagonal retinal mosaic pair in the cortex for the perception of edge and of movement. Inter-twin pairing products of the three differently oriented sets of aligned twins are independent of each other and sum vectorially in the cortex. This system of three sub-retinas is called the retrinet. Two one-quantum excitations in any of a twin's receptors make the percept colored. The odd blue cone produces already a blue signal for a single one-quantum excitation. Intra-receptor pairing in a rod, a red cone and a green cone is for white, red, and green respectively. Red and green cone products of a trion cross-pair in the retina and produce a yellow signal. Red and green cone products of a hexagon of adjacent trions cross-pair in the cortex and produce a white signal. This large hexagon with a total of seven trions is called a persepton. After subliminal excitations in a twin have paired successfully, further subliminal receptor excitations in neighboring and aligned twins are expressed to a certain extent in the percept's area, duration and color. Earlier experiments on absolute and color thresholds are the basis for this theory, which is developed in this paper.
Collapse
|
21
|
Vimal RP. Spatial-frequency-tuned mechanisms of the red-green channel estimated by oblique masking. JOURNAL OF THE OPTICAL SOCIETY OF AMERICA. A, OPTICS, IMAGE SCIENCE, AND VISION 2002; 19:276-288. [PMID: 11822590 DOI: 10.1364/josaa.19.000276] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/23/2023]
Abstract
The sustained spatial-frequency-tuned (SF-tuned) mechanisms of nonoriented units were examined by means of orthogonal masking for the Red-Green (R-G) color channel, and those of oriented units by oblique masking for the achromatic channel but not for the color channels. An oblique-masking technique minimizes the artifacts that are due to spatial phase effects, local cues, spatial beats, spatial probability summation, and changing criteria. Therefore the spatial characteristics of the R-G color channel are now investigated by an oblique-masking technique and linked with my paper on orthogonal masking [J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 15, 1 (1998)]. The R-G channel was defined by the minimum-flicker and hue-cancellation techniques. A color monitor system was used to generate spatially localized (D6) vertical color test patterns [0.063-8 cycles per degree (cpd)] and sinusoidal oblique color masks (0.031-16 cpd, 1.2-60% contrasts). Color contrast sensitivity functions (CSFs), threshold elevation (TE) versus mask SF (TvSF) curves, and TE versus mask contrast (TvC) curves were measured by the method of constant stimuli with a two-interval forced-choice technique by using Powell's achromatizing lens under sustained (Gaussian, 2-s-duration) conditions. Results show the following: (1) The color CSF is a low-pass function of SF with average half-height SF of 0.7 cpd and cutoff SF of 14 cpd with the use of a color-detection criterion. (2) TvSF curves are broadly bandpass and fall into five groups, peaking at approximately 0.13, 0.5, 2, 4, and 8 cpd. The root-mean-square cone-color CSF is 3.8-5.4 times the stimulus-color CSF. (3) A "crowding effect" similar to that of the TvSF curves of the achromatic channel was also found, but the TvSF curves of the R-G channel are not sharply peaked, similar to the result for orthogonal masking. Data analysis led to the following conclusions: (1) A simple multiple-mechanism model yields one low-pass color mechanism (with average half-height SF of 0.54 cpd) and five bandpass SF-tuned color mechanisms; these six mechanisms are necessary to explain the CSF, TvSF, and TvC data simultaneously. (2) The bandpass mechanisms peaked at approximately 0.13, 0.5, 2, 4, and 8 cpd with average full bandwidths at half-heights of 3.6, 3.2, 2.1, 1.2, and 1.3 octaves, respectively. (3) Since oblique-masking color mechanisms (unlike achromatic oriented mechanisms) have broad orientation tuning under sustained conditions and there is a significant orthogonal masking, the oblique-masking color mechanisms may have contributions from both oriented and nonoriented units. (4) The high degree of similarity between the SF-tuned filters of mechanisms derived from oblique- and orthogonal-masking data suggests that most of the chromatic SF tuning is already accomplished by nonoriented units. (5) The quality of the fit to oblique- and orthogonal-masking data combined dropped enough to reject the hypothesis that the former taps the performance of only the same nonoriented mechanisms as those by the latter. Adding gain parameters that reduce the TEs for orthogonal masking gave a better fit, suggesting that orientation gains are one of the factors involved in the transformation of information from nonoriented to oriented mechanisms. However, the fit was still worse than that for oblique-(6) Since masking-alone or orthogonal-masking-alone data, suggesting that more factors may be involved. primate parvo lateral geniculate nucleus (pLGN) units behave in a fairly linear manner, the color contrast nonlinearity (which follows the linear filter) of a mechanism may be post-pLGN.
Collapse
|
22
|
Abstract
The contrast matching function (CMF) is the reciprocal of test contrast that perceptually matches the contrast of standard pattern, measured as a function of test spatial frequency (SF). Achromatic CMFs usually flatten as the contrast of the standard is raised, and are broader than the achromatic, bandpass, contrast sensitivity function (CSF). This report investigates whether chromatic CMFs have similar characteristics. For this purpose, the red-green color channel was defined using minimum flicker and hue cancellation techniques. Spatially localized (D6), vertical, equiluminant patterns (SFs: 0.063-8 cpd; contrast: 3-80%) were used to measure the CSF and CMF of isoluminant patterns presented with a temporal Gaussian envelope. CMFs were measured using a randomized double-staircase procedure and the two-interval forced choice technique. Two color-normal observers, whose task was to select the interval that had higher color contrast, participated in experiments. Results show that: (a) the color CMFs are lowpass functions of SF at low standard contrasts (3-12.5%), broad-bandpass at intermediate contrasts (6.25-60%), and near-flat at high contrasts (80%); and (b) isoluminant CMFs have higher upper cut-off frequencies than isoluminant CSFs. It is concluded that: (i) color-contrast-constancy (CMF independent of SF) is partly achieved at high contrasts because color CMFs flatten as contrast increases; (ii) the information processing at suprathreshold levels is different from that at the threshold levels; and (iii) the model that explained achromatic CMFs using achromatic threshold mechanisms could not explain chromatic CMFs using chromatic threshold mechanisms.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- R L Vimal
- Vision Research Institute, 102 Maple St., 02420-2544, Lexington, MA, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Webster MA, Miyahara E, Malkoc G, Raker VE. Variations in normal color vision. I. Cone-opponent axes. JOURNAL OF THE OPTICAL SOCIETY OF AMERICA. A, OPTICS, IMAGE SCIENCE, AND VISION 2000; 17:1535-1544. [PMID: 10975363 DOI: 10.1364/josaa.17.001535] [Citation(s) in RCA: 30] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/23/2023]
Abstract
Early postreceptoral color vision is thought to be organized in terms of two principal axes corresponding to opposing L- and M-cone signals (LvsM) or to S-cone signals opposed by a combination of L- and M-cone signals (SvsLM). These cone-opponent axes are now widely used in studies of color vision, but in most cases the corresponding stimulus variations are defined only theoretically, based on a standard observer. We examined the range and implications of interobserver variations in the cone-opponent axes. We used chromatic adaptation to empirically define the LvsM and SvsLM axes and used both thresholds and color contrast adaptation to determine sensitivity to the axes. We also examined the axis variations implied by individual differences in the color matching data of Stiles and Burch [Opt. Acta 6, 1 (1959)]. The axes estimated for individuals can differ measurably from the nominal standard-observer axes and can influence the interpretation of postreceptoral color organization (e.g., regarding interactions between the two axes). Thus, like luminance sensitivity, individual differences in chromatic sensitivity may be important to consider in studies of the cone-opponent axes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- M A Webster
- Department of Psychology, University of Nevada, Reno 89557, USA.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
24
|
Stockman A, Sharpe LT. The spectral sensitivities of the middle- and long-wavelength-sensitive cones derived from measurements in observers of known genotype. Vision Res 2000; 40:1711-37. [PMID: 10814758 DOI: 10.1016/s0042-6989(00)00021-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 384] [Impact Index Per Article: 16.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
The spectral sensitivities of middle- (M-) and long- (L-) wavelength-sensitive cones have been measured in dichromats of known genotype: M-cone sensitivities in nine protanopes, and L-cone sensitivities in 20 deuteranopes. We have used these dichromat cone spectral sensitivities, along with new luminous efficiency determinations, and existing spectral sensitivity and color matching data from normal trichromats, to derive estimates of the human M- and L-cone spectral sensitivities for 2 and 10 degrees dia. central targets, and an estimate of the photopic luminosity function [V(lambda)] for 2 degrees dia. targets, which we refer to as V(2)*(lambda). These new estimates are consistent with dichromatic and trichromatic spectral sensitivities and color matches.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- A Stockman
- Department of Psychology 0109, University of California San Diego, 9500 Gilman Drive, La Jolla, CA, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
25
|
Abstract
The spatial arrangement of L and M cones in the human peripheral retina was estimated from red-green color naming of small test flashes (0.86 min of arc, 555 nm, constant intensity) presented at different locations (grid with 1.5 min of arc steps) centered at 17 degrees temporal eccentricity. Simulated red-green color naming ratings were generated by a model based on an ideal observer for all possible patterns of placement and relative numerosities of L and M cones, constrained by the anatomical data on the statistics of cone spacing at this retinal location. The best matching simulated performance as compared to the human observer's data determined the cone array most likely to produce that observer's color naming results. The mosaics for two color normal observers showed L and M cones randomly arrayed over this retinal region. Consequences of random cone placements for spectral sampling and color opponency are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- S Otake
- Department of Cognitive Sciences, University of California, Irvine 92697-5100, USA
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
26
|
Dobkins KR, Gunther KL, Peterzell DH. What covariance mechanisms underlie green/red equiluminance, luminance contrast sensitivity and chromatic (green/red) contrast sensitivity? Vision Res 2000; 40:613-28. [PMID: 10824265 DOI: 10.1016/s0042-6989(99)00211-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 38] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
In order to investigate the mechanisms underlying green/red equiluminance matches in human observers and their relationship to mechanisms subserving luminance and/or chromatic (green/red) contrast sensitivity, we tested 21 human subjects along these dimensions at 16 different spatial and temporal frequencies (spatial frequency, 0.25-2 c/deg; temporal frequency, 2-16 Hz) and applied factor analysis to extract mechanisms underlying the data set. The results from our factor analysis revealed separate sources of variability for green/red equiluminance, luminance sensitivity and chromatic sensitivity, thus suggesting separate mechanisms underlying each of the three main conditions. When factor analysis was applied separately to green/red equiluminance data, two temporally-tuned factors were revealed (factor 1, 2-4 Hz; factor 2, 8-16 Hz), suggesting the existence of separate mechanisms underlying equiluminance settings at low versus high temporal frequencies. In addition, although the three main conditions remained separate in our factor analysis of the entire data set, our correlation matrix nonetheless revealed systematic correlations between equiluminance settings and luminance sensitivity at high temporal frequencies, and between equiluminance settings and chromatic sensitivity at low temporal frequencies. Taken together, these data suggest that the high temporal frequency factor underlying green/red equiluminance is governed predominantly by luminance mechanisms, while the low temporal frequency factor receives contribution from chromatic mechanisms.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- K R Dobkins
- Department of Psychology, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla 92093, USA.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
27
|
Kremers J, Scholl HP, Knau H, Berendschot TT, Usui T, Sharpe LT. L/M cone ratios in human trichromats assessed by psychophysics, electroretinography, and retinal densitometry. JOURNAL OF THE OPTICAL SOCIETY OF AMERICA. A, OPTICS, IMAGE SCIENCE, AND VISION 2000; 17:517-526. [PMID: 10708033 DOI: 10.1364/josaa.17.000517] [Citation(s) in RCA: 56] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/23/2023]
Abstract
Estimates of the relative numbers of long-wavelength-sensitive (L) and middle-wavelength-sensitive (M) cones vary considerably among normal trichromats and depend significantly on the nature of the experimental method employed. Here we estimate L/M cone ratios in a population of normal observers, using three psychophysical tasks-detection thresholds for cone-isolating stimuli at different temporal frequencies, heterochromatic flicker photometry, and cone contrast ratios at minimal flicker perception--as well as flicker electroretinography and retinal densitometry. The psychophysical tasks involving high temporal frequencies, specifically designed to tap into the luminance channel, provide average L/M cone ratios that significantly differ from unity with large interindividual variation. In contrast, the psychophysical tasks involving low temporal frequencies, chosen to tap into the red-green chromatic channel, provide L/M cone ratios that are always close to unity. L/M cone ratios determined from electroretinographic recordings or from retinal densitometry correlate with those determined from the high-temporal-frequency tasks. These findings suggest that the sensitivity of the luminance channel is directly related to the relative densities of the L and the M cones and that the red-green chromatic channel introduces a gain adjustment to compensate for differences in L and M cone signal strength.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- J Kremers
- Department of Experimental Ophthalmology, University of Tübingen Eye Hospital, Germany.
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
28
|
Carroll J, McMahon C, Neitz M, Neitz J. Flicker-photometric electroretinogram estimates of L:M cone photoreceptor ratio in men with photopigment spectra derived from genetics. JOURNAL OF THE OPTICAL SOCIETY OF AMERICA. A, OPTICS, IMAGE SCIENCE, AND VISION 2000; 17:499-509. [PMID: 10708031 DOI: 10.1364/josaa.17.000499] [Citation(s) in RCA: 45] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/23/2023]
Abstract
Relative proportions of long-wavelength-sensitive (L) to middle-wavelength-sensitive (M) cones were estimated by use of the flicker-photometric electroretinogram (ERG). It has been demonstrated that a major source of error in estimates of cone proportions from spectral luminosity functions is the known variation in the lambda(max) of the photopigments [Vision Res. 38, 1961 (1998)]. To correct for these errors, estimates of cone proportions were derived by use of individualized L-cone spectral sensitivity curves deduced from photopigment gene sequences from each subject. For some individuals this correction made a large difference in the estimated cone proportions compared with the value obtained when a fixed standard L cone was assumed. The largest discrepancy occurred in a man estimated to have 62% L cones (L:M ratio 1.6:1) when a standard L pigment was assumed but a value of 80% L cones (L:M ratio 4:1) when his individualized L-cone spectrum was used. From repeated measurements made with the ERG, it was determined that individual estimates of the relative L-to-M cone contributions, expressed as %L cones, are usually reliable within approximately 2%. The average L:M ratio for 15 male subjects was estimated at 2:1 (67% L cones). Previously, a large range of individual variability was reported for L:M ratios obtained from photometry. An unresolved issue concerns how much of the range might be attributed to error. Here efforts have been taken to markedly reduce measurement error. Nonetheless, a large range of individual differences persists. Estimated L:M ratios for individuals ranged from 0.6:1 to 12:1 (40% L to 92% L).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- J Carroll
- Department of Ophthalmology, Medical College of Wisconsin, Milwaukee 53226-0509, USA
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
29
|
Otake S, Cicerone CM. L and M cone relative numerosity and red-green opponency from fovea to midperiphery in the human retina. JOURNAL OF THE OPTICAL SOCIETY OF AMERICA. A, OPTICS, IMAGE SCIENCE, AND VISION 2000; 17:615-627. [PMID: 10708043 DOI: 10.1364/josaa.17.000615] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/23/2023]
Abstract
The relative numerosity of the long-wavelength-sensitive (L) and middle-wavelength-sensitive (M) cones and the red-green color appearance, as assessed by means of unique yellow, are stable from fovea to midperiphery (+/- 28 deg nasotemporal). As foveal tests decrease in size, unique yellow progressively shifts toward longer wavelengths, favoring a model of red-green opponency carried by cells whose centers receive input from either L or M cones and whose surrounds receive mixed contributions from both. Individual differences in unique yellow over a 20-nm range and the relative numerosity of L and M cones can be linked by means of this model, suggesting that the relative number of L and M cones is a factor that regulates individual variations in red-green color appearance.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- S Otake
- Department of Cognitive Sciences, University of California, Irvine 92697, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
30
|
Volbrecht VJ, Shrago EE, Schefrin BE, Werner JS. Spatial summation in human cone mechanisms from 0 degrees to 20 degrees in the superior retina. JOURNAL OF THE OPTICAL SOCIETY OF AMERICA. A, OPTICS, IMAGE SCIENCE, AND VISION 2000; 17:641-50. [PMID: 10708046 PMCID: PMC2698456 DOI: 10.1364/josaa.17.000641] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/23/2023]
Abstract
The maximum area of complete spatial summation (i.e., Ricco's area) for human short-wavelength-sensitive-(S-) and long-wavelength-sensitive- (L-) cone mechanisms was measured psychophysically at the fovea and at 1.5 degrees , 4 degrees , 8 degrees , and 20 degrees along the vertical meridian in the superior retina. Increment thresholds were measured for three observers by a temporal two-alternative forced-choice procedure. Test stimuli ranging from -0.36 to 4.61 log area (min2) were presented on concentric 12.3 degrees adapting and auxiliary fields, which isolated either an S- or an L-cone mechanism on the plateau of its respective threshold versus intensity function. Test flash durations were 50 and 10 ms for the S- and L-cone mechanisms, respectively. The data indicate that, from 0 degrees to 20 degrees, Ricco's area increases monotonically for the L-cone mechanism, is variable for the S-cone mechanism, and is larger for the S-cone mechanism than for the L-cone mechanism for essentially all retinal locations. This pattern of results most likely reflects differences in ganglion cell density and changes in neural convergence with retinal eccentricity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- V J Volbrecht
- Department of Psychology, Colorado State University, Fort Collins 80523-1876, USA.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
31
|
Krauskopf J. Relative number of long- and middle-wavelength-sensitive cones in the human fovea. JOURNAL OF THE OPTICAL SOCIETY OF AMERICA. A, OPTICS, IMAGE SCIENCE, AND VISION 2000; 17:510-516. [PMID: 10708032 DOI: 10.1364/josaa.17.000510] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/23/2023]
Abstract
Flicker photometric measurements yield spectral sensitivity curves that are well fitted by sums of the spectral sensitivity curves of long-wavelength-sensitive (L) cones and middle-wavelength-sensitive (M) cones if the L cones are given twice the weight of the M cones. This result has been interpreted as implying that L cones are more numerous than M cones but is also consistent with a different numerical ratio, say, 1:1, and with the assignment of greater weight to the L cone input than to the M cone input by the mechanism subserving flicker photometry. Measurements of temporal sensitivity are presented for lights that modulate the inputs of either only the L cones or only the M cones. Sensitivity to modulation of the L cones is approximately twice that of modulation of the M cones at approximately 30 Hz, but that advantage disappears at approximately 2 Hz. Thus flicker sensitivity is equivocal with regard to cone numerosity. Electrophysiological, anatomical, and psychophysical evidence is reviewed, with particular weight placed on the statistics of color appearance of small, brief, monochromatic lights and on increment thresholds measured on the same observers. It is concluded that, in the central fovea, the ratio of L:M cone numbers is close to unity and may not be so variable as is usually supposed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- J Krauskopf
- Center for Neural Science, New York University, New York 10003, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
32
|
Dobkins KR, Thiele A, Albright TD. Comparison of red-green equiluminance points in humans and macaques: evidence for different L:M cone ratios between species. JOURNAL OF THE OPTICAL SOCIETY OF AMERICA. A, OPTICS, IMAGE SCIENCE, AND VISION 2000; 17:545-556. [PMID: 10708036 DOI: 10.1364/josaa.17.000545] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/23/2023]
Abstract
The human spectral luminosity function (V(lambda)) can be modeled as the linear sum of signals from long-wavelength-selective (L) and middle-wavelength-selective (M) cones, with L cones being weighted by a factor of approximately 2. This factor of approximately 2 is thought to reflect an approximate 2:1 ratio of L:M cones in the human retina, which has been supported by studies that allow for more direct counting of different cone types in the retina. In contrast to humans, several lines of retinally based evidence in macaques suggest an L:M ratio closer to 1:1. To investigate the consequences of differences in L:M cone ratios between humans and macaques, red-green equiluminance matches obtained psychophysically in humans (n = 11) were compared with those obtained electrophysiologically from single neurons in the extrastriate middle temporal visual area of macaques (M. mulatta, n = 5). Neurons in the middle temporal visual area were tested with sinusoidal red-green moving gratings across a range of luminance contrasts, with equiluminance being defined as the red-green contrast yielding a response minimum. Human subjects were tested under analogous conditions, by a minimally distinct motion technique, to establish psychophysical equiluminance. Although red-green equiluminance points in both humans and macaques were found to vary across individuals, the means across species differed significantly; compared with humans, macaque equiluminance points reflected relatively greater sensitivity to green. By means of a simple model based on equating the weighted sum of L and M cone signals, the observed red-green equiluminance points were found to be consistent with L:M cone ratios of approximately 2:1 in humans and 1:1 in macaques. These data thus support retinally based estimates of L:M cone ratios and further demonstrate that the information carried in the cone mosaic has functional consequences for red-green spectral sensitivity revealed perceptually and in the dorsal stream of visual cortex.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- K R Dobkins
- Department of Psychology, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla 92093, USA.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
33
|
Brainard DH, Roorda A, Yamauchi Y, Calderone JB, Metha A, Neitz M, Neitz J, Williams DR, Jacobs GH. Functional consequences of the relative numbers of L and M cones. JOURNAL OF THE OPTICAL SOCIETY OF AMERICA. A, OPTICS, IMAGE SCIENCE, AND VISION 2000; 17:607-614. [PMID: 10708042 DOI: 10.1364/josaa.17.000607] [Citation(s) in RCA: 108] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/23/2023]
Abstract
Direct imaging of the retina by adaptive optics allows assessment of the relative number of long-wavelength-sensitive (L) and middle-wavelength-sensitive (M) cones in living human eyes. We examine the functional consequences of variation in the relative numbers of L and M cones (L/M cone ratio) for two observers whose ratios were measured by direct imaging. The L/M cone ratio for the two observers varied considerably, taking on values of 1.15 and 3.79. Two sets of functional data were collected: spectral sensitivity measured with the flicker electroretinogram (ERG) and the wavelength of unique yellow. A genetic analysis was used to determine L and M cone spectra appropriate for each observer. Rayleigh matches confirmed the use of these spectra. We determined the relative strength of L and M cone contributions to ERG spectral sensitivity by fitting the data with a weighted sum of L and M cone spectra. The relative strengths so determined (1.06 and 3.38) were close to the cone ratios established by direct imaging. Thus variation in L/M cone ratio is preserved at the sites tapped by the flicker ERG. The wavelength of unique yellow varied only slightly between the two observers (576.8 and 574.7 nm). This small variation indicates that neural factors play an important role in stabilizing unique yellow against variation in the L/M cone ratio.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- D H Brainard
- Department of Psychology and Neuroscience Research Institute, University of California, Santa Barbara 93106, USA.
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
34
|
Deeb SS, Diller LC, Williams DR, Dacey DM. Interindividual and topographical variation of L:M cone ratios in monkey retinas. JOURNAL OF THE OPTICAL SOCIETY OF AMERICA. A, OPTICS, IMAGE SCIENCE, AND VISION 2000; 17:538-544. [PMID: 10708035 DOI: 10.1364/josaa.17.000538] [Citation(s) in RCA: 28] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/23/2023]
Abstract
We analyzed the ratio of L:M cone photopigment mRNA in the retinas of Old World monkeys, using the method of rapid polymerase chain reaction-single-strand conformation polymorphism. The L:M cone pigment mRNA ratio in whole retina ranged from 0.6 to 7.0, with a mean of approximately 1.6 (standard deviation, +/- 0.56; n = 26). There was no change in this ratio with eccentricity up to 9 mm (approximately 45 degrees), though the ratio was approximately 30% greater in temporal than in nasal retina. The mRNA ratios are in good agreement with the L:M cone ratio in these same retinas, inferred from electrophysiological recordings of cone signal gain in horizontal cell interneurons. The correlation between mRNA ratios and physiological cone gain ratio supports the conclusion that both measures reflect the relative number of L and M cones.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- S S Deeb
- Department of Medicine, University of Washington, Seattle 98195, USA.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
35
|
|
36
|
Abstract
Human colour vision depends on three classes of receptor, the short- (S), medium- (M), and long- (L) wavelength-sensitive cones. These cone classes are interleaved in a single mosaic so that, at each point in the retina, only a single class of cone samples the retinal image. As a consequence, observers with normal trichromatic colour vision are necessarily colour blind on a local spatial scale. The limits this places on vision depend on the relative numbers and arrangement of cones. Although the topography of human S cones is known, the human L- and M-cone submosaics have resisted analysis. Adaptive optics, a technique used to overcome blur in ground-based telescopes, can also overcome blur in the eye, allowing the sharpest images ever taken of the living retina. Here we combine adaptive optics and retinal densitometry to obtain what are, to our knowledge, the first images of the arrangement of S, M and L cones in the living human eye. The proportion of L to M cones is strikingly different in two male subjects, each of whom has normal colour vision. The mosaics of both subjects have large patches in which either M or L cones are missing. This arrangement reduces the eye's ability to recover colour variations of high spatial frequency in the environment but may improve the recovery of luminance variations of high spatial frequency.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- A Roorda
- Center for Visual Science, University of Rochester, New York 14627, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
37
|
Abstract
To measure cone signal strengths in the flicker electroretinogram (ERG) of dichromats and trichromats, we developed a set of flickering stimuli (30 Hz), which excite the middle-wavelength-sensitive (M-) and long-wavelength-sensitive (L-) cones independently. ERG responses to eight different ratios of L- to M-cone contrasts were recorded from each subject. The short-wavelength-sensitive (S-) cone contrast was 0% in all measurements. The recordings were Fourier analyzed to determine the amplitude of the fundamental component. ERG threshold values for each subject resulted in ellipses when plotted in an L-/M-cone contrast space. As expected, the orientations of the threshold ellipses of the protanopes (N = 2) were parallel to the L-cone axis, whereas those of the deuteranopes (N = 2) were parallel to the M-cone axis. For the trichromats (N = 5), there was considerable interindividual variation in ellipse orientation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- T Usui
- Department of Experimental Ophthalmology, University Eye Hospital, Tübingen, Germany
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
38
|
Gowdy PD, Cicerone CM. The spatial arrangement of the L and M cones in the central fovea of the living human eye. Vision Res 1998; 38:2575-89. [PMID: 12116704 DOI: 10.1016/s0042-6989(97)00416-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
Experiments designed to estimate the placement of L and M cones in fovea centralis of the living human eye are presented. Hyperacuity performances for two observers were measured for the full and the separate L and M cone submosaics using 2-dot chromatic stimuli on cone-selective adapting backgrounds. Simulated performances, based on an ideal observer model, were generated for all possible mosaics by varying L and M cone relative numerosity and spatial configuration. The best match between the simulated and measured performances determined the solution mosaic. Each observer's solution mosaic contained more L than M cones, randomly arrayed as assessed by statistical tests.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- P D Gowdy
- Department of Cognitive Sciences, University of California, Irvine, Irvine, CA 92697, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
39
|
Vimal RL. Color-luminance interaction: data produced by oblique cross masking. JOURNAL OF THE OPTICAL SOCIETY OF AMERICA. A, OPTICS, IMAGE SCIENCE, AND VISION 1998; 15:1756-1766. [PMID: 9656476 DOI: 10.1364/josaa.15.001756] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/22/2023]
Abstract
Threshold-elevation (TE-) versus-mask-spatial-frequency (SF) curves and TE-versus-mask-contrast curves, produced by the oblique-masking technique, were reported for uncrossed stimuli (color-test-on-color-mask and luminance-test-on-luminance-mask) [Invest. Ophthalmol. Visual Sci. Suppl. 34, 751 (1993) and Vision. Res. 23, 873 (1983)]. The technique minimizes the artifacts that are due to spatial phase effects, spatial beats, spatial probability summation, and local cues. My goal was to measure these curves for crossed stimuli (color-test-on-luminance-mask and luminance-test-on-color-mask) by this oblique-masking technique and to compare the curves with those reported in previous studies. For this purpose threshold contrasts were measured by a yes-no procedure with randomized double staircases. Test targets were vertical spatially localized (D6) patterns, and masks were oblique sinusoidal patterns; both the test and the mask were presented simultaneously, for 2 s (Gaussian window), on a color monitor interfaced with an ATVista system and a Powell achromatizing lens. The test SF's were 0.125, 0.5, 2, 4, and 8 cycles per degree (cpd); mask SF's were 0.031-16 cpd; and mask contrasts were 6.25%-50%. Furthermore, the Red-Green channel was defined by the minimum flicker and the hue cancellation techniques. Results show mostly masking effect (TE > 1) at contrasts above threshold; sometimes, separability (TE = 1) and above-threshold facilitation (TE < 1) effects were also observed, depending on the test SF, the mask SF, the mask contrast, and the subject. In general, the magnitudes of TE's are smaller and the TE-versus-mask-SF curves are slightly narrower for the oblique-cross-masking conditions than those for the respective oblique uncross masking. In addition, the TE-versus-mask-contrast curves for the crossed conditions are mostly shallower than those for the respective uncrossed conditions. Furthermore, mostly the color-luminance asymmetry (color masks luminance more than luminance masks color) is found, in mild form, for SF's > or = 0.5 cpd. For the lower SF of 0.125 cpd, there is either a lack of asymmetry or a very mild asymmetry of the opposite kind (luminance masks color slightly more than color masks luminance) seems to prevail. In general, the oblique-masking data shows mild asymmetry and reduced facilitation; both are consistent with reduced local cues, similar to those shown by randomized phase data, thus making the data suitable for SF analysis; moreover, at high contrast, the masking data are consistent with those reported in previous studies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- R L Vimal
- Vision Research Institute, Lexington, Massachusetts 02173, USA
| |
Collapse
|
40
|
Bieber ML, Kraft JM, Werner JS. Effects of known variations in photopigments on L/M cone ratios estimated from luminous efficiency functions. Vision Res 1998; 38:1961-6. [PMID: 9797942 DOI: 10.1016/s0042-6989(97)00302-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
Abstract
The extent to which known variations in photopigment lambda max and optical density may affect cone ratios estimated from the spectral luminous efficiency function (LEF) was examined. LEFs were generated using L- and M-cone fundamentals, one of which had been shifted in lambda max (+/- 1, 2, 4 or 6 nm) or varied in peak optical density (increased or decreased by 10, 25 or 50%). A curve-fitting program was then used to estimate the L/M cone ratios for the generated LEFs assuming standard L- and M-cone fundamentals. These modeling exercises indicate that L/M cone ratios estimated from LEFs are highly correlated with long-wave sensitivity and with known variations in L-cone lambda max. Variations in M-cone lambda max and photopigment optical density for both cone types are also correlated with L/M cone ratios, but have much less impact on the estimated ratios.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- M L Bieber
- Department of Psychology, University of Colorado, Boulder 80309-0345, USA.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
41
|
Abstract
Thresholds for detecting brief, white, foveal test flashes drop abruptly within 0.2 sec of the offset of a white adapting field. The magnitude of the abrupt drop is proportional to the square root of field intensity (square root of I) correct for bleaching and dark light. Thresholds are then stable out to 1.6 sec for 200 msec tests, or recover only slightly for 20 msec tests. These results exclude some simple deterministic models in which Weber-like gain controls in the luminance pathway are assumed to recover exponentially in the dark, but can be explained parsimoniously if turning off the field abolishes photon-driven noise, improving the S/N ratio while leaving visual responsivity virtually unaltered. This theory was first put forward by Krauskopf and Reeves [(1980) Vision Research, 20, 193-196] for S-cone thresholds; it implies that the Weber law for increment thresholds is not due to a single gain control, but rather expresses the product of two distinct square root of I factors, adjustment of responsivity and photon-driven noise. Removal of the noise, not recovery of gain, permits thresholds to fall in early dark adaptation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- A Reeves
- Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, Boston, MA 02115, USA.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
42
|
Miyahara E, Pokorny J, Smith VC, Baron R, Baron E. Color vision in two observers with highly biased LWS/MWS cone ratios. Vision Res 1998; 38:601-12. [PMID: 9536382 DOI: 10.1016/s0042-6989(97)88334-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 61] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/07/2023]
Abstract
Two sisters, heterozygous carriers for congenital X-linked protanopia, were diagnosed as normal trichromats by the Rayleigh match on the anomaloscope. The heterozygous state was established by molecular analysis of their visual pigment genes. The normal color match establishes that the spectral sensitivities of their long-wavelength-sensitive (LWS) and middle-wavelength-sensitive (MWS) cone visual photopigments are within normal variability. Their FM 100-hue test error scores were low, demonstrating superior chromatic discrimination. Heterochromatic flicker photometric (HEP) spectral sensitivities were like those of protanopes. The estimated LWS/MWS cone ratios from the HFP data were 0.09/1 and 0.03/1, compared with ratios in the range of 0.6/1 to 10/1 for typical normal trichromats. Measurements of chromatic grating acuity on chromatically selective backgrounds were performed to study the cone mosaic. The data were consistent with a sparsity of LWS cones. Both protan carriers showed normal spectral sensitivities for all three cone types under cone isolating chromatic adaptation and normal three-peaked curves for increment thresholds on a white pedestal. Hue estimation, run on one carrier was normal. The equilibrium yellow locus was measured in the other carrier and was in the range of normal trichromats. The data indicate that normal color vision can occur even when the LWS/MWS cone ratio is quite abnormal.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- E Miyahara
- Department of Psychology, Faculty of Letters, Kyoto University, Japan.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
43
|
Vimal RL. Spatial-frequency tuning of sustained nonoriented units of the red-green channel. JOURNAL OF THE OPTICAL SOCIETY OF AMERICA. A, OPTICS, IMAGE SCIENCE, AND VISION 1998; 15:1-15. [PMID: 9459791 DOI: 10.1364/josaa.15.000001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/22/2023]
Abstract
The existence of nonoriented cells but not of sustained orthogonal masking for achromatic stimuli led to an investigation of the spatial-frequency (SF) tuning of sustained nonoriented color units. For this purpose the Red-Green channel was isolated by the minimum flicker and hue cancellation techniques. Chromatic contrast sensitivity functions (CSF's), threshold elevation (TE) curves, and contrast nonlinearities (TE-versus-mask-contrast curves) were measured with spatially localized vertical color tests and sinusoidal orthogonal color masks by the method of constant stimuli under Gaussian temporal presentation. Results show that (1) color CSF's are a low-pass function of SF, whereas TE curves are a bandpass function of mask SF, and (2) a minimum of six SF-tuned color mechanisms (one low pass and five bandpass functions of SF, with peak SF's of 0.13, 0.5, 2, 4, and 8 cycles per degree and bandwidths of 3.9, 4.4, 2.9, 2.1, 1.1, and 1.2 octaves), similar to oblique-masking color mechanisms, are extracted by the multiple-mechanisms model. These data imply that (1) most of the SF tuning of the broadly oriented color units is already present in the circularly symmetric units, and (2) the latter may be an input to the former.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- R L Vimal
- Vision Research Institute, Lexington, Massachusetts 02173, USA
| |
Collapse
|
44
|
Pandey Vimal RL. Orientation tuning of the spatial-frequency-tuned mechanisms of the red-green channel. JOURNAL OF THE OPTICAL SOCIETY OF AMERICA. A, OPTICS, IMAGE SCIENCE, AND VISION 1997; 14:2622-2632. [PMID: 9316276 DOI: 10.1364/josaa.14.002622] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/22/2023]
Abstract
Significant orthogonal masking for color stimuli [Invest. Ophthalmol. Visual Sci. 34, 782 (No. 405-54) (1993)] but not for achromatic stimuli [J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 1, 226 (1984)] under sustained presentation led us to investigate the orientation tuning of the spatial-frequency- (SF-) tuned color mechanisms. The Red-Green channel was isolated from the achromatic channel by the minimum flicker technique and from the Yellow-Blue channel by the hue cancellation technique. Contrast sensitivity functions, threshold elevation versus mask orientation curves (measured by orientation masking), and threshold elevation versus mask contrast curves were measured by the method of constant stimuli and a two-interval forced-choice technique on two normal observers. Test targets were spatially localized (D6), vertical color patterns, and masks were sinusoidal color patterns oriented 15 degrees-90 degrees from the vertical in 15 degrees steps and had the same SF's as those of test patterns. Mask contrasts were varied between 1.2% and 60%. The orientation tuning curves of the SF-tuned color mechanisms were extracted by obtaining the best fit to contrast sensitivity and threshold elevation data simultaneously at a given SF with use of the masking model. Results show that threshold elevations depend on test SF, mask SF, mask orientation, and mask contrast. Half-bandwidths at half-height (with respect to 15 degrees from the vertical) of threshold elevation versus mask orientation curves range from 90 degrees to 29 degrees depending on SF's. The slopes of threshold elevation versus mask contrast curves range from 0.76 to 0.29 on octave-octave coordinates depending on SF's. Orientation half-bandwidths at half-height of orientation tuning curves of the SF-tuned color mechanisms (1) range from 79 degrees to 28 degrees and (2) average from 68 degrees to 30 degrees for SF's 0.063-8 cycles per degree (cpd). Data suggest that the orientation tuning curves of the SF-tuned chromatic mechanisms are broader (except at 2 cpd) than those of the achromatic mechanisms (orientation half-bandwidths: 32 degrees-15 degrees for 0.5-11.3 cpd [J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 1, 226 (1984)]); moreover, the orientation bandwidths are SF dependent.
Collapse
|
45
|
Xu J, Pokorny J, Smith VC. Optical density of the human lens. JOURNAL OF THE OPTICAL SOCIETY OF AMERICA. A, OPTICS, IMAGE SCIENCE, AND VISION 1997; 14:953-960. [PMID: 9114506 DOI: 10.1364/josaa.14.000953] [Citation(s) in RCA: 42] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/22/2023]
Abstract
Optic disk reflectance was measured from 27 normal observers with their physiological lenses (aged 21-74 yr) and from two pseudophakic observers (aged 69 and 70 yr) with use of a Utrecht fundus reflection densitometer. Psychophysical heterochromatic flicker photometric luminance matches (10 degrees field) were obtained on the same group of the observers. A four-parameter model incorporating lens density, hemoglobin absorption, optic disk reflectance, and superficial stray light was used to fit the reflectometric data. A model incorporating lens density and the Judd revised spectral luminous-efficiency function was used to fit the psychophysical data. The lens-density spectrum used the two-factor aging model of Pokorny, et al. [Appl. Opt. 26, 1437 (1987)]. The lens density for each normal observer was estimated through a least-squares fitting procedure yielding an estimated lens age. For the reflectometric data the observer's chronological age agreed with estimated lens age with a correlation coefficient of 0.92. The reflectometric regression line underestimated chronological age by approximately 5 yr. The mean reflectance of the optic disk was 0.047 with standard error of the mean of 0.0044. Data from the pseudophakic observers were well described when corneal density was used to replace lens density. The lens density was also estimated from the psychophysical data. The observer's chronological age agreed with psychophysically estimated lens age with a correlation coefficient of 0.92. It was concluded that the in vivo lens density can be estimated from the reflectance spectrum measured off the optic disk. The reflectance spectrum of the optic disk was inferred to be close to spectrally neutral.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- J Xu
- University of Chicago, Illinois 60637, USA
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
46
|
Abstract
In order to better understand the nature of long-wavelength (L) and middle-wavelength (M) cone input into spectral sensitivity functions and determine the reliability with which it is possible to predict L:M cone inputs, we developed analytical methods to determine confidence intervals for L:M cone input for spectral sensitivity functions or data transformed to cone-contrast space. Spectral sensitivity functions measured by direct heterochromatic brightness matches are dominated by the L/M opponent channel over most of the spectral range. For detection of large/ long test stimuli, spectral sensitivity functions show a characteristic "notch" at the adapting wavelength, with the L/M opponent channel dominating most of the spectral range. Flicker increment threshold (FIT) spectral sensitivity functions display many of the characteristics of the luminance flicker mechanism described by Stromeyer et al. (1987). [Vision Research, 27, 1113-1137]. Previous modelling of FIT spectral sensitivity functions proposed a 2:1 L:M cone input for most of testing conditions. We show that FIT spectral sensitivity functions are dominated by L cones but show L cone suppression under bright red adapting fields. For the fitted spectral sensitivity functions or simulated data sets, we found small confidence intervals for L:M cone input into the L/M opponent channel and conclude that it is possible to reliably predict L:M cone input ratios. However, for similar data sets of additive spectral sensitivity functions, we found large confidence intervals for L:M cone input ratios and conclude that it is not possible reliably predict L:M cone input into the L/M non-opponent channel using available spectral sensitivity functions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- M Kalloniatis
- Department of Optometry and Vision Sciences, University of Melbourne, Parkville, Victoria, Australia.
| | | |
Collapse
|
47
|
Jacobs GH, Deegan JF, Moran JL. ERG measurements of the spectral sensitivity of common chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). Vision Res 1996; 36:2587-94. [PMID: 8917819 DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(95)00335-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 37] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/03/2023]
Abstract
The spectral sensitivity of the common chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) was measured with electroretinogram (ERG) flicker photometry. Chromatic adaptation conditions were used to establish the presence of S-, M- and L-cone pigments. Each of 26 chimpanzees showed substantial and approximately equivalent adaptational changes over the middle and long wavelengths implying an absence of any significant polymorphic variations in the M- and L-cone pigments. As inferred from ERG measurements, the S-cone pigment of the chimpanzee has a spectral peak of about 430 nm. Chimpanzee spectral sensitivity measurements were compared to those obtained from equivalently tested normal human trichromats. The spectral sensitivity of the two species is very similar, chimpanzees being slightly more sensitive to short wavelength lights and slightly less sensitive to long wavelength lights than human subjects. Curve-fitting analyses suggest that spectral filtering may be lower in the chimpanzee lens than it is in the human lens, and that the L/M cone ratio is lower in the chimpanzee.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- G H Jacobs
- Neuroscience Research Institute, Santa Barbara, CA, USA.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
48
|
Berendschot TT, van de Kraats J, van Norren D. Foveal cone mosaic and visual pigment density in dichromats. J Physiol 1996; 492 ( Pt 1):307-14. [PMID: 8730604 PMCID: PMC1158882 DOI: 10.1113/jphysiol.1996.sp021310] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/01/2023] Open
Abstract
1. Optical reflectance spectra of the fovea were measured in ten subjects with normal colour vision, ten protanopes and seven deuteranopes. Four conditions were used: perpendicular and oblique angle of incident and reflected light on the retina, both in a dark-adapted and a fully bleached state. 2. The spectra were analysed to assess the effects of dichromacy on the cone mosaic. A replacement model, i.e. one where the total number of cones remains unchanged and all cones are filled with a single type of pigment, was found to fit our data best. 3. The analysis of the spectral fundus reflectance also provided estimates for densities of photo-labile and photo-stable retinal pigments and fraction of long wavelength-sensitive (LWS) cones. Visual pigment density was 0.39 for protanopes and 0.42 for deuteranopes, significantly lower than the 0.57 found for colour normals. Macular pigment density was 0.54 for colour normals, 0.46 for protanopes and 0.42 for deuteranopes. 4. For colour normals the LWS cone fraction was 0.56, in agreement with psychophysical literature. The LWS cone fraction for protanopes was -0.04, and for deuteranopes 0.96, consistent with their Rayleigh matches.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- T T Berendschot
- Department of Ophthalmology, Utrecht University, The Netherlands
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
49
|
Chapter 1 Temporal resolution in visual perception. Perception 1996. [DOI: 10.1016/s1874-5822(96)80004-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register]
|
50
|
Cole GR, Hine TJ, McIlhagga W. Estimation of linear detection mechanisms for stimuli of medium spatial frequency. Vision Res 1994; 34:1267-78. [PMID: 8023436 DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(94)90202-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/28/2023]
Abstract
Detection thresholds were obtained for a circularly-symmetric Gabor profile and Craik-Cornsweet profiles presented on a large white adapting field. These stimuli possessed peak spatial power between 1 and 6 c/deg. Their contrast was represented in an L, M and S cone contrast space. Detection thresholds were obtained for many vectors close to specific but theoretically important planes within this space. These data were fitted with a model comprising independent mechanisms, each a weighted sum of cone contrasts. The fit revealed a chromatic mechanism driven by delta L/L-delta M/M with no S cone input. Within cone contrast space, this mechanism was more sensitive than both a luminance mechanism with little S cone input but considerable variation in relative L to M cone input, and a blue-yellow chromatic mechanism.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- G R Cole
- Optical Sciences Centre, Australian National University, Canberra
| | | | | |
Collapse
|