1
|
Smithers SP, Brett MF, How MJ, Scott-Samuel NE, Roberts NW. Fiddler crabs (Afruca tangeri) detect second-order motion in both intensity and polarization. Commun Biol 2024; 7:1255. [PMID: 39362984 PMCID: PMC11450093 DOI: 10.1038/s42003-024-06953-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/10/2023] [Accepted: 09/24/2024] [Indexed: 10/05/2024] Open
Abstract
Motion vision is vital for a wide range of animal behaviors. Fiddler crabs, for example, rely heavily on motion to detect the movement of avian predators. They are known to detect first-order motion using both intensity (defined by spatiotemporal correlations in luminance) and polarization information (defined separately as spatiotemporal correlations in the degree and/or angle of polarization). However, little is known about their ability to detect second-order motion, another important form of motion information; defined separately by spatiotemporal correlations in higher-order image properties. In this work we used behavioral experiments to test how fiddler crabs (Afruca tangeri) responded to both second-order intensity and polarization stimuli. Fiddler crabs responded to a number of different intensity based second-order stimuli. Furthermore, the crabs also responded to second-order polarization stimuli, a behaviorally relevant stimulus applicable to an unpolarized flying bird when viewed against a polarized sky. The detection of second-order motion in polarization is, to the best of our knowledge, the first demonstration of this ability in any animal. This discovery therefore opens a new dimension in our understanding of how animals use polarization vision for target detection and the broader importance of second-order motion detection for animal behavior.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Samuel P Smithers
- School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Bristol Life Sciences Building, Bristol, UK.
- Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, Boston, MA, USA.
| | - Maisie F Brett
- School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Bristol Life Sciences Building, Bristol, UK
| | - Martin J How
- School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Bristol Life Sciences Building, Bristol, UK
| | | | - Nicholas W Roberts
- School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Bristol Life Sciences Building, Bristol, UK.
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Fangrui L, Jiaoli Z, Schunter C, Lin W, Yongzheng T, Zhiqiang H, Bin K. How Oratosquilla oratoria compound eye response to the polarization of light: In the perspective of vision genes and related proteins. Int J Biol Macromol 2024; 259:129053. [PMID: 38161015 DOI: 10.1016/j.ijbiomac.2023.129053] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/28/2023] [Revised: 12/23/2023] [Accepted: 12/24/2023] [Indexed: 01/03/2024]
Abstract
The special rhabdom structure of the mid-band ommatidium in compound eye contributes to the mantis shrimp being the only animal species known to science that can recognize circularly polarized light (CPL). Although the number of mid-band ommatidium of Oratosquilla oratoria is reduced, the mid-band ommatidium still has orthogonal geometric interleaved rhabdom and short oval distal rhabdom, which may mean that the O. oratoria has weakened circular polarized light vision (CPLV). Here we explored the molecular mechanisms of how O. oratoria response to the polarization of light. Based on the specific expression patterns of vision-related functional genes and proteins, we suggest that the order of light response by O. oratoria compound eye was first natural light, then left-circularly polarized light (LCPL), linearly polarized light, right-circularly polarized light (RCPL) and dark. Meanwhile, we found that the expression levels of vision-related functional genes and proteins in O. oratoria compound eye under RCPL were not significantly different from those in DL, which may imply that O. oratoria cannot respond to RCPL. Furthermore, the response of LCPL is likely facilitated by the differential expression of opsin and microvilli - related functional genes and proteins (arrestin and sodium-coupled neutral amino acid transporter). In conclusion, this study systematically illustrated for the first time how O. oratoria compound eye response to the polarization of light at the genetic level, and it can improve the visual ecological theory behind polarized light vision evolution.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lou Fangrui
- School of Ocean, Yantai University, Yantai, Shandong 264005, China
| | - Zhou Jiaoli
- School of Ocean, Yantai University, Yantai, Shandong 264005, China
| | - Celia Schunter
- Swire Institute of Marine Science, School of Biological Sciences, The University of Hong Kong Hong Kong SAR, China
| | - Wang Lin
- Key Laboratory of Sustainable Development of Marine Fisheries, Ministry of Agriculture and Rural Affairs, Yellow Sea Fisheries Research Institute, Chinese Academy of Fishery Sciences, Qingdao, Shandong 266071, China
| | - Tang Yongzheng
- School of Ocean, Yantai University, Yantai, Shandong 264005, China
| | - Han Zhiqiang
- Fishery College, Zhejiang Ocean University, Zhoushan, Zhejiang 316022, China.
| | - Kang Bin
- Fisheries College, Ocean University of China, Qingdao, Shandong 266003, China.
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
How MJ, Robertson A, Smithers SP, Wilby D. Polarization vision in terrestrial hermit crabs. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 2023; 209:899-905. [PMID: 37043013 PMCID: PMC10643299 DOI: 10.1007/s00359-023-01631-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/03/2022] [Revised: 03/07/2023] [Accepted: 04/03/2023] [Indexed: 04/13/2023]
Abstract
Polarization vision is used by a wide range of animals for navigating, orienting, and detecting objects or areas of interest. Shallow marine and semi-terrestrial crustaceans are particularly well known for their abilities to detect predator-like or conspecific-like objects based on their polarization properties. On land, some terrestrial invertebrates use polarization vision for detecting suitable habitats, oviposition sites or conspecifics, but examples of threat detection in the polarization domain are less well known. To test whether this also applies to crustaceans that have evolved to occupy terrestrial habitats, we determined the sensitivity of two species of land and one species of marine hermit crab to predator-like visual stimuli varying in the degree of polarization. All three species showed an ability to detect these cues based on polarization contrasts alone. One terrestrial species, Coenobita rugosus, showed an increased sensitivity to objects with a higher degree of polarization than the background. This is the inverse of most animals studied to date, suggesting that the ecological drivers for polarization vision may be different in the terrestrial environment.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Martin J How
- School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK.
| | | | - Samuel P Smithers
- School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK
- Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, Boston, MA, USA
| | - David Wilby
- School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK
- Research Software Engineering Team, Department of Computer Science, University of Sheffield, Sheffield, UK
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Zeil J. Views from 'crabworld': the spatial distribution of light in a tropical mudflat. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 2023; 209:859-876. [PMID: 37460846 PMCID: PMC10643439 DOI: 10.1007/s00359-023-01653-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/23/2023] [Revised: 06/18/2023] [Accepted: 06/29/2023] [Indexed: 11/14/2023]
Abstract
Natural scene analysis has been extensively used to understand how the invariant structure of the visual environment may have shaped biological image processing strategies. This paper deals with four crucial, but hitherto largely neglected aspects of natural scenes: (1) the viewpoint of specific animals; (2) the fact that image statistics are not independent of the position within the visual field; (3) the influence of the direction of illumination on luminance, spectral and polarization contrast in a scene; and (4) the biologically relevant information content of natural scenes. To address these issues, I recorded the spatial distribution of light in a tropical mudflat with a spectrographic imager equipped with a polarizing filter in an attempt to describe quantitatively the visual environment of fiddler crabs. The environment viewed by the crabs has a distinct structure. Depending on the position of the sun, the luminance, the spectral composition, and the polarization characteristics of horizontal light distribution are not uniform. This is true for both skylight and for reflections from the mudflat surface. The high-contrast feature of the line of horizon dominates the vertical distribution of light and is a discontinuity in terms of luminance, spectral distribution and of image statistics. On a clear day, skylight intensity increases towards the horizon due to multiple scattering, and its spectral composition increasingly resembles that of sunlight. Sky-substratum contrast is highest at short wavelengths. I discuss the consequences of this extreme example of the topography of vision for extracting biologically relevant information from natural scenes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jochen Zeil
- Research School of Biology, Australian National University, P.O. Box 475, Canberra, ACT, 2601, Australia.
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Lima RLS, Araújo PT, Barbosa Neto NM. Troubleshooting spectral artifacts from biplate retarders for reliable Stokes spectropolarimetry. THE REVIEW OF SCIENTIFIC INSTRUMENTS 2023; 94:103102. [PMID: 37796094 DOI: 10.1063/5.0160132] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/30/2023] [Accepted: 09/17/2023] [Indexed: 10/06/2023]
Abstract
Polarimetry is generally used to determine the polarization state of light beams in various research fields, such as biomedicine, astronomy, and materials science. In particular, the rotating quarter-wave plate polarimeter is an inexpensive and versatile option used in several single-wavelength applications to determine the four Stokes parameters. Extending this technique to broadband spectroscopic measurements is of great scientific interest since the information on light polarization is highly sensitive to anisotropic phenomena. However, the need for achromatic polarizing elements, especially quarter-wave plates, requires special attention in their modeling. In this study, we implemented a rotating retarder spectropolarimeter for broadband measurements using a commercially available quasi-achromatic biplate retarder over the visible range. Here, we present a comprehensive approach for troubleshooting this type of spectropolarimeter through the observation of artifacts stemming from the standard single-plate retarder model. Then, we derive a more suitable model for a quasi-achromatic retarder consisting of a biplate junction. This new biplate model requires knowledge of the intrinsic dispersive properties of the biplate, namely the equivalent retardance, fast axis tilt, and rotatory angle. Hence, in this study, we also show a self-consistent methodology to determine these biplate properties using the same polarimeter apparatus so that accurate Stokes parameters can be determined independently. Finally, the comparison of data generated with the standard single-plate and new biplate models shows a significant improvement in the measurement precision of the investigated polarization states, which confirms that remodeling the retarder for reliable spectropolarimetry is necessary.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ruan L S Lima
- Institute of Natural Sciences, Federal University of Pará, Belém, PA, Brazil
| | - Paulo T Araújo
- Departament of Physics and Astronomy, University of Alabama, Box 870324, Tuscaloosa, Alabama 35487, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
6
|
Palecanda S, Iwanicki T, Steck M, Porter ML. Crustacean conundrums: a review of opsin diversity and evolution. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2022; 377:20210289. [PMID: 36058240 PMCID: PMC9441232 DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2021.0289] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/02/2021] [Accepted: 02/06/2022] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Knowledge of crustacean vision is lacking compared to the more well-studied vertebrates and insects. While crustacean visual systems are typically conserved morphologically, the molecular components (i.e. opsins) remain understudied. This review aims to characterize opsin diversity across crustacean lineages for an integrated view of visual system evolution. Using publicly available data from 95 species, we identified opsin sequences and classified them by clade. Our analysis produced 485 putative visual opsins and 141 non-visual opsins. The visual opsins were separated into six clades: long wavelength sensitive (LWS), middle wavelength sensitive (MWS) 1 and 2, short wavelength or ultraviolet sensitive (SWS/UVS) and a clade of thecostracan opsins, with multiple LWS and MWS opsin copies observed. The SWS/UVS opsins were relatively conserved in most species. The crustacean classes Cephalocarida, Remipedia and Hexanauplia exhibited reduced visual opsin diversity compared to others, with the malacostracan decapods having the highest opsin diversity. Non-visual opsins were identified from all investigated classes except Cephalocarida. Additionally, a novel clade of non-visual crustacean-specific, R-type opsins (Rc) was discovered. This review aims to provide a framework for future research on crustacean vision, with an emphasis on the need for more work in spectral characterization and molecular analysis. This article is part of the theme issue 'Understanding colour vision: molecular, physiological, neuronal and behavioural studies in arthropods'.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sitara Palecanda
- School of Life Sciences, University of Hawai'i at Mānoa, Honolulu, HI 96822, USA
| | - Thomas Iwanicki
- School of Life Sciences, University of Hawai'i at Mānoa, Honolulu, HI 96822, USA
| | - Mireille Steck
- School of Life Sciences, University of Hawai'i at Mānoa, Honolulu, HI 96822, USA
| | - Megan L. Porter
- School of Life Sciences, University of Hawai'i at Mānoa, Honolulu, HI 96822, USA
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Venables SV, Drerup C, Powell SB, Marshall NJ, Herbert-Read JE, How MJ. Polarization vision mitigates visual noise from flickering light underwater. SCIENCE ADVANCES 2022; 8:eabq2770. [PMID: 36083913 PMCID: PMC9462692 DOI: 10.1126/sciadv.abq2770] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/29/2022] [Accepted: 07/26/2022] [Indexed: 06/15/2023]
Abstract
In shallow water, downwelling light is refracted from surface waves onto the substrate creating bands of light that fluctuate in both time and space, known as caustics. This dynamic illumination can be a visual hindrance for animals in shallow underwater environments. Animals in such habitats may have evolved to use polarization vision for discriminating objects while ignoring the variations in illumination caused by caustics. To explore this possibility, crabs (Carcinus maenas) and cuttlefish (Sepia officinalis), both of which have polarization vision, were presented with moving stimuli overlaid with caustics. Dynamic caustics inhibited the detection of an intensity-based stimulus but not when these stimuli were polarized. This study is the first to demonstrate that polarization vision reduces the negative impacts that dynamic illumination can have on visual perception.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | | | | | - James E. Herbert-Read
- Department of Zoology, University of Cambridge, UK
- Aquatic Ecology Unit, Department of Biology, Lund University, Sweden
| | - Martin J. How
- School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, UK
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Supple JA, Varennes-Phillit L, Gajjar-Reid D, Cerkvenik U, Belušič G, Krapp HG. Generating spatiotemporal patterns of linearly polarised light at high frame rates for insect vision research. J Exp Biol 2022; 225:275926. [PMID: 35708202 PMCID: PMC9339910 DOI: 10.1242/jeb.244087] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/31/2022] [Accepted: 06/13/2022] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
Polarisation vision is commonplace among invertebrates; however, most experiments focus on determining behavioural and/or neurophysiological responses to static polarised light sources rather than moving patterns of polarised light. To address the latter, we designed a polarisation stimulation device based on superimposing polarised and non-polarised images from two projectors, which can display moving patterns at frame rates exceeding invertebrate flicker fusion frequencies. A linear polariser fitted to one projector enables moving patterns of polarised light to be displayed, whilst the other projector contributes arbitrary intensities of non-polarised light to yield moving patterns with a defined polarisation and intensity contrast. To test the device, we measured receptive fields of polarisation-sensitive Argynnis paphia butterfly photoreceptors for both non-polarised and polarised light. We then measured local motion sensitivities of the optic flow-sensitive lobula plate tangential cell H1 in Calliphora vicina blowflies under both polarised and non-polarised light, finding no polarisation sensitivity in this neuron. Summary: Design of a versatile visual stimulation device for presenting moving patterns of polarised light, and demonstration of its use to characterise polarisation sensitivity in butterfly photoreceptors and blowfly motion-sensitive interneurons.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jack A Supple
- Department of Bioengineering, Imperial College London, Royal School of Mines, Exhibition Road, London, SW7 2AZ, UK
| | - Léandre Varennes-Phillit
- Department of Bioengineering, Imperial College London, Royal School of Mines, Exhibition Road, London, SW7 2AZ, UK
| | - Dexter Gajjar-Reid
- Department of Bioengineering, Imperial College London, Royal School of Mines, Exhibition Road, London, SW7 2AZ, UK
| | - Uroš Cerkvenik
- Department of Biology, Biotechnical Faculty, University of Ljubljana, 1000 Ljubljana, Slovenia
| | - Gregor Belušič
- Department of Biology, Biotechnical Faculty, University of Ljubljana, 1000 Ljubljana, Slovenia
| | - Holger G Krapp
- Department of Bioengineering, Imperial College London, Royal School of Mines, Exhibition Road, London, SW7 2AZ, UK
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Silva DJA, Erickson MF, dos Santos Guidi R, Pessoa DMA. Thin-fingered fiddler crabs display a natural preference for UV light cues but show no sensory bias to other hypertrophied claw coloration. Behav Processes 2022; 200:104667. [DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2022.104667] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/24/2021] [Revised: 05/06/2022] [Accepted: 05/30/2022] [Indexed: 12/27/2022]
|
10
|
Kagel SM, Garcia M, Cummings ME, Gruev V, Brady PC. Comparison of the polarization contrast of gelatinous zooplankton and a transparent single-use plastic bag-Implications for marine animals. MARINE POLLUTION BULLETIN 2021; 168:112438. [PMID: 33991983 DOI: 10.1016/j.marpolbul.2021.112438] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/28/2020] [Revised: 04/25/2021] [Accepted: 04/26/2021] [Indexed: 06/12/2023]
Abstract
Plastic pollution in the ocean is an increasingly detrimental issue for marine organisms. As a form of polarized light pollution, transparent plastic debris may be more visible and pose additional threats to organisms that can detect and interpret polarized light. Plastic can mimic the visual features of common marine prey items, such as transparent gelatinous zooplankton, which may lead to more significant plastic ingestion. We measured, in situ, the polarization and radiance contrast between a transparent plastic bag and gelatinous zooplankton with an underwater video polarimeter. The plastic bag had significantly higher polarization contrast than the gelatinous zooplankton, yet both shared similar radiance contrasts. This higher polarization contrast may contribute to the observed high ingestion rates of transparent plastic by marine organisms. Further study into the connection between polarization-sensitive organisms and plastic ingestion is recommended.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Samantha M Kagel
- University of Texas at Austin, Department of Integrative Biology, Austin, TX 78712, USA.
| | - Missael Garcia
- University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering, Urbana, IL 61801, USA
| | - Molly E Cummings
- University of Texas at Austin, Department of Integrative Biology, Austin, TX 78712, USA
| | - Viktor Gruev
- University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering, Urbana, IL 61801, USA; University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology, Urbana, IL 61801, USA
| | - Parrish C Brady
- University of Texas at Austin, Department of Integrative Biology, Austin, TX 78712, USA
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Dynamic visual noise promotes social attraction, but does not affect group size preference, in a shoaling fish. Anim Behav 2021. [DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2021.04.017] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
|
12
|
Drerup C, How MJ. Polarization contrasts and their effect on the gaze stabilization of crustaceans. J Exp Biol 2021; 224:237796. [PMID: 33692078 PMCID: PMC8077661 DOI: 10.1242/jeb.229898] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/25/2020] [Accepted: 03/01/2021] [Indexed: 01/20/2023]
Abstract
Many animals go to great lengths to stabilize their eyes relative to the visual scene and do so to enhance the localization of moving objects and to functionally partition the visual system relative to the outside world. An important cue that is used to control these stabilization movements is contrast within the visual surround. Previous studies on insects, spiders and fish have shown that gaze stabilization is achromatic (‘colour blind’), meaning that chromatic contrast alone (in the absence of apparent intensity contrasts) does not contribute to gaze stabilization. Following the assumption that polarization vision is analogous in many ways to colour vision, the present study shows that five different crustacean species do not use the polarization of light alone for gaze stabilization, despite being able to use this modality for detecting predator-like objects. This work therefore suggests that the gaze stabilization in many crustaceans cannot be elicited by the polarization of light alone. Summary: Five different crustacean species do not use the polarization of light alone for gaze stabilization, despite being able to use this modality for detecting predator-like objects.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Christian Drerup
- CCMAR (Centro de Ciências do Mar), Universidade do Algarve, Campus de Gambelas, 8005-139 Faro, Portugal.,Marine Behavioural Ecology Group, Department of Zoology, University of Cambridge, Downing St, Cambridge CB2 3EJ, UK
| | - Martin J How
- School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, 24 Tyndall Avenue, Bristol BS8 1TQ, UK
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Zhong B, Wang X, Gan X, Yang T, Gao J. A Biomimetic Model of Adaptive Contrast Vision Enhancement from Mantis Shrimp. SENSORS 2020; 20:s20164588. [PMID: 32824224 PMCID: PMC7472206 DOI: 10.3390/s20164588] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/13/2020] [Revised: 08/11/2020] [Accepted: 08/13/2020] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
Mantis shrimp have complex visual sensors, and thus, they have both color vision and polarization vision, and are adept at using polarization information for visual tasks, such as finding prey. In addition, mantis shrimp, almost unique among animals, can perform three-axis eye movements, such as pitch, yaw, and roll. With this behavior, polarization contrast in their field of view can be adjusted in real time. Inspired by this, we propose a bionic model that can adaptively enhance contrast vision. In this model, a pixel array is used to simulate a compound eye array, and the angle of polarization (AoP) is used as an adjustment mechanism. The polarization information is pre-processed by adjusting the direction of the photosensitive axis point-to-point. Experiments were performed around scenes where the color of the target and the background were similar, or the visibility of the target was low. The influence of the pre-processing model on traditional feature components of polarized light was analyzed. The results show that the model can effectively improve the contrast between the object and the background in the AoP image, enhance the significance of the object, and have important research significance for applications, such as contrast-based object detection.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Binbin Zhong
- School of Computer and Information, Hefei University of Technology, Hefei 230009, China; (B.Z.); (X.G.); (T.Y.); (J.G.)
| | - Xin Wang
- School of Computer and Information, Hefei University of Technology, Hefei 230009, China; (B.Z.); (X.G.); (T.Y.); (J.G.)
- Intelligent Interconnected Systems Laboratory of Anhui Province, Hefei 230009, China
- Correspondence:
| | - Xin Gan
- School of Computer and Information, Hefei University of Technology, Hefei 230009, China; (B.Z.); (X.G.); (T.Y.); (J.G.)
| | - Tian Yang
- School of Computer and Information, Hefei University of Technology, Hefei 230009, China; (B.Z.); (X.G.); (T.Y.); (J.G.)
| | - Jun Gao
- School of Computer and Information, Hefei University of Technology, Hefei 230009, China; (B.Z.); (X.G.); (T.Y.); (J.G.)
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Brodrick EA, Roberts NW, Sumner-Rooney L, Schlepütz CM, How MJ. Light adaptation mechanisms in the eye of the fiddler crab Afruca tangeri. J Comp Neurol 2020; 529:616-634. [PMID: 32592497 DOI: 10.1002/cne.24973] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/12/2020] [Revised: 06/18/2020] [Accepted: 06/22/2020] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
Abstract
A great diversity of adaptations is found among animals with compound eyes and even closely related taxa can show variation in their light-adaptation strategies. A prime example of a visual system evolved to function in specific light environments is the fiddler crab, used widely as a model to research aspects of crustacean vision and neural pathways. However, questions remain regarding how their eyes respond to the changes in brightness spanning many orders of magnitude, associated with their habitat and ecology. The fiddler crab Afruca tangeri forages at low tide on tropical and semi-tropical mudflats, under bright sunlight and on moonless nights, suggesting that their eyes undergo effective light adaptation. Using synchrotron X-ray tomography, light and transmission electron microscopy and in vivo ophthalmoscopy, we describe the ultrastructural changes in the eye between day and night. Dark adaptation at dusk triggered extensive widening of the rhabdoms and crystalline cone tips. This doubled the ommatidial acceptance angles and increased microvillar surface area for light capture in the rhabdom, theoretically boosting optical sensitivity 7.4 times. During daytime, only partial dark-adaptation was achieved and rhabdoms remained narrow, indicating strong circadian control on the process. Bright light did not evoke changes in screening pigment distributions, suggesting a structural inability to adapt rapidly to the light level fluctuations frequently experienced when entering their burrow to escape predators. This should enable fiddler crabs to shelter for several minutes without undergoing significant dark-adaptation, their vision remaining effectively adapted for predator detection when surfacing again in bright light.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Emelie A Brodrick
- Ecology of Vision Laboratory, School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK
| | - Nicholas W Roberts
- Ecology of Vision Laboratory, School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK
| | | | | | - Martin J How
- Ecology of Vision Laboratory, School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Guo X, Chu J, Wang Y, Wan Z, Li J, Lin M. Formation experiment with heading angle reference using sky polarization pattern at twilight. APPLIED OPTICS 2019; 58:9331-9337. [PMID: 31873522 DOI: 10.1364/ao.58.009331] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/19/2019] [Accepted: 10/31/2019] [Indexed: 06/10/2023]
Abstract
Consistency has always been an important topic in formation cooperation research. Traditional navigation methods, such as inertial navigation and geomagnetic navigation, have the disadvantages of error accumulation and low stability, thus reducing the consistency of formation. We propose to use the skylight polarization pattern to provide heading angle reference for formation cooperation of multi-agents. The experimental results show that the polarization navigation has good stability and no error accumulation. First, we analyzed the consistency of using the skylight polarization pattern to provide a heading reference for formation experiments. Then, based on the bionic polarization navigation sensor, we measured the difference of the skylight polarization azimuth of different observers at twilight. Further, a mobile robot platform was built with its heading angle provided by a polarization navigation sensor. Finally, we present an overview of a 3-robots platform formation experiment at twilight.
Collapse
|
16
|
Meglič A, Ilić M, Pirih P, Škorjanc A, Wehling MF, Kreft M, Belušič G. Horsefly object-directed polarotaxis is mediated by a stochastically distributed ommatidial subtype in the ventral retina. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2019; 116:21843-21853. [PMID: 31591223 PMCID: PMC6815168 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1910807116] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/09/2023] Open
Abstract
The ventral compound eye of many insects contains polarization-sensitive photoreceptors, but little is known about how they are integrated into visual functions. In female horseflies, polarized reflections from animal fur are a key stimulus for host detection. To understand how polarization vision is mediated by the ventral compound eye, we investigated the band-eyed brown horsefly Tabanus bromius using anatomical, physiological, and behavioral approaches. Serial electron microscopic sectioning of the retina and single-cell recordings were used to determine the spectral and polarization sensitivity (PS) of photoreceptors. We found 2 stochastically distributed subtypes of ommatidia, analogous to pale and yellow of other flies. Importantly, the pale analog contains an orthogonal analyzer receptor pair with high PS, formed by an ultraviolet (UV)-sensitive R7 and a UV- and blue-sensitive R8, while the UV-sensitive R7 and green-sensitive R8 in the yellow analog always have low PS. We tested horsefly polarotaxis in the field, using lures with controlled spectral and polarization composition. Polarized reflections without UV and blue components rendered the lures unattractive, while reflections without the green component increased their attractiveness. This is consistent with polarotaxis being guided by a differential signal from polarization analyzers in the pale analogs, and with an inhibitory role of the yellow analogs. Our results reveal how stochastically distributed sensory units with modality-specific division of labor serve as separate and opposing input channels for visual guidance.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Andrej Meglič
- Department of Biology, Biotechnical Faculty, University of Ljubljana, 1000 Ljubljana, Slovenia
| | - Marko Ilić
- Laboratory of Neuroethology, Sokendai - The Graduate University for Advanced Studies, 240-0193 Hayama, Japan
| | - Primož Pirih
- Department of Biology, Biotechnical Faculty, University of Ljubljana, 1000 Ljubljana, Slovenia
| | - Aleš Škorjanc
- Department of Biology, Biotechnical Faculty, University of Ljubljana, 1000 Ljubljana, Slovenia
| | - Martin F Wehling
- Nature-inspired Team, Sensor and Imaging Sciences Branch, Air Force Research Laboratory, Eglin Air Force Base, FL 32542
| | - Marko Kreft
- Department of Biology, Biotechnical Faculty, University of Ljubljana, 1000 Ljubljana, Slovenia
- Institute of Pathophysiology, Faculty of Medicine, University of Ljubljana, 1000 Ljubljana, Slovenia
- Celica Biomedical, 1000 Ljubljana, Slovenia
| | - Gregor Belušič
- Department of Biology, Biotechnical Faculty, University of Ljubljana, 1000 Ljubljana, Slovenia;
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Tzabari M, Lin W, Lerner A, Iluz D, Haspel C. Sensitivity study on the contribution of scattering by randomly oriented nonspherical hydrosols to linear polarization in clear to semi-turbid shallow waters. APPLIED OPTICS 2019; 58:7258-7279. [PMID: 31504002 DOI: 10.1364/ao.58.007258] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/20/2019] [Accepted: 07/26/2019] [Indexed: 06/10/2023]
Abstract
The influence of hydrosol nonsphericity on the polarization characteristics of light under water is investigated by combining accurate single-scattering models for randomly oriented spheroidal scatterers with a radiative transfer model that employs Stokes formalism and considers refraction of direct unpolarized solar radiation and 100% linearly polarized radiation at the air-water interface followed by single scattering. Variations in what we call the "linear polarization phase function" (the degree of linear polarization as a function of scattering angle and the angle of linear polarization as a function of scattering angle) are examined for a wide range of spheroid aspect ratios and complex refractive indices of hydrosols. Implications for polarization-sensitive marine organisms and for remote sensing of the marine environment are discussed.
Collapse
|
18
|
Smithers SP, Roberts NW, How MJ. Parallel processing of polarization and intensity information in fiddler crab vision. SCIENCE ADVANCES 2019; 5:eaax3572. [PMID: 31457103 PMCID: PMC6703871 DOI: 10.1126/sciadv.aax3572] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/16/2019] [Accepted: 07/15/2019] [Indexed: 06/10/2023]
Abstract
Many crustaceans are sensitive to the polarization of light and use this information for object-based visually guided behaviors. For these tasks, it is unknown whether polarization and intensity information are integrated into a single-contrast channel, whereby polarization directly contributes to perceived intensity, or whether they are processed separately and in parallel. Using a novel type of visual display that allowed polarization and intensity properties of visual stimuli to be adjusted independently and simultaneously, we conducted behavioral experiments with fiddler crabs to test which of these two models of visual processing occurs. We found that, for a loom detection task, fiddler crabs process polarization and intensity information independently and in parallel. The crab's response depended on whichever contrast was the most salient. By contributing independent measures of visual contrast, polarization and intensity provide a greater range of detectable contrast information for the receiver, increasing the chance of detecting a potential threat.
Collapse
|
19
|
Perez DM, Backwell PRY. Selection for conspicuous visual signals in a fiddler crab. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 2019. [DOI: 10.1007/s00265-019-2670-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/25/2022]
|
20
|
Swimming behaviour tunes fish polarization vision to double prey sighting distance. Sci Rep 2019; 9:944. [PMID: 30700806 PMCID: PMC6353921 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-018-37632-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/30/2018] [Accepted: 12/07/2018] [Indexed: 11/17/2022] Open
Abstract
The analysis of the polarization of light expands vision beyond the realm of colour and intensity and is used for multiple ecological purposes among invertebrates including orientation, object recognition, and communication. How vertebrates use polarization vision as part of natural behaviours is widely unknown. In this study, I tested the hypothesis that polarization vision improves the detection of zooplankton prey by the northern anchovy, Engraulis mordax, the only vertebrate with a demonstrated photoreceptor basis explaining its polarization sensitivity. Juvenile anchovies were recorded free foraging on zooplankton under downwelling light fields of varying percent polarization (98%, 67%, 19%, and 0% - unpolarized light). Analyses of prey attack sequences showed that anchovies swam in the horizontal plane perpendicular, on average, to the polarization direction of downwelling light and attacked prey at pitch angles that maximized polarization contrast perception of prey by the ventro-temporal retina, the area devoted to polarization vision in this animal. Consequently, the mean prey location distance under polarized light was up to 2.1 times that under unpolarized conditions. All indicators of polarization vision mediated foraging were present under 19% polarization, which is within the polarization range commonly found in nature during daylight hours. These results demonstrate: (i) the first use of oriented swimming for enhancing polarization contrast detection of prey, (ii) its relevance to improved foraging under available light cues in nature, and (iii) an increase in target detection distance that is only matched by polarization based artificial systems.
Collapse
|
21
|
Stewart FJ, Kinoshita M, Arikawa K. Monopolatic motion vision in the butterfly Papilio xuthus. J Exp Biol 2019; 222:222/1/jeb191957. [DOI: 10.1242/jeb.191957] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/07/2018] [Accepted: 10/31/2018] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
ABSTRACT
The swallowtail butterfly Papilio xuthus can perceive the linear polarization of light. Using a novel polarization projection system, we recently demonstrated that P. xuthus can detect visual motion based on polarization contrast. In the present study, we attempt to infer via behavioural experiments the mechanism underlying this polarization-based motion vision. Papilio xuthus do not perceive contrast between unpolarized and diagonally polarized light, implying that they cannot unambiguously estimate angle and degree of polarization, at least as far as motion detection is concerned. Furthermore, they conflate brightness and polarization cues, such that bright vertically polarized light resembles dim unpolarized light. These observations are consistent with a one-channel ‘monopolatic’ detector mechanism. We extend our existing model of motion vision in P. xuthus to incorporate these polarization findings, and conclude that the photoreceptors likely to form the basis for the putative monopolatic polarization detector are R3 and R4, which respond maximally to horizontally polarized green light. R5–R8, we propose, form a polarization-insensitive secondary channel tuned to longer wavelengths of light. Consistent with this account, we see greater sensitivity to polarization for green-light stimuli than for subjectively equiluminant red ones. Somewhat counter-intuitively, our model predicts greatest sensitivity to vertically polarized light; owing to the non-linearity of photoreceptor responses, light polarized to an angle orthogonal to a monopolatic detector's orientation offers the greatest contrast with unpolarized light.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Finlay J. Stewart
- Department of Evolutionary Studies of Biosystems, SOKENDAI (The Graduate University for Advanced Studies), Shonan International Village, Hayama, Kanagawa, 240-0193 Japan
| | - Michiyo Kinoshita
- Department of Evolutionary Studies of Biosystems, SOKENDAI (The Graduate University for Advanced Studies), Shonan International Village, Hayama, Kanagawa, 240-0193 Japan
| | - Kentaro Arikawa
- Department of Evolutionary Studies of Biosystems, SOKENDAI (The Graduate University for Advanced Studies), Shonan International Village, Hayama, Kanagawa, 240-0193 Japan
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
Backwell PRY. Synchronous waving in fiddler crabs: a review. Curr Zool 2018; 65:83-88. [PMID: 30697243 PMCID: PMC6347057 DOI: 10.1093/cz/zoy053] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/29/2018] [Accepted: 06/28/2018] [Indexed: 11/15/2022] Open
Abstract
Many animals that use acoustic communication synchronize their mate attraction signals: individuals precisely time their calls to overlap those of their neighbors. In contrast, synchrony in the mate attraction displays of species with visual/motion-based signals is rare. It has only been documented in five species of fiddler crabs. In all of them, small groups of males wave their single large claw in close synchrony. Here, I review what we know about synchrony in fiddler crabs, comparing the five species with each other to determine whether similar mechanisms and functions are common to all. I also propose future research questions that, if answered, would shed light on synchronous behavior in both visual and acoustic signallers.
Collapse
|
23
|
Basnak MA, Pérez-Schuster V, Hermitte G, Berón de Astrada M. Polarized object detection in crabs: a two-channel system. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2018; 221:jeb.173369. [PMID: 29650753 DOI: 10.1242/jeb.173369] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/02/2017] [Accepted: 04/10/2018] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
Many animal species take advantage of polarization vision for vital tasks such as orientation, communication and contrast enhancement. Previous studies have suggested that decapod crustaceans use a two-channel polarization system for contrast enhancement. Here, we characterize the polarization contrast sensitivity in a grapsid crab. We estimated the polarization contrast sensitivity of the animals by quantifying both their escape response and changes in heart rate when presented with polarized motion stimuli. The motion stimulus consisted of an expanding disk with an 82 deg polarization difference between the object and the background. More than 90% of animals responded by freezing or trying to avoid the polarized stimulus. In addition, we co-rotated the electric vector (e-vector) orientation of the light from the object and background by increments of 30 deg and found that the animals' escape response varied periodically with a 90 deg period. Maximum escape responses were obtained for object and background e-vectors near the vertical and horizontal orientations. Changes in cardiac response showed parallel results but also a minimum response when e-vectors of object and background were shifted by 45 deg with respect to the maxima. These results are consistent with an orthogonal receptor arrangement for the detection of polarized light, in which two channels are aligned with the vertical and horizontal orientations. It has been hypothesized that animals with object-based polarization vision rely on a two-channel detection system analogous to that of color processing in dichromats. Our results, obtained by systematically varying the e-vectors of object and background, provide strong empirical support for this theoretical model of polarized object detection.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Melanie Ailín Basnak
- Laboratorio de Neurobiología de la Memoria, Departamento de Fisiología, Biología Molecular y Celular, FCEyN, Universidad de Buenos Aires, IFIBYNE-CONICET, Buenos Aires, 1428, Argentina.,Department of Neurobiology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA 02115, USA
| | - Verónica Pérez-Schuster
- Laboratorio de Neurobiología de la Memoria, Departamento de Fisiología, Biología Molecular y Celular, FCEyN, Universidad de Buenos Aires, IFIBYNE-CONICET, Buenos Aires, 1428, Argentina.,Departamento de Física, FCEyN, Universidad de Buenos Aires, Buenos Aires, 1428, Argentina
| | - Gabriela Hermitte
- Laboratorio de Neurobiología de la Memoria, Departamento de Fisiología, Biología Molecular y Celular, FCEyN, Universidad de Buenos Aires, IFIBYNE-CONICET, Buenos Aires, 1428, Argentina
| | - Martín Berón de Astrada
- Laboratorio de Neurobiología de la Memoria, Departamento de Fisiología, Biología Molecular y Celular, FCEyN, Universidad de Buenos Aires, IFIBYNE-CONICET, Buenos Aires, 1428, Argentina
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Tibbs AB, Daly IM, Roberts NW, Bull DR. Denoising imaging polarimetry by adapted BM3D method. JOURNAL OF THE OPTICAL SOCIETY OF AMERICA. A, OPTICS, IMAGE SCIENCE, AND VISION 2018; 35:690-701. [PMID: 29603959 DOI: 10.1364/josaa.35.000690] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/31/2017] [Accepted: 02/26/2018] [Indexed: 06/08/2023]
Abstract
In addition to the visual information contained in intensity and color, imaging polarimetry allows visual information to be extracted from the polarization of light. However, a major challenge of imaging polarimetry is image degradation due to noise. This paper investigates the mitigation of noise through denoising algorithms and compares existing denoising algorithms with a new method, based on BM3D (Block Matching 3D). This algorithm, Polarization-BM3D (PBM3D), gives visual quality superior to the state of the art across all images and noise standard deviations tested. We show that denoising polarization images using PBM3D allows the degree of polarization to be more accurately calculated by comparing it with spectral polarimetry measurements.
Collapse
|
25
|
Foster JJ, Temple SE, How MJ, Daly IM, Sharkey CR, Wilby D, Roberts NW. Polarisation vision: overcoming challenges of working with a property of light we barely see. THE SCIENCE OF NATURE - NATURWISSENSCHAFTEN 2018; 105:27. [PMID: 29589169 PMCID: PMC5871655 DOI: 10.1007/s00114-018-1551-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 41] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/28/2017] [Revised: 02/27/2018] [Accepted: 03/01/2018] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
Abstract
In recent years, the study of polarisation vision in animals has seen numerous breakthroughs, not just in terms of what is known about the function of this sensory ability, but also in the experimental methods by which polarisation can be controlled, presented and measured. Once thought to be limited to only a few animal species, polarisation sensitivity is now known to be widespread across many taxonomic groups, and advances in experimental techniques are, in part, responsible for these discoveries. Nevertheless, its study remains challenging, perhaps because of our own poor sensitivity to the polarisation of light, but equally as a result of the slow spread of new practices and methodological innovations within the field. In this review, we introduce the most important steps in designing and calibrating polarised stimuli, within the broader context of areas of current research and the applications of new techniques to key questions. Our aim is to provide a constructive guide to help researchers, particularly those with no background in the physics of polarisation, to design robust experiments that are free from confounding factors.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- James J Foster
- Vision Group, Department of Biology, Lund University, Sölvegatan 35, 223 62, Lund, Sweden.
| | - Shelby E Temple
- Ecology of Vision Laboratory, School of Biological Sciences, Life Sciences Building, University of Bristol, Tyndall Avenue, Bristol, BS8 1TQ, UK
- Azul Optics Ltd., 7 Bishop Manor Road, Westbury-On-Trym, Bristol, BS10 5BD, UK
| | - Martin J How
- Ecology of Vision Laboratory, School of Biological Sciences, Life Sciences Building, University of Bristol, Tyndall Avenue, Bristol, BS8 1TQ, UK
| | - Ilse M Daly
- Ecology of Vision Laboratory, School of Biological Sciences, Life Sciences Building, University of Bristol, Tyndall Avenue, Bristol, BS8 1TQ, UK
| | - Camilla R Sharkey
- Department of Physiology, Development and Neuroscience, Cambridge University, Cambridge, CB2 3EG, UK
| | - David Wilby
- Ecology of Vision Laboratory, School of Biological Sciences, Life Sciences Building, University of Bristol, Tyndall Avenue, Bristol, BS8 1TQ, UK
| | - Nicholas W Roberts
- Ecology of Vision Laboratory, School of Biological Sciences, Life Sciences Building, University of Bristol, Tyndall Avenue, Bristol, BS8 1TQ, UK
| |
Collapse
|
26
|
Feller KD, Jordan TM, Wilby D, Roberts NW. Selection of the intrinsic polarization properties of animal optical materials creates enhanced structural reflectivity and camouflage. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2018; 372:rstb.2016.0336. [PMID: 28533453 PMCID: PMC5444057 DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2016.0336] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 03/03/2017] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Many animals use structural coloration to create bright and conspicuous visual signals. Selection of the size and shape of the optical structures animals use defines both the colour and intensity of the light reflected. The material used to create these reflectors is also important; however, animals are restricted to a limited number of materials: commonly chitin, guanine and the protein, reflectin. In this work we highlight that a particular set of material properties can also be under selection in order to increase the optical functionality of structural reflectors. Specifically, polarization properties, such as birefringence (the difference between the refractive indices of a material) and chirality (which relates to molecular asymmetry) are both under selection to create enhanced structural reflectivity. We demonstrate that the structural coloration of the gold beetle Chrysina resplendens and silvery reflective sides of the Atlantic herring, Clupea harengus are two examples of this phenomenon. Importantly, these polarization properties are not selected to control the polarization of the reflected light as a source of visual information per se. Instead, by creating higher levels of reflectivity than are otherwise possible, such internal polarization properties improve intensity-matching camouflage. This article is part of the themed issue ‘Animal coloration: production, perception, function and application’.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kathryn D Feller
- School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Bristol BS8 1TQ, UK
| | - Thomas M Jordan
- School of Geographical Sciences, University of Bristol, Bristol, BS8 1SS, UK
| | - David Wilby
- School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Bristol BS8 1TQ, UK
| | - Nicholas W Roberts
- School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Bristol BS8 1TQ, UK
| |
Collapse
|
27
|
Novales Flamarique I. A vertebrate retina with segregated colour and polarization sensitivity. Proc Biol Sci 2018; 284:rspb.2017.0759. [PMID: 28878058 DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2017.0759] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/08/2017] [Accepted: 07/25/2017] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Besides colour and intensity, some invertebrates are able to independently detect the polarization of light. Among vertebrates, such separation of visual modalities has only been hypothesized for some species of anchovies whose cone photoreceptors have unusual ultrastructure that varies with retinal location. Here, I tested this hypothesis by performing physiological experiments of colour and polarization discrimination using the northern anchovy, Engraulis mordax Optic nerve recordings showed that the ventro-temporal (VT), but not the ventro-nasal (VN), retina was polarization sensitive, and this coincided with the exclusive presence of polarization-sensitive photoreceptors in the VT retina. Spectral (colour) sensitivity recordings from the VN retina indicated the contribution of two spectral cone mechanisms to the optic nerve response, whereas only one contributed to the VT retina. This was supported by the presence of only one visual pigment in the VT retina and two in the VN retina, suggesting that only the VN retina was associated with colour sensitivity. Behavioural tests further demonstrated that anchovies could discriminate colour and the polarization of light using the ventral retina. Thus, in analogy with the visual system of some invertebrates, the northern anchovy has a retina with segregated retinal pathways for colour and polarization vision.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Iñigo Novales Flamarique
- Department of Biological Sciences, Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, British Columbia, Canada V3J 4M5 .,Department of Biology, University of Victoria, Victoria, British Columbia, Canada V8W 2Y2
| |
Collapse
|
28
|
Tibbs AB, Daly IM, Bull DR, Roberts NW. Noise creates polarization artefacts. BIOINSPIRATION & BIOMIMETICS 2017; 13:015005. [PMID: 29185995 DOI: 10.1088/1748-3190/aa9e22] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/07/2023]
Abstract
The accuracy of calculations of both the degree and angle of polarization depend strongly on the noise in the measurements used. The noise in the measurements recorded by both camera based systems and spectrometers can lead to significant artefacts and incorrect conclusions about high degrees of polarization when in fact none exist. Three approaches are taken in this work: firstly, the absolute error introduced as a function of the signal to noise ratio for polarization measurements is quantified in detail. An important finding here is the reason for why several studies incorrectly suggest that black (low reflectivity) objects are highly polarized. The high degree of polarization is only an artefact of the noise in the calculation. Secondly, several simple steps to avoid such errors are suggested. Thirdly, if these points can not be followed, two methods are presented for mitigating the effects of noise: a maximum likelihood estimation method and a new denoising algorithm to best calculate the degree of polarization of natural polarization information.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- A B Tibbs
- School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Bristol, BS8 1TQ, United Kingdom. Department of Electrical and Electronic Engineering, University of Bristol, Bristol, BS8 1UB, United Kingdom
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
29
|
Tadepalli S, Slocik JM, Gupta MK, Naik RR, Singamaneni S. Bio-Optics and Bio-Inspired Optical Materials. Chem Rev 2017; 117:12705-12763. [PMID: 28937748 DOI: 10.1021/acs.chemrev.7b00153] [Citation(s) in RCA: 174] [Impact Index Per Article: 21.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/19/2023]
Abstract
Through the use of the limited materials palette, optimally designed micro- and nanostructures, and tightly regulated processes, nature demonstrates exquisite control of light-matter interactions at various length scales. In fact, control of light-matter interactions is an important element in the evolutionary arms race and has led to highly engineered optical materials and systems. In this review, we present a detailed summary of various optical effects found in nature with a particular emphasis on the materials and optical design aspects responsible for their optical functionality. Using several representative examples, we discuss various optical phenomena, including absorption and transparency, diffraction, interference, reflection and antireflection, scattering, light harvesting, wave guiding and lensing, camouflage, and bioluminescence, that are responsible for the unique optical properties of materials and structures found in nature and biology. Great strides in understanding the design principles adapted by nature have led to a tremendous progress in realizing biomimetic and bioinspired optical materials and photonic devices. We discuss the various micro- and nanofabrication techniques that have been employed for realizing advanced biomimetic optical structures.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sirimuvva Tadepalli
- Department of Mechanical Engineering and Materials Science and Institute of Materials Science and Engineering, Washington University in St. Louis , St. Louis, Missouri 63130, United States
| | | | | | | | - Srikanth Singamaneni
- Department of Mechanical Engineering and Materials Science and Institute of Materials Science and Engineering, Washington University in St. Louis , St. Louis, Missouri 63130, United States
| |
Collapse
|
30
|
Templin RM, How MJ, Roberts NW, Chiou TH, Marshall J. Circularly polarized light detection in stomatopod crustaceans: a comparison of photoreceptors and possible function in six species. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2017; 220:3222-3230. [PMID: 28667244 DOI: 10.1242/jeb.162941] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/16/2017] [Accepted: 06/27/2017] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
A combination of behavioural and electrophysiological experiments have previously shown that two species of stomatopod, Odontodactylus scyllarus and Gonodactylaceus falcatus, can differentiate between left- and right-handed circularly polarized light (CPL), and between CPL and linearly polarized light (LPL). It remains unknown if these visual abilities are common across all stomatopod species, and if so, how circular polarization sensitivity may vary between and within species. A subsection of the midband, a specialized region of stomatopod eyes, contains distally placed photoreceptor cells, termed R8 (retinular cell number 8). These cells are specifically built with unidirectional microvilli and appear to be angled precisely to convert CPL into LPL. They are mostly quarter-wave retarders for human visible light (400-700 nm), as well as being ultraviolet-sensitive linear polarization detectors. The effectiveness of the R8 cells in this role is determined by their geometric and optical properties. In particular, the length and birefringence of the R8 cells are crucial for retardation efficiency. Here, our comparative studies show that most species investigated have the theoretical ability to convert CPL into LPL, such that the handedness of an incoming circular reflection or signal could be discriminated. One species, Haptosquilla trispinosa, shows less than quarter-wave retardance. Whilst some species are known to produce circularly polarized reflections (some Odontodactylus species and G. falcatus, for example), others do not, so a variety of functions for this ability are worth considering.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rachel M Templin
- Queensland Brain Institute, University of Queensland, St Lucia, QLD 4072, Australia
| | - Martin J How
- School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Bristol BS8 1TQ, UK
| | - Nicholas W Roberts
- School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Bristol BS8 1TQ, UK
| | - Tsyr-Huei Chiou
- Department of Life Sciences, National Cheng Kung University, Tainan 701, Taiwan
| | - Justin Marshall
- Queensland Brain Institute, University of Queensland, St Lucia, QLD 4072, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
31
|
Abstract
The visual world is rich in linearly polarized light stimuli, which are hidden from the human eye. But many invertebrate species make use of polarized light as a source of valuable visual information. However, exploiting light polarization does not necessarily imply that the electric (e)-vector orientation of polarized light can be perceived as a separate modality of light. In this Review, I address the question of whether invertebrates can detect specific e-vector orientations in a manner similar to that of humans perceiving spectral stimuli as specific hues. To analyze e-vector orientation, the signals of at least three polarization-sensitive sensors (analyzer channels) with different e-vector tuning axes must be compared. The object-based, imaging polarization vision systems of cephalopods and crustaceans, as well as the water-surface detectors of flying backswimmers, use just two analyzer channels. Although this excludes the perception of specific e-vector orientations, a two-channel system does provide a coarse, categoric analysis of polarized light stimuli, comparable to the limited color sense of dichromatic, 'color-blind' humans. The celestial compass of insects employs three or more analyzer channels. However, that compass is multimodal, i.e. e-vector information merges with directional information from other celestial cues, such as the solar azimuth and the spectral gradient in the sky, masking e-vector information. It seems that invertebrate organisms take no interest in the polarization details of visual stimuli, but polarization vision grants more practical benefits, such as improved object detection and visual communication for cephalopods and crustaceans, compass readings to traveling insects, or the alert 'water below!' to water-seeking bugs.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Thomas Labhart
- Institute of Molecular Life Sciences, University of Zurich, Winterthurerstrasse 190, Zürich CH 8057, Switzerland
| |
Collapse
|