1
|
Beavan A, Hanke L, Spielmann J, Skorski S, Mayer J, Meyer T, Fransen J. The effect of stroboscopic vision on performance in a football specific assessment. SCI MED FOOTBALL 2022; 5:317-322. [PMID: 35077302 DOI: 10.1080/24733938.2020.1862420] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
Abstract
Purpose: This study aimed to investigate how restricted visual feedback affects performance in a football-specific skills assessment that incorporates the coupling of football a-specific perceptual information with football-specific motor actions. Methods: The Footbonaut is a 14x14m cage equipped with 8 ball dispensers and 64 targets measuring passing accuracy and time to complete each pass. Eighty-four amateur male participants (19.5 ± 5.4 years old; 13.1 ± 6.0 years experience) completed two sessions under two different visual conditions: stroboscopic and normal vision. Results: A linear regression revealed that performance under normal conditions was significantly associated (p < 0.001) with the performance decrement under stroboscopic vision conditions. Players were then subdivided into skilled (S; top 25%) and less-skilled (LS; bottom 25%) groups. Restricting visual feedback impacted the average time required to complete the passes in both S and LS groups equally (S: +0.18 s; LS: + 0.12 s; p = 0.385), yet S athletes' accuracy (-11.1%) was more heavily reduced under restricted visual conditions compared to their Normal condition; whereas the LS athletes' accuracy remained relatively unchanged (-1.9%). Conclusion: Therefore, stroboscopic vision may be used to induce performance errors during practice to stimulate larger training effects, particularly in more skilled players.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Adam Beavan
- Institute for Sports and Preventive Medicine, Saarland University, Saarbrücken, Germany.,Think Tank, German Football Association (Deutscher Fußball-Bund; DFB), Frankfurt, Germany.,Sport and Exercise Discipline Group, Faculty of Health, University of Technology, Sydney, Australia
| | - Lars Hanke
- Sport Science, University of Applied Sciences, Saarbrücken, Germany
| | - Jan Spielmann
- Psychology, TSG ResearchLab gGmbH, Zuzenhausen, Germany
| | - Sabrina Skorski
- Institute for Sports and Preventive Medicine, Saarland University, Saarbrücken, Germany
| | - Jan Mayer
- Psychology, TSG ResearchLab gGmbH, Zuzenhausen, Germany.,Performance Diagnostics, TSG 1899 Hoffenheim, Zuzenhausen, Germany
| | - Tim Meyer
- Institute for Sports and Preventive Medicine, Saarland University, Saarbrücken, Germany
| | - Job Fransen
- Sport and Exercise Discipline Group, Faculty of Health, University of Technology, Sydney, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Hodges NJ, Lohse KR. An extended challenge-based framework for practice design in sports coaching. J Sports Sci 2022; 40:754-768. [PMID: 35019816 DOI: 10.1080/02640414.2021.2015917] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 10.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/06/2023]
Abstract
The challenge-point framework as a model for thinking about motor learning was first proposed in 2004. Although it has been well-cited, surprisingly this framework has not made its way into much of the applied sport science literature. One of the reasons for this omission is that the original framework had not been encapsulated into a paper accessible for sports practitioners. The framework had mostly a theoretical focus, providing a mechanistic summary of motor learning research. Our aims in this paper were to explain and elaborate on the challenge point framework to present an applied framework guiding practice design. We connect the framework to other theories that involve predictive coding, where information is attended when it disconfirms current predictions, providing a strong signal for learning. We also consider how two new dimensions (learners' motivation and practice specificity) need to be considered when designing practice settings. By moving around the different dimensions of functional difficulty, motivation, and specificity, coaches can optimize practice to achieve different learning goals. Specifically, we present three general "types" of practice: practice to learn, to transfer to competition, and to maintain current skills. Practical examples are given to illustrate how this framework can inform coach practice.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nicola J Hodges
- University of British Columbia, School of Kinesiology, Vancouver, Canada
| | - Keith R Lohse
- School of Medicine, Washington University, St. Louis, United States
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Khan MA, Franks IM, Goodman D. The Effect of Practice on the Control of Rapid Aiming Movements: Evidence for an Interdependency between Programming and Feedback Processing. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2018. [DOI: 10.1080/713755756] [Citation(s) in RCA: 40] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/17/2022]
Abstract
The purpose of this experiment was to investigate how the control of aiming movements performed as fast and as accurately as possible changes with practice. We examined: (1) the influence of visual feedback on the initial impulse and error correction phases of aiming movements during acquisition; and (2) the effect of removing visual feedback at different levels of practice. Results from the acquisition trials indicated that vision had a major impact on the organization of the initial impulse and error correction phases. Also, consistent with findings from research involving temporally constrained movements, the cost of removing vision was greater after extensive levels than after moderate levels of practice. Collectively, these results denote the importance of visual feedback to the learning of this particular class of aiming movements. Learning appears to be a dual process of improved programming of the initial impulse and increased efficiency of feedback processing. Practice not only acts on programming and feedback processes directly, but also indirectly through a reciprocal interplay between these two processes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michael A. Khan
- School of Human Kinetics, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada
| | - Ian M. Franks
- School of Human Kinetics, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada
| | - David Goodman
- School of Kinesiology, Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, British Columbia, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Cassell VE, Beattie SJ, Lawrence GP. Changing performance pressure between training and competition influences action planning because of a reduction in the efficiency of action execution. ANXIETY STRESS AND COPING 2017; 31:107-120. [PMID: 28945116 DOI: 10.1080/10615806.2017.1373389] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
Abstract
Background and objectives Specificity of practice proposes optimal performance is linked to the conditions under which learning occurred. The present study investigated this effect within a pressure context to determine whether offline and/or online control processes develop specificity through the introduction or removal of performance pressure. Methods Forty novices practiced a two-dimensional stimulus-response discrimination task in one of four groups; two control (control-control and anxiety-anxiety) and two experimental (control-anxiety and anxiety-control). In the experimental groups, participants experienced a switch in conditions of pressure both early and late in practice, i.e., practiced in low-pressure and transferred to high-pressure (control-anxiety group) or the reverse of this (anxiety-control group). Results A significant acquisition-to-transfer decrement in performance occurred for both experimental groups. This offers support for a pressure-performance specificity effect because a change in conditions of pressure (regardless if that was an increase or decrease) resulted in performance decrements. Furthermore, the reaction time measure of offline control was affected by the change to a significantly greater extent than the movement time measure of online control. Conclusions Increases in offline control processes was a performance strategy adopted to combat the disruption that pressure caused to the processes associated with adjusting or planning movements online.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Victoria E Cassell
- a School of Sport, Health and Exercise Sciences, Institute for the Psychology of Elite Performance , Bangor University , Bangor , Gwynedd
| | - Stuart J Beattie
- a School of Sport, Health and Exercise Sciences, Institute for the Psychology of Elite Performance , Bangor University , Bangor , Gwynedd
| | - Gavin P Lawrence
- a School of Sport, Health and Exercise Sciences, Institute for the Psychology of Elite Performance , Bangor University , Bangor , Gwynedd
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Practice with anxiety improves performance, but only when anxious: evidence for the specificity of practice hypothesis. PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2013; 78:634-50. [PMID: 24162389 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-013-0521-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/13/2012] [Accepted: 10/12/2013] [Indexed: 12/31/2022]
Abstract
We investigated for the first time whether the principles of specificity could be extended to the psychological construct of anxiety and whether any benefits of practicing with anxiety are dependent on the amount of exposure and timing of that exposure in relation to where in learning the exposure occurs. In Experiment 1, novices practiced a discrete golf-putting task in one of four groups: all practice trials under anxiety (anxiety), non-anxiety (control), or a combination of these two (i.e., the first half of practice under anxiety before changing to non-anxiety conditions, anxiety-control, or the reverse of this, control-anxiety). Following acquisition, all groups were transferred to an anxiety condition. Results revealed a significant acquisition-to-transfer decrement in performance between acquisition and transfer for the control group only. In Experiment 2, novices practiced a complex rock climbing task in one of the four groups detailed above, before being transferred to both a high-anxiety condition and a low-anxiety condition (the ordering of these was counterbalanced across participants). Performance in anxiety transfer was greater following practice with anxiety compared to practice without anxiety. However, these benefits were influenced by the timing of anxiety exposure since performance was greatest when exposure to anxiety occurred in the latter half of acquisition. In the low-anxiety transfer test, performance was lowest for those who had practiced with anxiety only, thus providing support for the specificity of practice hypothesis. Results demonstrate that the specificity of learning principle can be extended to include the psychological construct of anxiety. Furthermore, the specificity advantage appears dependent on its timing in the learning process.
Collapse
|
6
|
Proteau L, Tremblay L, Dejaeger D. Practice Does Not Diminish the Role of Visual Information in On-Line Control of a Precision Walking Task: Support for the Specificity of Practice Hypothesis. J Mot Behav 2010; 30:143-50. [DOI: 10.1080/00222899809601331] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
|
7
|
Krigolson OE, Tremblay L. The amount of practice really matters: specificity of practice may be valid only after sufficient practice. RESEARCH QUARTERLY FOR EXERCISE AND SPORT 2009; 80:197-204. [PMID: 19650384 DOI: 10.1080/02701367.2009.10599553] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/28/2023]
Abstract
Studies investigating the specificity hypothesis have not always demonstrated that reliance on a specific source offeedback increases with practice. The goal of the present study was to address this inconsistency by having participants practice a throwing task with or without vision at incremental levels (10, 50, 100, or 200 acquisition trials). Following acquisition, all participants in the present experiment performed 10 trials in a no-vision transfer condition. Our results demonstrated that, given a sufficient number of acquisition trials, feedback reliance increased as a function of time engaged in practice. Our results also suggest that increased reliance on a specific source of feedback occurs only after the control strategy for a task is optimized.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Olav E Krigolson
- Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, Canada.
| | | |
Collapse
|
8
|
Lawrence GP, Khan MA, Buckolz E, Oldham ARH. The contribution of peripheral and central vision in the control of movement amplitude. Hum Mov Sci 2006; 25:326-38. [PMID: 16616964 DOI: 10.1016/j.humov.2006.02.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/05/2005] [Revised: 02/02/2006] [Accepted: 02/03/2006] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
Abstract
Past research has revealed that central vision is more important than peripheral vision in controlling the amplitude of target-directed aiming movements. However, the extent to which central vision contributes to movement planning versus online control is unclear. Since participants usually fixate the target very early in the limb trajectory, the limb enters the central visual field during the late stages of movement. Hence, there may be insufficient time for central vision to be processed online to correct errors during movement execution. Instead, information from central vision may be processed offline and utilised as a form of knowledge of results, enhancing the programming of subsequent trials. In the present research, variability in limb trajectories was analysed to determine the extent to which peripheral and central vision is used to detect and correct errors during movement execution. Participants performed manual aiming movements of 450 ms under four different visual conditions: full vision, peripheral vision, central vision, no vision. The results revealed that participants utilised visual information from both the central and peripheral visual fields to adjust limb trajectories during movement execution. However, visual information from the central visual field was used more effectively to correct errors online compared to visual information from the peripheral visual field.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gavin P Lawrence
- Centre for Sport and Exercise Sciences, University of Leeds, Leeds LS2 9JT, United Kingdom.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
9
|
Khan MA, Franks IM. Online versus offline processing of visual feedback in the production of component submovements. J Mot Behav 2003; 35:285-95. [PMID: 12873843 DOI: 10.1080/00222890309602141] [Citation(s) in RCA: 43] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
Abstract
The present authors tested the assumptions in R. S. Woodworth's (1899) 2-component model regarding the specific roles of vision in the production of both the initial impulse and the error-correction phases of movement. Participants (N = 40) practiced a rapid aiming task (1,500 trials), with either no visual feedback, vision of only the 1st 50% of the movement, vision of only the 1st 75% of the movement, or vision of the entire movement. Consistent with previous research, the availability of vision over the 1st half of the movement had no effect on aiming accuracy during acquisition. In contrast, when visual feedback was available over the 1st 75% of the movement and the entire movement, initial impulse endpoints were less variable and the efficiency of the error-correction phase was improved. Analysis of spatial variability at various stages in the movement revealed that participants processed visual feedback offline to improve programming of the initial impulse and processed it online in regulating the deceleration of the initial impulse.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michael A Khan
- School of Sport, Health and Exercise Sciences, University of Wales, Bangor, George Building, Holyhead Road, Bangor, Gwynedd, Wales LL57 2PX, UK.
| | | |
Collapse
|
10
|
Khan MA, Lawrence G, Fourkas A, Franks IM, Elliott D, Pembroke S. Online versus offline processing of visual feedback in the control of movement amplitude. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2003; 113:83-97. [PMID: 12679045 DOI: 10.1016/s0001-6918(02)00156-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 71] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/23/2022] Open
Abstract
Researchers have suggested that visual feedback not only plays a role in the correction of errors during movement execution but that visual feedback from a completed movement is processed offline to improve programming on upcoming trials. In the present study, we examined the potential contribution of online and offline processing of visual feedback by analysing spatial variability at various kinematic landmarks in the limb trajectory (peak acceleration, peak velocity, peak negative acceleration and movement end). Participants performed a single degree of freedom video aiming task with and without vision of the cursor under four criterion movement times (225, 300, 375 and 450 ms). For movement times of 225 and 300 ms, the full vision condition was less variable than the no vision condition. However, the form of the variability profiles did not differ between visual conditions suggesting that the contribution of visual feedback was due to offline processes. In the 375 and 450 ms conditions, there was evidence for both online and offline control as the form of the variability profiles differed significantly between visual conditions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michael A Khan
- School of Sport, Health and Exercise Sciences, University of Wales-Bangor, George Building, Gwynedd, Wales, UK LL57 2PX.
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
11
|
Coull J, Weir PL, Tremblay L, Weeks DJ, Elliott D. Monocular and binocular vision in the control of goal-directed movement. J Mot Behav 2000; 32:347-60. [PMID: 11114228 DOI: 10.1080/00222890009601385] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
Abstract
In the present research the authors examined the time course of binocular integration in goal-directed aiming and grasping. With liquid-crystal goggles, the authors manipulated vision independently to the right and left eyes of 10 students during movement preparation and movement execution. Contrary to earlier findings reported in catching experiments (I. Olivier, D. J. Weeks, K. L. Ricker, J. Lyons, & D. Elliott, 1998), neither a temporal nor a spatial binocular advantage was obtained in 1 grasping and 2 aiming studies. That result suggests that, at least in some circumstances, monocular vision is sufficient for the precise control of limb movements. In a final aiming experiment involving 3-dimensional spatial variability and no trial-to-trial visual feedback about performance, binocular vision was associated with greater spatial accuracy. Binocular superiority appeared to be most pronounced when participants were unable to adjust their limb control strategy or procedure on the basis of terminal feedback about performance.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- J Coull
- Department of Kinesiology, McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario L8S 4K1, Canada.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
12
|
Khan MA, Franks IM. The effect of practice on component submovements is dependent on the availability of visual feedback. J Mot Behav 2000; 32:227-40. [PMID: 10975271 DOI: 10.1080/00222890009601374] [Citation(s) in RCA: 47] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
Abstract
Participants (N = 16) were given extensive practice (1,500 trials) on a perceptual-motor aiming task. The full-vision (FV) group practiced with vision of their response cursor, whereas the no-vision (NV) group practiced in a condition without vision. Movements were made as quickly and accurately as possible, and knowledge of results (KR) was provided. The authors tested the importance of vision early and late in practice by transferring participants to the NV condition without KR. The effects of practice differed between the two conditions. The FV group increased the speed of initial impulse to get to the target quickly, then relied on vision to make discrete error corrections. Transfer tests revealed that reliance on vision remained after extensive practice. For the NV group, practice effects were associated with a reduction in the extent to which discrete error corrections were produced.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- M A Khan
- School of Sport, Health and Physical Education Sciences, University of Wales, Bangor, Gwynedd, UK
| | | |
Collapse
|
13
|
Tremblay L, Proteau L. Specificity of practice: the case of powerlifting. RESEARCH QUARTERLY FOR EXERCISE AND SPORT 1998; 69:284-289. [PMID: 9777665 DOI: 10.1080/02701367.1998.10607695] [Citation(s) in RCA: 50] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/22/2023]
Affiliation(s)
- L Tremblay
- Département de kinésiologie, Université de Montréal.
| | | |
Collapse
|
14
|
Vindras P, Desmurget M, Prablanc C, Viviani P. Pointing errors reflect biases in the perception of the initial hand position. J Neurophysiol 1998; 79:3290-4. [PMID: 9636129 DOI: 10.1152/jn.1998.79.6.3290] [Citation(s) in RCA: 99] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/07/2023] Open
Abstract
By comparing the visuomotor performance of 10 adult, normal subjects in three tasks, we investigated whether errors in pointing movements reflect biased estimations of the hand starting position. In a manual pointing task with no visual feedback, subjects aimed at 48 targets spaced regularly around two starting positions. Nine subjects exhibited a similar pattern of systematic errors across targets, i.e., a parallel shift of the end points that accounted, on average, for 49% of the total variability. The direction of the shift depended on the starting location. Systematic errors decreased dramatically in the second condition where subjects were allowed to see their hand before movement onset. The third task was to use a joystick held by the left hand to estimate the location of their (unseen) right hand. The systematic perceptual errors in this condition were found to be highly correlated with the motor errors in the first condition. The results support the following conclusions. 1) Kinesthetic estimation of hand position may be consistently biased. Some of the mechanisms responsible for these biases are always active, irrespective of whether position is estimated overtly (e.g., with a matching paradigm), or covertly as part of the motor planning for aimed movements. 2) Pointing errors reflect to a significant extent the erroneous estimation of initial hand position. This suggests that aimed hand movements are planned vectorially, i.e., in terms of distance and direction, rather than in terms of absolute position in space.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- P Vindras
- Faculté de Psychologie et des Sciences de l'Education, Université de Genève, 1227 Carouge, Switzerland
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
15
|
Bennett S, Davids K. Manipulating peripheral visual information in manual aiming: Exploring the notion of specificity of learning. Hum Mov Sci 1998. [DOI: 10.1016/s0167-9457(97)00033-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
|
16
|
The effect of task constraints on the manipulation of visual information and the implications for the specificity of learning hypothesis. Hum Mov Sci 1997. [DOI: 10.1016/s0167-9457(97)00002-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
|
17
|
Robertson S, Elliott D. Specificity of learning and dynamic balance. RESEARCH QUARTERLY FOR EXERCISE AND SPORT 1996; 67:69-75. [PMID: 8735996 DOI: 10.1080/02701367.1996.10607927] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/22/2023]
Abstract
Contrary to a strict specificity of learning position, Robertson, Collins, Elliott, and Starkes (1994) have reported that the balance beam performance of expert gymnasts is less affected by the withdrawal of vision than is the performance of novice gymnasts. In this study, we employed a training paradigm in order to exercise complete control over the sensory conditions under which a dynamic balance beam task was acquired. Novice participants were trained either with or without vision to walk across a balance beam as quickly as possible and later tested in the other vision condition. Although participants improved more in the condition in which they trained, practice in one sensory condition did not negatively affect performance in a different sensory circumstance. The finding that vision was still extremely important after 5 days of practice is problematic for models of motor learning that propose a progression with learning from closed-loop to open-loop control.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- S Robertson
- Department of Kinesiology, Purdue University, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
18
|
Chaput S, Proteau L. Modifications with aging in the role played by vision and proprioception for movement control. Exp Aging Res 1996; 22:1-21. [PMID: 8665984 DOI: 10.1080/03610739608253994] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/01/2023]
Abstract
Young and older adults performed manual aiming movements to a visible target for either 40 or 200 trials. Under each level of practice, half of the subjects practiced the task under normal visual conditions (proprioception + vision [PV] condition), whereas the other half were not permitted to see their ongoing movement toward the target (proprioception-only [P] condition). Each acquisition trial was followed with knowledge of results (KR). After the last acquisition trial, all subjects were transferred to a common task in which only the target to be reached was visually available, with no KR. During acquisition, the younger subjects were found to be spatially more accurate than their older counterparts, and this was so regardless of the number of acquisition trials. Withdrawing KR during the transfer test did not modify the spatial accuracy of the subjects who had trained under the P condition. This indicates that the subjects had a reliable reference of the movement to be realized. Withdrawing vision of the moving hand and KR in the transfer test caused a significant increase in spatial error for both the older and the younger subjects. However, the increase in error was less pronounced for the older than for the younger subjects. In fact, the older subjects performed as well in the transfer test as the subjects who had trained in the P condition. This pattern of results suggests that in the transfer test, the older subjects could still guide their movements with the proprioceptive information that was available during both acquisition and transfer. However, such was not the case for the younger subjects. This suggests that, unlike the younger subjects, the older subjects could still rely on the proprioceptive cues available during acquisition in the PV condition. These results are taken to indicate that practicing with numerous sources of afferent information, as was the case in the PV condition, resulted in an integrated reference store for the younger subjects. In contrast, while practicing the task in the PV condition, the older subjects appeared to process independently from each other the different sources of sensory information available.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- S Chaput
- Département d'Education Physique, Université de Montréal, Canada
| | | |
Collapse
|
19
|
Desmurget M, Rossetti Y, Prablanc C, Stelmach GE, Jeannerod M. Representation of hand position prior to movement and motor variability. Can J Physiol Pharmacol 1995; 73:262-72. [PMID: 7621365 DOI: 10.1139/y95-037] [Citation(s) in RCA: 73] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/26/2023]
Abstract
Pointing accuracy of six human subjects was measured in two blocked conditions where the hand was either never visible (T: target only) or only visible in static position prior to movement onset (H+T: hand+target). It was shown in condition H+T that, viewing the hand prior to movement greatly decreased end-point variability compared with condition T. This effect was associated with a significant modification of the movement kinematics: the H+T condition induced a shortened acceleration phase with a corresponding lengthened deceleration phase, compared with the T condition. These results led us to the hypothesis that viewing the hand prior to movement onset allowed a decrease of pointing variability through a feedback process. This hypothesis was further tested by turning the target off during the deceleration phase of the movement at half peak velocity. It was shown that turning the target off had no effect upon the T condition but induced a significant increase of pointing variability in the H+T condition. This result suggests that vision of the static hand enhances the proprioceptive localization of the limb and allows for a better visual to kinesthesic feedback.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- M Desmurget
- Vision et motricité, Institut national de la santé et de la recherche médicale, Bron, France
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
20
|
Elliott D, Chua R, Pollock BJ, Lyons J. Optimizing the use of vision in manual aiming: the role of practice. THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY. A, HUMAN EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 1995; 48:72-83. [PMID: 7754087 DOI: 10.1080/14640749508401376] [Citation(s) in RCA: 68] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/27/2023]
Abstract
The purpose of this study was to determine how subjects learn to adjust the characteristics of their manual aiming movements in order to make optimal use of the visual information and reduce movement error. Subjects practised aiming (120 trials) with visual information available for either 400 msec or 600 msec. Following acquisition, they were transferred to conditions in which visual information was available for either more or less time. Over acquisition, subjects appeared to reduce target-aiming error by moving to the target area more quickly in order to make greater use of vision when in the vicinity of the target. With practice, there was also a reduction in the number of modifications in the movement. After transfer, both performance and kinematic data indicated that the time for which visual information was available was a more important predictor of aiming error than the similarity between training and transfer conditions. These findings are not consistent with a strong "specificity of learning" position. They also suggest that, if some sort of general representation or motor programme develops with practice, that representation includes rules or procedures for the utilization of visual feedback to allow for the on-line adjustment of the goal-directed movement.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- D Elliott
- Department of Kinesiology, McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario, Canada
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|