1
|
Boyd R, Richerson PJ. Large-scale cooperation in small-scale foraging societies. Evol Anthropol 2022; 31:175-198. [PMID: 35485603 DOI: 10.1002/evan.21944] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 9.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/12/2021] [Revised: 10/16/2021] [Accepted: 03/16/2022] [Indexed: 02/06/2023]
Abstract
We present evidence that people in small-scale mobile hunter-gatherer societies cooperated in large numbers to produce collective goods. Foragers engaged in large-scale communal hunts and constructed shared capital facilities; they made shared investments in improving the local environment; and they participated in warfare, formed enduring alliances, and established trading networks. Large-scale collective action often played a crucial role in subsistence. The provision of public goods involved the cooperation of many individuals, so each person made only a small contribution. This evidence suggests that large-scale cooperation occurred in the Pleistocene societies that encompass most of human evolutionary history, and therefore it is unlikely that large-scale cooperation in Holocene food producing societies results from an evolved psychology shaped only in small-group interactions. Instead, large-scale human cooperation needs to be explained as an adaptation, likely rooted in distinctive features of human biology, grammatical language, increased cognitive ability, and cumulative cultural adaptation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Robert Boyd
- Institute for Human Origins, School of Human Evolution and Social Change, Arizona State University, Tempe, Arizona, USA
| | - Peter J Richerson
- Department of Environmental Science and Policy, University of California, Davis, California, USA
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Tucker B, Ringen EJ, Tombo J, Hajasoa P, Gérard S, Lahiniriko R, Garçon AH. Ethnic Markers without Ethnic Conflict : Why do Interdependent Masikoro, Mikea, and Vezo of Madagascar Signal their Ethnic Differences? HUMAN NATURE-AN INTERDISCIPLINARY BIOSOCIAL PERSPECTIVE 2021; 32:529-556. [PMID: 34546550 DOI: 10.1007/s12110-021-09412-w] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 07/20/2021] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Abstract
People often signal their membership in groups through their clothes, hairstyle, posture, and dialect. Most existing evolutionary models argue that markers label group members so individuals can preferentially interact with those in their group. Here we ask why people mark ethnic differences when interethnic interaction is routine, necessary, and peaceful. We asked research participants from three ethnic groups in southwestern Madagascar to sort photos of unfamiliar people by ethnicity, and by with whom they would prefer or not prefer to cooperate, in a wage labor vignette. Results indicate that southwestern Malagasy reliably send and detect ethnic signals; they signal less in the marketplace, a primary site of interethnic coordination and cooperation; and they do not prefer co-ethnics as cooperation partners in novel circumstances. Results from a cultural knowledge survey and calculations of cultural FST suggest that these ethnic groups have relatively little cultural differentiation. We concur with Moya and Boyd (Human Nature 26:1-27, 2015) that ethnicity is unlikely to be a singular social phenomenon. The current functions of ethnic divisions and marking may be different from those at the moment of ethnogenesis. Group identities may persist without group conflict or differentiation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Bram Tucker
- Department of Anthropology, University of Georgia, Athens, GA, USA.
| | - Erik J Ringen
- Department of Anthropology, Emory University, Atlanta, GA, USA
| | - Jaovola Tombo
- Department of Geography, Université de Toliara, Toliara, Madagascar
| | - Patricia Hajasoa
- Department of Geography, Université de Toliara, Toliara, Madagascar
| | - Soanahary Gérard
- Department of Geography, Université de Toliara, Toliara, Madagascar
| | - Rolland Lahiniriko
- Department of Malagasy Language, Civilization, and Letters, Université de Toliara, Toliara, Madagascar
| | | |
Collapse
|
3
|
Schahbasi A, Huber S, Fieder M. Factors affecting attitudes toward migrants-An evolutionary approach. Am J Hum Biol 2021; 33:e23435. [PMID: 32458587 PMCID: PMC7900986 DOI: 10.1002/ajhb.23435] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/22/2019] [Revised: 04/14/2020] [Accepted: 05/06/2020] [Indexed: 12/31/2022] Open
Abstract
OBJECTIVE To understand migration from an evolutionary perspective, this phenomenon has so far been mainly investigated in animal species. We therefore aim to investigate the potential evolutionary roots of attitudes toward migrants in humans. METHODS We used data from the European Social Survey (n = 83 734), analyzing attitudes toward migrants by performing ordinal mixed models. RESULTS We found that men have a more restrictive attitude toward migration than women, which increases with age and is stronger with a child in the household. Attitude toward migrants is also more skeptical if migrants have a different ethnicity and are from poorer countries. Increasing education and religiousness are associated with a more positive attitude toward migrants, particularly toward migrants of different ethnicity and from poorer countries. DISCUSSION Although migration flows are a hallmark of the human species, previous findings suggest that (pre-)historic migration flows were at times accompanied by conflict and violence, while at the same time, they insured survival by allowing cultural exchange and the avoidance of inbreeding. Accordingly, we assume that contemporary attitudes toward migration are rooted in our evolutionary past. We discuss the respective behavioral patterns from an evolutionary perspective, arguing that both-a negative attitude as well as openness-make sense.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alexander Schahbasi
- Department of Evolutionary AnthropologyUniversity of ViennaViennaAustria
- Erlangen Centre for Islam & Law in EuropeFriedrich‐Alexander University Erlangen‐NürnbergErlangenGermany
| | - Susanne Huber
- Department of Evolutionary AnthropologyUniversity of ViennaViennaAustria
| | - Martin Fieder
- Department of Evolutionary AnthropologyUniversity of ViennaViennaAustria
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Woodley Of Menie MA, Kanazawa S, Pallesen J, Sarraf MA. Paternal Age is Negatively Associated with Religious Behavior in a Post-60s But Not a Pre-60s US Birth Cohort: Testing a Prediction from the Social Epistasis Amplification Model. JOURNAL OF RELIGION AND HEALTH 2020; 59:2733-2752. [PMID: 32006140 DOI: 10.1007/s10943-020-00987-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/08/2023]
Abstract
Participation in social behaviors that enhance group-level fitness may be influenced by mutations that affect patterns of social epistasis in human populations. Mutations that cause individuals to not participate in these behaviors may weaken the ability of members of a group to coordinate and regulate behavior, which may in turn negatively affect fitness. To investigate the possibility that de novo mutations degrade these adaptive social behaviors, we examine the effect of paternal age (as a well-established proxy for de novo mutation load) on one such social behavior, namely religious observance, since religiosity may be a group-level cultural adaptation facilitating enhanced social coordination. Using two large samples (Wisconsin Longitudinal Study and AddHealth), each of a different US birth cohort, paternal age was used to hierarchically predict respondent's level of church attendance after controlling for multiple covariates. The effect is absent in WLS (β = .007, ns, N = 4560); however, it is present in AddHealth (β = - .046, p < .05, N = 4873) increasing the adjusted model R2 by .005. The WLS respondents were (mostly) born in the 1930s, whereas the AddHealth respondents were (mostly) born in the 1970s. This may indicate that social-epistatic regulation of behavior has weakened historically in the USA, which might stem from and enhance the ability for de novo mutations to influence behavior among more recently born cohorts-paralleling the secular rise in the heritability of age at sexual debut after the sexual revolution.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michael A Woodley Of Menie
- Center Leo Apostel for Interdisciplinary Studies, Vrije Universiteit Brussel, Brussels, Belgium.
- Unz Foundation, Palo Alto, CA, USA.
| | - Satoshi Kanazawa
- School of Management, London School of Economics and Political Science, London, UK
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
5
|
Levels of analysis and problems of evidential support in the study of asymmetric conflict. Behav Brain Sci 2019; 42:e142. [PMID: 31407976 DOI: 10.1017/s0140525x19000682] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
Abstract
The contribution by De Dreu and Gross oversimplifies the complexity of the topic. I provide counterarguments that undermine the two sweeping contentions on which the article's argument depends, and I argue that asymmetric conflict is best understood at the finer-grained level of studying the sequences of strikes and counterstrikes that the rival actors have in store for one another.
Collapse
|
6
|
Grigoryev D, Fiske ST, Batkhina A. Mapping Ethnic Stereotypes and Their Antecedents in Russia: The Stereotype Content Model. Front Psychol 2019; 10:1643. [PMID: 31379677 PMCID: PMC6646730 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01643] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/23/2019] [Accepted: 06/28/2019] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
The stereotype content model (SCM), originating in the United States and generalized across nearly 50 countries, has yet to address ethnic relations in one of the world's most influential nations. Russia and the United States are somewhat alike (large, powerful, immigrant-receiving), but differ in other ways relevant to intergroup images (culture, religions, ideology, and history). Russian ethnic stereotypes are understudied, but significant for theoretical breadth and practical politics. This research tested the SCM on ethnic stereotypes in a Russian sample (N = 1115). Study 1 (N = 438) produced an SCM map of the sixty most numerous domestic ethnic groups (both ethnic minorities and immigrants). Four clusters occupied the SCM warmth-by-competence space. Study 2 (N = 677) compared approaches to ethnic stereotypes in terms of status and competition, cultural distance, perceived region, and four intergroup threats. Using the same Study 1 groups, the Russian SCM map showed correlated warmth and competence, with few ambivalent stereotypes. As the SCM predicts, status predicted competence, and competition negatively predicted warmth. Beyond the SCM, status and property threat both were robust antecedents for both competence and warmth for all groups. Besides competition, cultural distance also negatively predicted warmth for all groups. The role of the other antecedents, as expected, varied from group to group. To examine relative impact, a network analysis demonstrated that status, competition, and property threat centrally influence many other variables in the networks. The SCM, along with antecedents from other models, describes Russian ethnic-group images. This research contributes: (1) a comparison of established approaches to ethnic stereotypes (from acculturation and intergroup relations) showing the stability of the main SCM predictions; (2) network structures of the multivariate dependencies of the considered variables; (3) systematically cataloged images of ethnic groups in Russia for further comparisons, illuminating the Russian historical, societal, and interethnic context.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dmitry Grigoryev
- National Research University Higher School of Economics, Moscow, Russia
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
7
|
Liu S, Mou Z, Xie W, Zhang C, Chen Y, Guo W, Zhang X, Zhang L. The Effect of the Irreversible Inequality on Pro-social Behaviors of People With Disabilities. Front Psychol 2019; 10:12. [PMID: 30766495 PMCID: PMC6366110 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/22/2018] [Accepted: 01/04/2019] [Indexed: 11/30/2022] Open
Abstract
Inequalities have always been central to psychology, sociology and related fields such as social policy, gender studies, critical race studies, and human geography. Although inequality affects pro-social behaviors, there are still some controversies over this issue among people with disabilities. The current study aimed to investigate pro-social behaviors of people with disabilities and the effect of the irreversible inequality on pro-social behaviors. A dictator game was used to explore the difference of pro-social behaviors between people with disabilities and people without disabilities, when facing intra- or inter-group members. The results showed that compared to people with disabilities, people without disabilities were likely to show more pro-social behaviors. People with disabilities preferred intra-group cooperation, while people without disabilities preferred inter-group cooperation. Indeed, the intra-group cooperation was significantly greater than the expected cooperation of the intra-group members for people with disabilities. When facing the inter-group members, people without disabilities showed more than expected, that others would cooperate with them. These findings indicated that social avoidance was a common phenomenon for people with disabilities in China, but the situation would be different when they faced different groups. In addition, irreversible inequality could influence individuals' cooperative strategies when facing individuals in a different status.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Shen Liu
- Department and Institute of Psychology, Ningbo University, Ningbo, China
- School of Humanities and Social Science, University of Science and Technology of China, Hefei, China
| | - Zhongchen Mou
- Department and Institute of Psychology, Ningbo University, Ningbo, China
- School of Psychology, Nanjing Normal UniversityNanjing, China
| | - Wenlan Xie
- Department and Institute of Psychology, Ningbo University, Ningbo, China
- Ningbo Institute of Education, Ningbo, China
| | - Chong Zhang
- Department of Mechanical and Automation Engineering, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong, China
| | - Yijun Chen
- CAS Key Laboratory of Brain Function and Disease, School of Life Sciences, University of Science and Technology of China, Hefei, China
| | - Wen Guo
- School of Humanities and Social Science, University of Science and Technology of China, Hefei, China
| | - Xiaochu Zhang
- School of Humanities and Social Science, University of Science and Technology of China, Hefei, China
- CAS Key Laboratory of Brain Function and Disease, School of Life Sciences, University of Science and Technology of China, Hefei, China
- Hefei Medical Research Center on Alcohol Addiction, Anhui Mental Health Center, Hefei, China
- Academy of Psychology and Behavior, Tianjin Normal University, Tianjin, China
| | - Lin Zhang
- Department and Institute of Psychology, Ningbo University, Ningbo, China
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Mapping Ideology: Combining the Schwartz Value Circumplex with Evolutionary Theory to Explain Ideological Differences. EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE 2018. [DOI: 10.1007/s40806-018-0165-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/28/2022]
|