1
|
Norris MN, McDermott CH, Noles NS. Listen to Your Mother: Children Use Hierarchical Social Roles to Guide Their Judgments about People. JOURNAL OF COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT 2023. [DOI: 10.1080/15248372.2023.2176854] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 03/01/2023]
|
2
|
Tong Y, Danovitch J, Wang F, Williams A, Li H. Unsafe to eat? How familiar cartoon characters affect children's learning about potentially harmful foods. Appetite 2021; 167:105649. [PMID: 34400223 DOI: 10.1016/j.appet.2021.105649] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/08/2021] [Revised: 07/05/2021] [Accepted: 08/11/2021] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
Young children learn about the properties of foods, such as taste and healthiness, from others. By using selective trust tasks in which a familiar cartoon character and an unfamiliar informant provided different testimony about food safety, this study examined how an informant's familiarity affected 4- to 6-year-old children's selective social learning about food safety. In Experiment 1, when judging the safety of foods from the familiar cartoon character and the unfamiliar character, children across all age groups showed a preference for asking the familiar character for information. For endorse questions, 4- and 5-year-olds did not consistently accept or reject either character's statements, while 6-year-olds endorsed the unfamiliar cartoon character's statements more often than the familiar character's statements. In Experiment 2, when the unfamiliar informant was a real adult instead of a fictional cartoon character, children sought out information from the familiar character more often than from the adult, and they did not differentially endorse statements by either informant. Moreover, children who had less advanced theory of mind skills and who viewed cartoon characters as more real were more likely to ask the cartoon character. These results suggest that although children prefer to obtain information from familiar characters, they accept information about food safety from multiple kinds of sources and their social-cognitive skills play a role in their decisions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yu Tong
- School of Psychology, Central China Normal University, Wuhan, Hubei, 430079, China
| | - Judith Danovitch
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Louisville, Louisville, KY, 40292, USA
| | - Fuxing Wang
- School of Psychology, Central China Normal University, Wuhan, Hubei, 430079, China.
| | - Allison Williams
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Louisville, Louisville, KY, 40292, USA
| | - Hui Li
- School of Education, Central China Normal University, Wuhan, Hubei, 430079, China.
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Lascaux A. Of Kids and Unicorns: How Rational Is Children's Trust in Testimonial Knowledge? Cogn Sci 2021; 44:e12819. [PMID: 32090379 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.12819] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/01/2018] [Revised: 01/24/2020] [Accepted: 01/24/2020] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
Abstract
When young children confront a vast array of adults' testimonial claims, they should decide which testimony to endorse. If they are unable to immediately verify the content of testimonial assertions, children adopt or reject their informants' statements on the basis of forming trust in the sources of testimony. This kind of trust needs to be based on some underlying reasons. The rational choice theory, which currently dominates the social, cognitive, and psychological sciences, posits that trust should be formed on a rational basis, as a result of probabilistic assessments and utility-maximizing calculations. In this paper, the predictions stemming from the rational choice approach to trust are systematically compared with the empirical evidence from the field of developmental psychology on how children establish their trust in testimonial statements. The results of this comparison demonstrate an obvious inadequacy of the rational choice explanation of the emergence and development of children's testimonial trust, regardless of which form of trust rationality-weighting, threshold, or ordering-is examined. As none of the three forms of rationality of children's trust in testimony squares with the empirical data, this paper introduces a new version of trust rationality, adaptively rational trust. It explores the compatibility of the concept of adaptively rational trust with the recent empirical findings in the area of developmental psychology and addresses some avenues for future research on the rationality of testimonial trust.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alexander Lascaux
- IBS, Russian Presidential Academy of National Economy and Public Affairs
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Who decides? Mothers’ and children’s beliefs about food disagreements. COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT 2020. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2020.100919] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
|
5
|
DeJesus JM, Venkatesh S. Show or tell: Children's learning about food from action vs verbal testimony. Pediatr Obes 2020; 15:e12719. [PMID: 32869955 DOI: 10.1111/ijpo.12719] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/31/2020] [Accepted: 08/07/2020] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Childhood obesity continues to be a critical health concern in the United States. Nonetheless, interventions that focus on delivering verbal lessons about food and health to children in preschool classrooms have had only modest effects. OBJECTIVES The present study examines the relative effectiveness of showing vs telling children about food to promote healthy eating, with a focus on unfamiliar foods and vegetables. METHODS Three- to six-year-old children (n = 71) were tested in a laboratory study in which they watched videos of two people eating apple-broccoli puree. One person took five bites of the food; the other said they liked the food. RESULTS Children did not differentiate between the food they saw someone eat and the food they heard someone talk about. Children's food intake was negatively associated with parent reports of children's eating behavior on the Food Fussiness subscale of the Child Eating Behavior Questionnaire. We found similar patterns in an analogous toy task. In an unfamiliar object task, children selected the action demonstration as the right way to use the object. CONCLUSIONS We find no evidence that action vs verbal testimony is more persuasive in guiding children's food choices, but action testimony may be persuasive in other domains. The associations between children's food intake and pickiness provide growing evidence of alignment between parent assessments of their children's typical eating behavior and children's food choices in laboratory studies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jasmine M DeJesus
- Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina Greensboro, Greensboro, North Carolina, USA
| | - Shruthi Venkatesh
- Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina Greensboro, Greensboro, North Carolina, USA
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
From foods to artifacts: Children’s evaluative and taxonomic categorization across multiple domains. COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT 2020. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2020.100894] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
|
7
|
Girgis H, Nguyen SP. Grown or made? Children’s determination of the origins of natural versus processed foods. COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT 2020. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2020.100887] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
|
8
|
|
9
|
Tong Y, Wang F, Danovitch J. The role of epistemic and social characteristics in children's selective trust: Three meta-analyses. Dev Sci 2019; 23:e12895. [PMID: 31433880 DOI: 10.1111/desc.12895] [Citation(s) in RCA: 42] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/29/2019] [Revised: 06/29/2019] [Accepted: 07/29/2019] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
Abstract
Over the last 15 years, researchers have been increasingly interested in understanding the nature and development of children's selective trust. Three meta-analyses were conducted on a total of 51 unique studies (88 experiments) to provide a quantitative overview of 3- to 6-year-old children's selective trust in an informant based on the informant's epistemic or social characteristics, and to examine the relation between age and children's selective trust decisions. The first and second meta-analyses found that children displayed medium-to-large pooled effects in favor of trusting the informant who was knowledgeable or the informant with positive social characteristics. Moderator analyses revealed that 4-year-olds were more likely to endorse knowledgeable informants than 3-year-olds. The third meta-analysis examined cases where two informants simultaneously differed in their epistemic and social characteristics. The results revealed that 3-year-old children did not selectively endorse informants who were more knowledgeable but had negative social characteristics over informants who were less knowledgeable but had positive social characteristics. However, 4- to 6-year-olds consistently prioritized epistemic cues over social characteristics when deciding who to trust. Together, these meta-analyses suggest that epistemic and social characteristics are both valuable to children when they evaluate the reliability of informants. Moreover, with age, children place greater value on epistemic characteristics when deciding whether to endorse an informant's testimony. Implications for the development of epistemic trust and the design of studies of children's selective trust are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yu Tong
- School of Psychology, Central China Normal University, Wuhan, China
| | - Fuxing Wang
- School of Psychology, Central China Normal University, Wuhan, China
| | - Judith Danovitch
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Louisville, Louisville, KY, USA
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Williams AJ, Danovitch JH. What does Mickey Mouse know about food? Children's trust in favorite characters versus experts. J Exp Child Psychol 2019; 187:104647. [PMID: 31325648 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2019.05.014] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/06/2018] [Revised: 05/29/2019] [Accepted: 05/31/2019] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
Abstract
Children receive information from multiple sources, including people who are more or less knowledgeable and more or less familiar. In some cases, children also encounter messages from fictional characters who vary across these dimensions. Two studies investigated children's trust in a familiar animal character versus a human expert when hearing conflicting information about items related to or unrelated to the expert's knowledge. In Study 1, 3-, 4-, and 5-year-olds (N = 60) heard conflicting labels for unfamiliar fruits and tools from a familiar character and an unfamiliar fruit expert. They then identified which informant was correct and from whom they would seek out new information. Overall, children endorsed the fictional character's statements over the fruit expert's statements. Younger children preferred to seek out new information from the character, whereas 5-year-olds preferred the expert. In Study 2, 3-, 4-, and 5-year-olds (N = 60) heard similar conflicting objective statements about fruits and tools and heard conflicting subjective statements about unknown foods. The 4- and 5-year-olds trusted the fruit expert's objective statements about fruit and did not consistently endorse either informant's objective statements about tools, but they endorsed either informant when hearing subjective statements about unknown foods. Children also endorsed positive statements (e.g., that the food tastes good) regardless of the source. Taken together, these results suggest that when children decide who to trust, they consider both familiarity and relevant expertise and they weigh each factor differently depending on what kind of judgment is being made.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Allison J Williams
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Louisville, Louisville, KY 40208, USA.
| | - Judith H Danovitch
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Louisville, Louisville, KY 40208, USA
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Wertz AE, Wynn K. Can I eat that too? 18-month-olds generalize social information about edibility to similar looking plants. Appetite 2019; 138:127-135. [DOI: 10.1016/j.appet.2019.02.013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/05/2018] [Revised: 02/17/2019] [Accepted: 02/21/2019] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
|
12
|
Dial LA, Musher-Eizenman DR. Healthy? Tasty? Children’s evaluative categorization of novel foods. COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT 2019. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2019.02.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/15/2022]
|
13
|
Girgis H, Nguyen SP. Shape or substance? Children’s strategy when labeling a food and its healthfulness. COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT 2018. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2018.08.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
|
14
|
DeJesus JM, Gelman SA, Viechnicki GB, Appugliese DP, Miller AL, Rosenblum KL, Lumeng JC. An investigation of maternal food intake and maternal food talk as predictors of child food intake. Appetite 2018; 127:356-363. [PMID: 29758271 PMCID: PMC6768399 DOI: 10.1016/j.appet.2018.04.018] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/22/2017] [Revised: 04/16/2018] [Accepted: 04/23/2018] [Indexed: 01/13/2023]
Abstract
Though parental modeling is thought to play a critical role in promoting children's healthy eating, little research has examined maternal food intake and maternal food talk as independent predictors of children's food intake. The present study examines maternal food talk during a structured eating protocol, in which mothers and their children had the opportunity to eat a series of familiar and unfamiliar vegetables and desserts. Several aspects of maternal talk during the protocol were coded, including overall food talk, directives, pronoun use, and questions. This study analyzed the predictors of maternal food talk and whether maternal food talk and maternal food intake predicted children's food intake during the protocol. Higher maternal body mass index (BMI) predicted lower amounts of food talk, pronoun use, and questions. Higher child BMI z-scores predicted more first person pronouns and more wh-questions within maternal food talk. Mothers of older children used fewer directives, fewer second person pronouns, and fewer yes/no questions. However, maternal food talk (overall and specific types of food talk) did not predict children's food intake. Instead, the most robust predictor of children's food intake during this protocol was the amount of food that mothers ate while sitting with their children. These findings emphasize the importance of modeling healthy eating through action and have implications for designing interventions to provide parents with more effective tools to promote their children's healthy eating.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jasmine M DeJesus
- Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, United States; Department of Pediatrics and Communicable Diseases, School of Medicine, University of Michigan, United States.
| | - Susan A Gelman
- Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, United States
| | | | | | - Alison L Miller
- Department of Health Behavior and Health Education, School of Public Health, University of Michigan, United States
| | | | - Julie C Lumeng
- Department of Pediatrics and Communicable Diseases, School of Medicine, University of Michigan, United States; Department of Nutritional Sciences, School of Public Health, University of Michigan, United States
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Willingness to revise own testimony: 3- and 4-year-olds' selective trust in unexpected testimony from accurate and inaccurate informants. J Exp Child Psychol 2018; 173:1-15. [PMID: 29631087 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2018.03.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/01/2017] [Revised: 03/11/2018] [Accepted: 03/17/2018] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
Abstract
Prior work has shown that young children trust single accurate and inaccurate individuals to a similar extent in their endorsement of novel information. However, it remains unknown to what extent children trust a credible or noncredible individual when given information that is pitted against their own beliefs. The current study examined whether children, when given unexpected testimony that contradicted their initial beliefs but was not completely unbelievable, would selectively revise their beliefs depending on the informant's past history of accuracy. The participants (3- and 4-year-olds; N = 100) were familiarized with an informant who labeled a series of common objects either accurately or inaccurately. Following that, all children saw a picture of an ambiguous hybrid artifact that consisted of features of two typical common artifacts and were asked to identify the hybrid object with their own label. Subsequently, children watched the previously accurate or inaccurate informant give the same hybrid object a different but plausible label. Children expressed a greater tendency to override their initial judgments and endorse the unexpected testimony from a previously accurate informant than from someone who had consistently made naming errors. The findings provide novel understandings of the circumstances under which 3- and 4-year-old preschoolers may or may not rely on the informant's prior reliability in their selective learning.
Collapse
|
16
|
Toyama N. Development of the selection of trusted informants in the domain of illness. INFANT AND CHILD DEVELOPMENT 2017. [DOI: 10.1002/icd.2039] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/10/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Noriko Toyama
- School of Human Sciences; Waseda University; Tokorozawa Japan
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Railton P. Moral Learning: Conceptual foundations and normative relevance. Cognition 2016; 167:172-190. [PMID: 27601269 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.08.015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 30] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/15/2016] [Revised: 08/06/2016] [Accepted: 08/25/2016] [Indexed: 01/01/2023]
Abstract
What is distinctive about a bringing a learning perspective to moral psychology? Part of the answer lies in the remarkable transformations that have taken place in learning theory over the past two decades, which have revealed how powerful experience-based learning can be in the acquisition of abstract causal and evaluative representations, including generative models capable of attuning perception, cognition, affect, and action to the physical and social environment. When conjoined with developments in neuroscience, these advances in learning theory permit a rethinking of fundamental questions about the acquisition of moral understanding and its role in the guidance of behavior. For example, recent research indicates that spatial learning and navigation involve the formation of non-perspectival as well as ego-centric models of the physical environment, and that spatial representations are combined with learned information about risk and reward to guide choice and potentiate further learning. Research on infants provides evidence that they form non-perspectival expected-value representations of agents and actions as well, which help them to navigate the human environment. Such representations can be formed by highly-general mental processes such as causal and empathic simulation, and thus afford a foundation for spontaneous moral learning and action that requires no innate moral faculty and can exhibit substantial autonomy with respect to community norms. If moral learning is indeed integral with the acquisition and updating of casual and evaluative models, this affords a new way of understanding well-known but seemingly puzzling patterns in intuitive moral judgment-including the notorious "trolley problems."
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Peter Railton
- Department of Philosophy, University of Michigan, 2215 Angell Hall, 435 South State Street, Ann Arbor, MI 48109-1003, United States.
| |
Collapse
|