1
|
Paape D, Avetisyan S, Lago S, Vasishth S. Modeling Misretrieval and Feature Substitution in Agreement Attraction: A Computational Evaluation. Cogn Sci 2021; 45:e13019. [PMID: 34379348 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13019] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/01/2021] [Revised: 05/30/2021] [Accepted: 06/25/2021] [Indexed: 12/12/2022]
Abstract
We present computational modeling results based on a self-paced reading study investigating number attraction effects in Eastern Armenian. We implement three novel computational models of agreement attraction in a Bayesian framework and compare their predictive fit to the data using k-fold cross-validation. We find that our data are better accounted for by an encoding-based model of agreement attraction, compared to a retrieval-based model. A novel methodological contribution of our study is the use of comprehension questions with open-ended responses, so that both misinterpretation of the number feature of the subject phrase and misassignment of the thematic subject role of the verb can be investigated at the same time. We find evidence for both types of misinterpretation in our study, sometimes in the same trial. However, the specific error patterns in our data are not fully consistent with any previously proposed model.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dario Paape
- Department of Linguistics, University of Potsdam
| | | | - Sol Lago
- Institute for Romance Languages and Literatures, Goethe University Frankfurt
| | | |
Collapse
|
2
|
Romero-Ayuso D, Castillero-Perea Á, González P, Navarro E, Molina-Massó JP, Funes MJ, Ariza-Vega P, Toledano-González A, Triviño-Juárez JM. Assessment of cognitive instrumental activities of daily living: a systematic review. Disabil Rehabil 2019; 43:1342-1358. [PMID: 31549907 DOI: 10.1080/09638288.2019.1665720] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/04/2023]
Abstract
PURPOSE Cognitive instrumental activities of daily living are particularly related to executive functions, such as scheduling appointments, monthly payments, managing the household economy, shopping or taking the bus. The aim of this systematic review was to determine the available tests for the assessment of executive functions with ecological validity to predict individuals' functioning. MATERIALS AND METHODS An electronic search was conducted in MEDLINE, Cochrane Central, PsyCInfo and IEEE Xplore until May 2019, in addition to a manual search. The PRISMA criteria and the Covidence platform were used to select articles and extract data. RESULTS After applying the search selection criteria, 76 studies were identified. They referred to 110 tools to assess instrumental activities of daily living. Those that have received most attention are related to menu preparation and shopping. Performance-based measures are the most widely used traditional methods. Most tests were aimed at the adult population with acquired brain damage, cognitive impairment or dementia. There was a predominance of tests based on the Multiple Errands Test paradigm. CONCLUSIONS In recent years, it has increased the number of tools that assess the instrumental activities of daily living based on technologies such as personal or environmental sensors and serious games.IMPLICATIONS FOR REHABILITATIONAssessment of Instrumental Activities of Daily Living through performance-based measures is especially useful for the early detection of dysfunctions or preclinical disability.Difficulties in performing instrumental activities of daily living are closely associated with deficits in executive functions and prospective memory.Activities of Daily Living can be understood as multitasks.The use of virtual reality-based tests was shown to be sensitive to the detection of cognitive deficits in Activities of Daily Living.An advantage of using virtual reality in assessments is that it can help to predict the level of personal autonomy in patients who are in an institutional environment and could be a first approximation to the real environment.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dulce Romero-Ayuso
- Division of Occupational Therapy, Department of Physical Therapy, Faculty of Health Sciences, University of Granada, Granada, Spain
| | | | - Pascual González
- LoUISE Research Group, Computing Systems Department, University of Castilla-La Mancha, Albacete, Spain
| | - Elena Navarro
- LoUISE Research Group, Computing Systems Department, University of Castilla-La Mancha, Albacete, Spain
| | - José Pascual Molina-Massó
- LoUISE Research Group, Computing Systems Department, University of Castilla-La Mancha, Albacete, Spain
| | - M Jesús Funes
- Mind, Brain and Behaviour Research Center (CIMCYC) and Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Granada, Granada, Spain
| | - Patrocinio Ariza-Vega
- Division of Occupational Therapy, Department of Physical Therapy, Faculty of Health Sciences, University of Granada, Granada, Spain.,Rehabilitation and Traumatology Service, Virgen de Las Nieves University Hospital of Granada, Granada, Spain
| | - Abel Toledano-González
- Department of Psychology, Faculty of Occupational Therapy, Speech Therapy and Nursing, University of Castilla-La Mancha, Ciudad Real, Spain
| | | |
Collapse
|
3
|
Mills L, Kinoshita S, Norris D. No Negative Priming Effect in the Manual Stroop Task. Front Psychol 2019; 10:1764. [PMID: 31428019 PMCID: PMC6688563 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01764] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/15/2019] [Accepted: 07/15/2019] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
The negative priming effect is an increase in interference when the response to the target on the current trial corresponds to the response to the distractor word on a preceding trial. Contrary to the commonly held belief that the negative priming effect is ubiquitous in the Stroop task, in the original study by Neill (1977), negative priming was found only in the oral, and not the manual Stroop task. The present paper makes three empirical observations. First, we replicate the discrepancy in the finding of the negative priming effect in the oral versus manual Stroop tasks tested under identical conditions, where response mode could be the only the causal factor. Second, we point out that previous manual Stroop experiments reporting the negative priming effect confounded the effect of response repetition. Third, we report the analysis of the negative priming effect at the level of whole RT distribution, which revealed that the effect was absent throughout the RT distribution in the manual task, and it was of constant size across the RT distribution in the oral task. Implications of the results for conflict control in the Stroop task is discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Luke Mills
- Department of Cognitive Science, Macquarie University, Sydney, NSW, Australia
| | - Sachiko Kinoshita
- Department of Cognitive Science, Macquarie University, Sydney, NSW, Australia.,Department of Psychology, Macquarie University, Sydney, NSW, Australia
| | - Dennis Norris
- MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Ritter FE, Tehranchi F, Oury JD. ACT‐R: A cognitive architecture for modeling cognition. WILEY INTERDISCIPLINARY REVIEWS. COGNITIVE SCIENCE 2018; 10:e1488. [DOI: 10.1002/wcs.1488] [Citation(s) in RCA: 38] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/09/2017] [Revised: 10/05/2018] [Accepted: 10/20/2018] [Indexed: 11/09/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Frank E. Ritter
- College of Information Sciences and Technology Pennsylvania State University University Park Pennsylvania
| | - Farnaz Tehranchi
- Department of Computer Science and Engineering Pennsylvania State University University Park Pennsylvania
| | - Jacob D. Oury
- College of Information Sciences and Technology Pennsylvania State University University Park Pennsylvania
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Abstract
Despite the widespread use of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), few studies have addressed scanner effects on performance. The studies that have examined this question show a wide variety of results. In this article we report analyses of three experiments in which participants performed a perceptual decision-making task both in a traditional setting as well as inside an MRI scanner. The results consistently show that response times increase inside the scanner. Error rates also increase, but to a lesser extent. To reveal the underlying mechanisms that drive the behavioral changes when performing a task inside the MRI scanner, the data were analyzed using the linear ballistic accumulator model of decision-making. These analyses show that, in the scanner, participants exhibit a slow down of the motor component of the response and have less attentional focus on the task. However, the balance between focus and motor slowing depends on the specific task requirements.
Collapse
|
6
|
Abstract
In this commentary, I will argue that the componential and emergent views of cognitive control as defined by Cooper (2010) do not necessarily oppose each other, and I will try to make a case for their interdependence. First, I will use the construct of cognitive inhibition-one of the main componential control functions mentioned in the target articles-to illustrate my line of reasoning. Then, I will comment on how some of the target articles, each from a different perspective, bring arguments in favor of this integrative view.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ion Juvina
- Department of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Falquez R, Lang S, Dinu-Biringer R, Nees F, Arens E, Kotchoubey B, Berger M, Barnow S. On the relationship between negative affective priming and prefrontal cognitive control mechanisms. Cogn Emot 2015; 30:225-44. [DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2014.994476] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
|
8
|
Grange JA, Juvina I. The effect of practice on n-2 repetition costs in set switching. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2015; 154:14-25. [PMID: 25461439 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.11.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/21/2014] [Revised: 10/30/2014] [Accepted: 11/08/2014] [Indexed: 11/16/2022] Open
Abstract
Inhibition in set switching is inferred from so-called n-2 repetition costs: slower response times to ABA sequences compared to CBA sequences (where A, B, and C are arbitrary labels for different tasks). These costs are thought to reflect the persisting inhibition of task A when it was disengaged recently (as is the case in an ABA sequence). In this study we were interested in whether more inhibition may be required when the tasks are relatively novel. To this end, we examined the effect of practice on the n-2 repetition cost in nine participants across five experimental sessions, with 1222 trials performed in each session. The results show a clear reduction in the n-2 repetition cost, being altogether absent from the final sessions. Such a reduction is predicted by both: (a) a recent computational model of the n-2 repetition cost (Grange, Juvina, & Houghton, 2013) due to the gradual strengthening of task-related memory elements with practice to the point where inhibition has less impact; and (b) prior work showing smaller n-2 repetition costs with greater cue-target association strength (Houghton, Pritchard, & Grange, 2009). In this paper, we integrate these two theoretical derivations by extending our computational model, which fit the current data-at the mean level, block level, and individual-subject (i.e., individual differences) level-well.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Ion Juvina
- Department of Psychology, Wright State University, USA
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Response-repetition costs in choice-RT tasks: biased expectancies or response inhibition? Acta Psychol (Amst) 2014; 145:21-32. [PMID: 24269885 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.10.015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/05/2013] [Revised: 08/13/2013] [Accepted: 10/29/2013] [Indexed: 11/20/2022] Open
Abstract
Repetition effects are often viewed as informative regarding the cognitive mechanisms of action control. One particular finding, namely costs for repeating the same response in subsequent trials, especially challenges theorizing. Costs for response repetitions have recently been reported in task-switch studies on task-switch trials (whereas benefits usually arise in task-repetition trials), but also in some choice-RT task studies. In three experiments, two of the most successful accounts for the response-repetition costs in choice-RT task studies and task switching were tested: an expectancy-based explanation, and an inhibition-based account. Using a choice-RT task introduced by Smith (1968) and manipulating the response-stimulus interval (RSI) and the categorizability of the stimuli, some specific predictions of the two accounts were tested. The results clearly revealed that expectancy-based explanations fail to account for the observed patterns of effects, whereas they are well in line with the predictions from the inhibition-based account. Finally, the results are further discussed with respect to alternative accounts from the field of task switching.
Collapse
|
10
|
Grzyb KR, Hübner R. Strategic modulation of response inhibition in task-switching. Front Psychol 2013; 4:545. [PMID: 23986730 PMCID: PMC3749430 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00545] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/08/2013] [Accepted: 08/02/2013] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
Residual activations from previous task performance usually prime the system toward response repetition. However, when the task switches, the repetition of a response (RR) produces longer reaction times and higher error rates. Some researchers assumed that these RR costs reflect strategic inhibition of just executed responses and that this serves for preventing perseveration errors. We investigated whether the basic level of response inhibition is adapted to the overall risk of response perseveration. In a series of 3 experiments, we presented different proportions of stimuli that carry either a high or a low risk of perseveration. Additionally, the discriminability of high- and low-risk stimuli was varied. The results indicate that individuals apply several processing and control strategies, depending on the mixture of stimulus types. When discriminability was high, control was adapted on a trial-by trial basis, which presumably reduces mental effort (Experiment 1). When trial-based strategies were prevented, RR costs for low-risk stimuli varied with the overall proportion of high-risk stimuli (Experiments 2 and 3), indicating an adaptation of the basic level of response inhibition.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kai Robin Grzyb
- Department of Psychology, Universität Konstanz Konstanz, Germany
| | | |
Collapse
|
11
|
Grzyb KR, Hübner R. Response Inhibition Modulates Response Conflict in Task Switching. ZEITSCHRIFT FUR PSYCHOLOGIE-JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY 2013. [DOI: 10.1027/2151-2604/a000128] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
Abstract
Although response repetition (RR) effects vary considerably between conditions and studies, little is known about the causes. Recently, RR costs on task-switch trials have been found to be larger for incongruent stimuli that activate both alternative responses than for neutral ones. Here, we investigated if this modulation can be explained by an amplification of response conflict account (ARC). It assumes that a response-shift bias that is responsible for the basic RR costs amplifies the response conflict induced by incongruent stimuli specifically on trials where the response repeats. Consequently, RR costs are increased for incongruent stimuli. Because supporting evidence for this account was restricted to task-shift trials, we tested if the ARC account holds also more generally, that is, on task-repetition trials. To this end, we applied a rather common alternating-runs paradigm and presented neutral and incongruent stimuli. Results show that the congruency effect was larger on RR trials than on RS trials. Because this relation was independent of task transition, it is consistent with the idea that, in order to promote behavioral flexibility in task-switching contexts, a general response-shift bias is induced by inhibiting the previous response.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Ronald Hübner
- Department of Psychology, University of Konstanz, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Ihrke M, Behrendt J, Schrobsdorff H, Visser I, Hasselhorn M. Negative priming persists in the absence of response-retrieval. Exp Psychol 2012; 60:12-21. [PMID: 22851376 DOI: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000169] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
Abstract
The hypothesis that retrieval of the prime response is responsible for the negative priming (NP) effect has gained popularity in recent studies of visual identity NP. In the current study we report an experiment in which we aimed to remove the response from the prime memory trace by means of spatio-temporal separation. Compared to an identical experiment without this separation (Ihrke et al., 2011), we find that the response-retrieval-specific interaction is absent indicating that the separation was successful in preventing response-retrieval. Still, both negative and positive priming are present as main effects which show that processes other than response-retrieval can produce NP. In addition, based on recordings of the eye-movements during task processing, we localize the NP effect in a target-selection process while positive priming manifests in facilitated response-selection. Our results are in line with a multiple-route view of NP.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Matthias Ihrke
- Max-Planck-Institute for Dynamics and Self-Organization, Institute for Nonlinear Dynamics, University of Göttingen, Bernstein Center for Computational Neuroscience, Göttingen, Germany.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
13
|
On costs and benefits of n−2 repetitions in task switching: towards a behavioural marker of cognitive inhibition. PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2012; 77:211-22. [DOI: 10.1007/s00426-012-0421-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/19/2011] [Accepted: 01/25/2012] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
|
14
|
Abstract
This article presents a cognitive model that reconciles a surprising observation in the picture-word interference (PWI) paradigm with the general notion that PWI is a form of Stroop interference. Dell'Acqua, Job, Peressotti, and Pascali (2007) assessed PWI using a psychological refractory period (PRP) paradigm, and concluded that the locus of interference in PWI is during the perceptual encoding stage. Stroop interference, on the other hand, is generally attributed to response selection. Based on these findings it was argued that PWI is not a Stroop effect. The present article discusses an alternative interpretation of these results. We assume that both effects are caused by the same interference mechanism, but that the processing speed associated with the different stimuli (colors vs. words) accounts for the previously reported differences. We support this argument by presenting a single computational model that accounts for both PWI and Stroop phenomena in single task and PRP settings.
Collapse
|