1
|
Deeb H, Vrij A, Burkhardt J, Leal S, Mann S. Veracity Judgments Based on Complications: A Training Experiment. Behav Sci (Basel) 2024; 14:839. [PMID: 39336054 PMCID: PMC11429330 DOI: 10.3390/bs14090839] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/15/2024] [Revised: 09/10/2024] [Accepted: 09/12/2024] [Indexed: 09/30/2024] Open
Abstract
Research has shown that complications are more common in truth tellers' accounts than in lie tellers' accounts, but there is currently no experiment that has examined the accuracy of observers' veracity judgments when looking at complications. A total of 87 participants were asked to judge 10 transcripts (five truthful and five false) derived from a set of 59 transcripts generated in a previous experiment by Deeb et al. Approximately half of the participants were trained to detect complications (Trained), and the other half did not receive training (Untrained). Trained participants were more likely to look for complications, but they did not detect them accurately, and thus their veracity judgments did not improve beyond Untrained participants' judgments. We discuss that the training may have been too brief or not sensitive enough to enhance decision-making.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Haneen Deeb
- School of Psychology, Sport and Health Sciences, University of Portsmouth, Portsmouth PO1 2DY, UK; (A.V.); (J.B.); (S.L.); (S.M.)
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
2
|
Leal S, Vrij A, Ashkenazi T, Vernham Z, Fisher RP, Palena N. Introducing the high-context communication style interview protocol to detect deception in pairs. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2024; 249:104440. [PMID: 39167909 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2024.104440] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/05/2023] [Revised: 07/26/2024] [Accepted: 07/30/2024] [Indexed: 08/23/2024] Open
Abstract
In four experiments, we examined whether pairs of truth tellers could be distinguished from pairs of lie tellers by taking advantage of the fact that only pairs of truth tellers can refer to shared events by using brief expressions (high-context communication style). In Experiments 1 and 2, pairs of friends and pairs of strangers pretending to be friends answered (i) questions they likely had expected to be asked (e.g., 'How did you first meet'?) and (ii) unexpected questions (e.g., 'First, describe a shared event in a few words. Then elaborate on it'). Pairs were interviewed individually (Experiment 1, N = 134 individuals) or collectively (Experiment 2, N = 130 individuals). Transcripts were coded for the verbal cues details, complications, plausibility, predictability, and overlap (Experiment 1 only) or repetitions (Experiment 2 only). In two lie detection experiments observers read the individual transcripts in Experiment 3 (N = 146) or the collective transcripts in Experiment 4 (N = 138). The verbal cues were more diagnostic of veracity and observers were better at distinguishing between truths and lies in the unexpected than in the expected questions condition, but only when the pair members were interviewed individually.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sharon Leal
- School of Psychology, Sport, & Health Sciences, University of Portsmouth, PO1 2DY Hants, UK.
| | - Aldert Vrij
- School of Psychology, Sport, & Health Sciences, University of Portsmouth, PO1 2DY Hants, UK
| | - Tzachi Ashkenazi
- Ashkelon Academic College, Department of Criminology, Bar Ilan University, Israel
| | - Zarah Vernham
- School of Psychology, Sport, & Health Sciences, University of Portsmouth, PO1 2DY Hants, UK
| | - Ronald P Fisher
- Department of Psychology, Florida International University, USA
| | - Nicola Palena
- Netherlands Institute for the Study of Crime and Law Enforcement, Leiden, the Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Prodan N, Ding XP, Visu-Petra L. Truthful yet misleading: Elementary second-order deception in school-age children and its sociocognitive correlates. J Exp Child Psychol 2024; 237:105759. [PMID: 37597452 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2023.105759] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/16/2023] [Revised: 06/29/2023] [Accepted: 07/24/2023] [Indexed: 08/21/2023]
Abstract
In highly competitive contexts, deceptive intentions might be transparent, so conveying only false information to the opponent can become a predictable strategy. In such situations, alternating between truths and lies (second-order lying behavior) represents a less foreseeable option. The current study investigated the development of 8- to 10-year-old children's elementary second-order deception in relation to their attribution of ignorance (first- and second-order ignorance) and executive functions (inhibitory control, shifting ability, and verbal working memory). An adapted version of the hide-and-seek paradigm was used to assess children's second-order lie-telling, in which children were asked to hide a coin in either of their hands. Unlike the standard paradigm, the opponent did not consistently look for the coin in the location indicated by the children, so children needed to switch between telling simple lies and truths (elementary second-order lies about the coin location) to successfully deceive the recipient. The results showed that older children were less likely to tell elementary second-order lies. However, across the sample, when children decided to lie, this ability was positively related to their second-order ignorance attribution and their verbal working memory. Moreover, we obtained preliminary evidence for the presence of a habituation effect in second-order lying, with children being more accurate and having less variability in their truthful-to-deceive responses (this being the more frequently elicited response) than when telling lies to deceive. Our findings could have implications for understanding the mechanisms underlying children's ability to alternate between truths and lies to deceive.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Narcisa Prodan
- Department of Psychology, Babes-Bolyai University, 400015 Cluj-Napoca, Cluj, Romania
| | - Xiao Pan Ding
- Department of Psychology, National University of Singapore, 117570 Singapore, Singapore
| | - Laura Visu-Petra
- Department of Psychology, Babes-Bolyai University, 400015 Cluj-Napoca, Cluj, Romania; Department of Social and Human Research, Romanian Academy, 400015 Cluj-Napoca, Romania.
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Junger M, Koning L, Hartel P, Veldkamp B. In their own words: deception detection by victims and near victims of fraud. Front Psychol 2023; 14:1135369. [PMID: 37251040 PMCID: PMC10213419 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1135369] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/31/2022] [Accepted: 04/18/2023] [Indexed: 05/31/2023] Open
Abstract
Aim Research on deception detection has usually been executed in experimental settings in the laboratory. In contrast, the present research investigates deception detection by actual victims and near victims of fraud, as reported in their own words. Materials and methods Our study is based on a nationally representative survey of 11 types of (mostly) online fraud victimization (N = 2,864). We used qualitative information from actual victims and near victims on why they didn't fall for the fraud, or how, in hindsight, it could have been prevented. Results The main detection strategies mentioned by near victims (N = 958) were 1) fraud knowledge (69%): these near victims clearly recognized fraud. Other strategies related to fraud knowledge were: noticing mistakes (27.9%), rules and principles about safe conduct (11.7%), and personal knowledge (7.1%). A second type of strategy was distrust (26.1%). A third strategy was 'wise through experience' (1.6%). Finally, a limited number of respondents (7.8%) searched for additional information: they contacted other people (5.5%), sought information online (4%), contacted the fraudster (2.9%), contacted their bank or credit card company (2.2%), or contacted the police (0.2%). Using knowledge as a strategy decreases the probability of victimization by a factor of 0.43. In contrast, all other strategies increased the likelihood of victimization by a factor of 1.6 or more. Strategies generally were uncorrelated, several strategies differed by type of fraud. About 40% of the actual victims (N = 243) believed that their victimization might have been prevented by: 1) seeking information (25.2%), 2) paying more attention (18.9%), 3) a third party doing something (16.2%), 4) following safety rules or principles, like using a safer way of paying or trading (14.4%), or by 5) 'simply not going along with it' (10.8%). Most of these strategies were associated with a higher, not lower, likelihood of victimization. Conclusion Clearly, knowledge of fraud is the best strategy to avoid fraud victimization. Therefore, a more proactive approach is needed to inform the public about fraud and attackers' modus operandi, so that potential victims already have knowledge of fraud upon encountering it. Just providing information online will not suffice to protect online users.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marianne Junger
- Industrial Engineering and Business Information Systems (IEBIS), Faculty of Behavioural, Management and Social Sciences (BMS), University of Twente, Enschede, Netherlands
| | - Luka Koning
- Industrial Engineering and Business Information Systems (IEBIS), Faculty of Behavioural, Management and Social Sciences (BMS), University of Twente, Enschede, Netherlands
| | - Pieter Hartel
- Department of Services, Cybersecurity and Safety, Faculty of Electrical Engineering, Mathematics and Computer Science, University of Twente, Enschede, Netherlands
| | - Bernard Veldkamp
- Department of Cognition, Data and Education (CODE) Faculty of Behavioural, Management and Social Sciences (BMS), University of Twente, Enschede, Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Armstrong ME, Jones KS, Namin AS. How Perceptions of Caller Honesty Vary During Vishing Attacks That Include Highly Sensitive or Seemingly Innocuous Requests. HUMAN FACTORS 2023; 65:275-287. [PMID: 33934614 DOI: 10.1177/00187208211012818] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/12/2023]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE To understand how aspects of vishing calls (phishing phone calls) influence perceived visher honesty. BACKGROUND Little is understood about how targeted individuals behave during vishing attacks. According to truth-default theory, people assume others are being honest until something triggers their suspicion. We investigated whether that was true during vishing attacks. METHODS Twenty-four participants read written descriptions of eight real-world vishing calls. Half included highly sensitive requests; the remainder included seemingly innocuous requests. Participants rated visher honesty at multiple points during conversations. RESULTS Participants initially perceived vishers to be honest. Honesty ratings decreased before requests occurred. Honesty ratings decreased further in response to highly sensitive requests, but not seemingly innocuous requests. Honesty ratings recovered somewhat, but only after highly sensitive requests. CONCLUSIONS The present results revealed five important insights: (1) people begin vishing conversations in the truth-default state, (2) certain aspects of vishing conversations serve as triggers, (3) other aspects of vishing conversations do not serve as triggers, (4) in certain situations, people's perceptions of visher honesty improve, and, more generally, (5) truth-default theory may be a useful tool for understanding how targeted individuals behave during vishing attacks. APPLICATION Those developing systems that help users deal with suspected vishing attacks or penetration testing plans should consider (1) targeted individuals' truth-bias, (2) the influence of visher demeanor on the likelihood of deception detection, (3) the influence of fabricated situations surrounding vishing requests on the likelihood of deception detection, and (4) targeted individuals' lack of concern about seemingly innocuous requests.
Collapse
|
6
|
Stel M, van Dijk E. Response: Commentary: Distrust, False Cues, and Below-Chance Deception Detection Accuracy: Commentary on Stel et al. (2020) and Further Reflections on (Un)Conscious Lie Detection From the Perspective of Truth-Default Theory. Front Psychol 2021; 12:763218. [PMID: 34777168 PMCID: PMC8579807 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.763218] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/23/2021] [Accepted: 09/30/2021] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Affiliation(s)
- Mariëlle Stel
- Department of Psychology of Conflict, Risk, and Safety, University of Twente, Enschede, Netherlands
| | - Eric van Dijk
- Department of Social, Economic, and Organisational Psychology, Leiden University, Leiden, Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Markowitz DM. The deception faucet: A metaphor to conceptualize deception and its detection. NEW IDEAS IN PSYCHOLOGY 2020. [DOI: 10.1016/j.newideapsych.2020.100816] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
|
8
|
Vrij A, Fisher RP. Unraveling the Misconception About Deception and Nervous Behavior. Front Psychol 2020; 11:1377. [PMID: 32625154 PMCID: PMC7314921 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01377] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/26/2020] [Accepted: 05/22/2020] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
In this article, we attempt to unravel the misconception about deception and nervous behavior. First we will cite research demonstrating that observers believe lie tellers display more nervous behaviors than truth tellers; that observers pay attention to nervous behaviors when they attempt to detect deception; and that lie tellers actually feel more nervous than truth tellers. This is all in alignment with a lie detection approach based on spotting nervous behaviors. We then will argue that the next, vital, step is missing: Research has found that lie tellers generally do not display more than truth tellers the nervous behaviors laypersons and professionals appear to focus on. If observers pay attention to nervous behaviors but lie tellers do not come across as being nervous, lie detection performance is expected to be poor. Research has supported this claim. We finally discuss ideas for research into lie detection based on non-verbal behaviors.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Aldert Vrij
- Department of Psychology, University of Portsmouth, Portsmouth, United Kingdom
| | - Ronald P Fisher
- Department of Psychology, Florida International University, Miami, FL, United States
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Ask K, Calderon S, Mac Giolla E, Reinhard MA. Approach, Avoidance, and the Perception of Credibility. OPEN PSYCHOLOGY 2020. [DOI: 10.1515/psych-2020-0002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/15/2022] Open
Abstract
AbstractBased on a functional approach to credibility judgments, the authors hypothesize that receivers’ judgments of senders’ credibility involve an evaluative dimension (i.e., good–bad) and are associated with approach and avoidance tendencies. In three experiments (total N = 645), participants (receivers) judged the credibility of suspects (senders) denying involvement in a mock theft. While watching or reading the message, receivers performed an approach-related (arm flexion) or an avoidance-related (arm extension) motor action. Although receivers’ affective evaluations of senders (good–bad) correlated strongly with credibility judgments in all three experiments, the results of the arm position manipulation were mixed. In Experiment 1, receivers in an arm flexion (vs. arm extension) state judged the sender as more credible, but only when informed beforehand about the upcoming credibility judgment. In Experiment 2 and 3, however, there was no evidence of an arm position effect on credibility judgments. A cross-experimental meta-analysis revealed that the effect of the manipulation was statistically indistinguishable from zero, Hedges’ g = 0.07, 95% CI [−0.09, 0.22], and provided strong support for the null hypothesis. Multiple interpretations of the results are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Karl Ask
- Department of Psychology, University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden
| | - Sofia Calderon
- Department of Psychology, University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden
| | - Erik Mac Giolla
- Department of Psychology, University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden
| | | |
Collapse
|
10
|
Volz S, Reinhard M, Müller P. Why don't you believe me? Detecting deception in messages written by nonnative and native speakers. APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2019. [DOI: 10.1002/acp.3615] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Sarah Volz
- Department of PsychologyUniversity of Kassel Kassel Germany
| | | | - Patrick Müller
- Faculty of Civil Engineering, Building Physics, and BusinessUniversity of Applied Sciences Stuttgart Stuttgart Germany
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Caso L, Palena N, Carlessi E, Vrij A. Police accuracy in truth/lie detection when judging baseline interviews. PSYCHIATRY, PSYCHOLOGY, AND LAW : AN INTERDISCIPLINARY JOURNAL OF THE AUSTRALIAN AND NEW ZEALAND ASSOCIATION OF PSYCHIATRY, PSYCHOLOGY AND LAW 2019; 26:841-850. [PMID: 32128012 PMCID: PMC7033705 DOI: 10.1080/13218719.2019.1642258] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/01/2023]
Abstract
Research has shown that a comparable truth baseline (CTB) approach elicits more cues to deception and results in higher accuracy rates than a small talk baseline. Past research focused on laypeople's accuracy rates. We examined whether the CTB also has a positive effect on law enforcement personnel accuracy. In this study, 95 police officers judged 10 interviews, whereby half of the senders told the truth, and the other half lied about a mock undercover mission. Half of the interviews included only questioning about the event under investigation, whereas the other half also included questioning aimed at creating a CTB. Total and truth accuracy did not differ, but observers who watched interviews with a CTB obtained higher lie detection accuracy rates than those who watched interviews without the baseline questioning. Signal detection analyses showed that this effect could be attributed to a decreased response bias in the CTB condition.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Letizia Caso
- Department of Human and Social Sciences, University of Bergamo, Bergamo, Italy
| | - Nicola Palena
- Department of Human and Social Sciences, University of Bergamo, Bergamo, Italy
| | - Elga Carlessi
- Department of Human and Social Sciences, University of Bergamo, Bergamo, Italy
- Bergamo Local Polics, Bergamo, Italy
| | - Aldert Vrij
- Department of Psychology, University of Portsmouth, Portsmouth, UK
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Street CNH, Vadillo MA. Commentary: Can Ordinary People Detect Deception after All? Front Psychol 2017; 8:1789. [PMID: 29081758 PMCID: PMC5645821 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01789] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/09/2017] [Accepted: 09/26/2017] [Indexed: 11/27/2022] Open
Affiliation(s)
- Chris N. H. Street
- Department of Psychology, University of Huddersfield, Huddersfield, United Kingdom
- International Research Centre for Investigative Psychology, University of Huddersfield, Huddersfield, United Kingdom
| | - Miguel A. Vadillo
- Departamento de Psicología Básica, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, Madrid, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Street CNH, Kingstone A. Aligning Spinoza with Descartes: An informed Cartesian account of the truth bias. Br J Psychol 2016; 108:453-466. [PMID: 27511287 DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12210] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/23/2015] [Revised: 06/13/2016] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
There is a bias towards believing information is true rather than false. The Spinozan account claims there is an early, automatic bias towards believing. Only afterwards can people engage in an effortful re-evaluation and disbelieve the information. Supporting this account, there is a greater bias towards believing information is true when under cognitive load. However, developing on the Adaptive Lie Detector (ALIED) theory, the informed Cartesian can equally explain this data. The account claims the bias under load is not evidence of automatic belief; rather, people are undecided, but if forced to guess they can rely on context information to make an informed judgement. The account predicts, and we found, that if people can explicitly indicate their uncertainty, there should be no bias towards believing because they are no longer required to guess. Thus, we conclude that belief formation can be better explained by an informed Cartesian account - an attempt to make an informed judgment under uncertainty.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chris N H Street
- Department of Psychology, University of Huddersfield, UK.,International Research Centre for Investigative Psychology, University of Huddersfield, UK
| | - Alan Kingstone
- Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Sporer SL. Deception and Cognitive Load: Expanding Our Horizon with a Working Memory Model. Front Psychol 2016; 7:420. [PMID: 27092090 PMCID: PMC4823263 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00420] [Citation(s) in RCA: 47] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/09/2015] [Accepted: 03/09/2016] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Recently, studies on deception and its detection have increased dramatically. Many of these studies rely on the “cognitive load approach” as the sole explanatory principle to understand deception. These studies have been exclusively on lies about negative actions (usually lies of suspects of [mock] crimes). Instead, we need to re-focus more generally on the cognitive processes involved in generating both lies and truths, not just on manipulations of cognitive load. Using Baddeley’s (2000, 2007, 2012) working memory model, which integrates verbal and visual processes in working memory with retrieval from long-term memory and control of action, not only verbal content cues but also nonverbal, paraverbal, and linguistic cues can be investigated within a single framework. The proposed model considers long-term semantic, episodic and autobiographical memory and their connections with working memory and action. It also incorporates ironic processes of mental control (Wegner, 1994, 2009), the role of scripts and schemata and retrieval cues and retrieval processes. Specific predictions of the model are outlined and support from selective studies is presented. The model is applicable to different types of reports, particularly about lies and truths about complex events, and to different modes of production (oral, hand-written, typed). Predictions regarding several moderator variables and methods to investigate them are proposed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Siegfried L Sporer
- Department of Psychology and Sports Science, University of Giessen Giessen, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Street CNH, Bischof WF, Vadillo MA, Kingstone A. Inferring Others' Hidden Thoughts: Smart Guesses in a Low Diagnostic World. JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DECISION MAKING 2015. [DOI: 10.1002/bdm.1904] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022]
Affiliation(s)
| | - Walter F. Bischof
- University of Alberta; Edmonton Canada
- University of British Columbia; Vancouver Canada
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
16
|
Street CNH, Masip J. The source of the truth bias: Heuristic processing? Scand J Psychol 2015; 56:254-63. [DOI: 10.1111/sjop.12204] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/18/2014] [Accepted: 12/14/2014] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
|