1
|
Basch S, Wang SH. Causal learning by infants and young children: From computational theories to language practices. WILEY INTERDISCIPLINARY REVIEWS. COGNITIVE SCIENCE 2024; 15:e1678. [PMID: 38567762 DOI: 10.1002/wcs.1678] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/25/2023] [Revised: 03/05/2024] [Accepted: 03/14/2024] [Indexed: 07/06/2024]
Abstract
Causal reasoning-the ability to reason about causal relations between events-is fundamental to understanding how the world works. This paper reviews two prominent theories on early causal learning and offers possibilities for theory bridging. Both theories grow out of computational modeling and have significant areas of overlap while differing in several respects. Explanation-Based Learning (EBL) focuses on young infants' learning about causal concepts of physical objects and events, whereas Bayesian models have been used to describe causal reasoning beyond infancy across various concept domains. Connecting the two models offers a more integrated approach to clarifying the developmental processes in causal reasoning from early infancy through later childhood. We further suggest that everyday language practices offer a promising space for theory bridging. We provide a review of selective work on caregiver-child conversations, in particular, on the use of scaffolding language including causal talk and pedagogical questions. Linking the research on language practices to the two cognitive theories, we point out directions for further research to integrate EBL and Bayesian models and clarify how causal learning unfolds in real life. This article is categorized under: Psychology > Learning Cognitive Biology > Cognitive Development.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Samantha Basch
- Psychology Department, University of California, Santa Cruz, California, USA
| | - Su-Hua Wang
- Psychology Department, University of California, Santa Cruz, California, USA
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Ohnishi A, Kosaka S, Hama Y, Saito K, Terada T. A Curiosity Estimation in Storytelling with Picture Books for Children Using Wearable Sensors. SENSORS (BASEL, SWITZERLAND) 2024; 24:4043. [PMID: 39000821 PMCID: PMC11244374 DOI: 10.3390/s24134043] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/21/2024] [Revised: 06/17/2024] [Accepted: 06/19/2024] [Indexed: 07/16/2024]
Abstract
Storytelling is one of the most important learning activities for children since reading aloud from a picture book stimulates children's curiosity, emotional development, and imagination. For effective education, the procedures for storytelling activities need to be improved according to the children's level of curiosity. However, young children are not able to complete questionnaires, making it difficult to analyze their level of interest. This paper proposes a method to estimate children's curiosity in picture book reading activities at five levels by recognizing children's behavior using acceleration and angular velocity sensors placed on their heads. We investigated the relationship between children's behaviors and their levels of curiosity, listed all observed behaviors, and clarified the behavior for estimating curiosity. Furthermore, we conducted experiments using motion sensors to estimate these behaviors and confirmed that the accuracy of estimating curiosity from sensor data is approximately 72%.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ayumi Ohnishi
- Graduate School of Engineering, Kobe University, 1-1 Rokkodaicho, Nada, Kobe 657-8501, Hyogo, Japan;
| | - Sayo Kosaka
- Graduete School of Frontier Sciences, The University of Tokyo, 5-1-5, Kashiwanoha, Kashiwa 277-8561, Chiba, Japan
| | - Yasukazu Hama
- Center for Spatial Information Science, The University of Tokyo, 5-1-5, Kashiwanoha, Kashiwa 277-8568, Chiba, Japan
| | - Kaoru Saito
- Graduete School of Frontier Sciences, The University of Tokyo, 5-1-5, Kashiwanoha, Kashiwa 277-8561, Chiba, Japan
| | - Tsutomu Terada
- Graduate School of Engineering, Kobe University, 1-1 Rokkodaicho, Nada, Kobe 657-8501, Hyogo, Japan;
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Sacheli LM, Roberti E, Turati C. Encoding interactive scripts at 10 months of age. J Exp Child Psychol 2023; 227:105588. [PMID: 36512919 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2022.105588] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/14/2022] [Revised: 09/30/2022] [Accepted: 11/02/2022] [Indexed: 12/14/2022]
Abstract
Understanding action-reaction associations that give origin to interactive scripts (e.g., give-and-take interactions) is essential for appreciating social exchanges. However, studies on infants' action understanding have mainly investigated the case of actions performed by individual agents. Moreover, although extensive literature has explored infants' comprehension of action-effect relationships in object functioning, no study has addressed whether it also plays a role when observing social interactions, an issue we addressed here. In a first study, 10-month-old infants observed short videos of dyadic exchanges. We investigated whether they were able to link specific human gestures directed toward another person to specific vocal reactions in the receiver. We used a double-habituation paradigm in which infants were sequentially habituated to two specific action-reaction associations. In the test phase, infants watched one of the two habituated (Familiar) videos, a video with a reversed action-reaction association (Violation), and a Novel video. Results showed that the infants looked longer at both the Novel and Violation test trials than at the Familiar test trials. In a control study, we show that these results could not be accounted for by associative learning; indeed, learning of the action-reaction association did not occur when the vocalization was not produced by the receiver but only contingent on the agent's action. Thus, we show that 10-month-old infants can encode specific social action-effect relationships during the observation of dyadic interactions and that the interactivity of the social context may be critical to shaping young infants' understanding of others' behaviors.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lucia Maria Sacheli
- Department of Psychology and Milan Center for Neuroscience (NeuroMI), University of Milano-Bicocca, 20126 Milano, Italy.
| | - Elisa Roberti
- Department of Psychology and Milan Center for Neuroscience (NeuroMI), University of Milano-Bicocca, 20126 Milano, Italy
| | - Chiara Turati
- Department of Psychology and Milan Center for Neuroscience (NeuroMI), University of Milano-Bicocca, 20126 Milano, Italy.
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Shinya Y, Ishibashi M. Observing effortful adults enhances not perseverative but sustained attention in infants aged 12 months. COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT 2022. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2022.101255] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/03/2022]
|
5
|
Gualtieri S, Attisano E, Denison S. Young children’s use of probabilistic reliability and base-rates in decision-making. PLoS One 2022; 17:e0268790. [PMID: 35613117 PMCID: PMC9132303 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0268790] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/17/2021] [Accepted: 05/08/2022] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Children are skilled reasoners who readily use causal, reliability, and base-rate (i.e., prior probability) information in their decisions. Though these abilities are typically studied in isolation, children often must consider multiple pieces of information to make an informed decision. Four experiments (N = 320) explored the development of children’s ability to use reliability and base-rate information when making decisions about draw outcomes. Experiment 1 examined the age at which children can first compare and choose between probabilistically reliable machines. Three- and 4-year-old children saw machines that were probabilistically reliable at obtaining objects while sampling from uniform distributions (i.e., all target or non-target objects). Although 4-year-old children correctly used reliability in their decisions, 3-year-olds did not. In Experiment 2a, 4- to 6-year-olds were presented with the same probabilistically reliable machines, although they sampled from a mixture of target and non-target items. Here, children tended to choose the machine with the better proportion of targets, regardless of reliability. This was replicated in Experiment 2b. In Experiment 3, children were presented with one perfectly reliable machine and one probabilistically unreliable machine. Here, children continued to mostly choose the machine with the better proportion of targets. These results raise questions about base-rate overuse early in development and highlight the need for additional work on children’s ability to use multiple pieces of information in decision-making.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Samantha Gualtieri
- Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
- * E-mail:
| | - Elizabeth Attisano
- Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada
| | - Stephanie Denison
- Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Peng Y, Lu H, Johnson SP. Infant perception of causal motion produced by humans and inanimate objects. Infant Behav Dev 2021; 64:101615. [PMID: 34333261 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2021.101615] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/09/2021] [Revised: 06/28/2021] [Accepted: 07/19/2021] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
Abstract
Both the movements of people and inanimate objects are intimately bound up with physical causality. Furthermore, in contrast to object movements, causal relationships between limb movements controlled by humans and their body displacements uniquely reflect agency and goal-directed actions in support of social causality. To investigate the development of sensitivity to causal movements, we examined the looking behavior of infants between 9 and 18 months of age when viewing movements of humans and objects. We also investigated whether individual differences in gender and gross motor functions may impact the development of the visual preferences for causal movements. In Experiment 1, infants were presented with walking stimuli showing either normal body translation or a "moonwalk" that reversed the horizontal motion of body translations. In Experiment 2, infants were presented with unperformable actions beyond infants' gross motor functions (i.e., long jump) either with or without ecologically valid body displacement. In Experiment 3, infants were presented with rolling movements of inanimate objects that either complied with or violated physical causality. We found that female infants showed longer looking times to normal walking stimuli than to moonwalk stimuli, but did not differ in their looking time to movements of inanimate objects and unperformable actions. In contrast, male infants did not show sensitivity to causal movement for either category. Additionally, female infants looked longer at social stimuli of human actions than male infants. Under the tested circumstances, our findings indicate that female infants have developed a sensitivity to causal consistency between limb movements and body translations of biological motion, only for actions with previous visual and motor exposures, and demonstrate a preference toward social information.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yujia Peng
- University of California, Los Angeles, United States.
| | - Hongjing Lu
- University of California, Los Angeles, United States
| | | |
Collapse
|
7
|
Design and validation of a smart garment to measure positioning practices of parents with young infants. Infant Behav Dev 2021; 62:101530. [PMID: 33548894 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2021.101530] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/16/2019] [Revised: 01/19/2021] [Accepted: 01/25/2021] [Indexed: 12/26/2022]
Abstract
The aim of this cross-sectional study was to evaluate the feasibility, construct validity, and reliability of a smart garment to characterize parent-child positioning practices in infants less than six months old. The smart garment (Get Around Garment, GG) was developed through feedback from seven infants and their parents. The final system was then tested with sixteen infants (M = 3.1 ± 1.1 months) assessed in their homes during one visit that consisted of a: 1) Structured Play Assessment (2.5 min): infant was placed in each of five positions (prone, supine, reclined/inclined, and upright) for 30 s, 2) Free Play Assessment (40-60 min): parents were asked to engage in typical daily activities, and 3) second Structured Play Assessment. Infants' body position was both coded from video and identified from sensor data using a custom program. Feasibility was measured by data from a Daily Wearing Log and Garment Perception Questionnaire. Validity was evaluated by comparing the coding and sensor data. Reliability was measured by comparing the sensor data between the two Structured Play Assessments. The GG was considered feasible for use. The smart wearable system showed high levels of accuracy for classifying body position secondby- second and when comparing cumulative duration across time. Reliability of the smart garment was excellent. Young infants spent more time in supine and supported upright positions relative to prone, reclined, or inclined positions. The results suggest that accelerometers can be integrated into garments in a manner that is feasible to provide accurate and consistent data about positioning practices of parents with young infants. Monitoring early positioning practices is important because these practices impact future motor and cognitive developmental trajectories.
Collapse
|
8
|
When correlation equals causation: A behavioral and computational account of second-order correlation learning in children. J Exp Child Psychol 2020; 202:105008. [PMID: 33091823 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2020.105008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/25/2020] [Revised: 09/11/2020] [Accepted: 09/12/2020] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
We examined 2- and 3-year-old children's ability to use second-order correlation learning-in which a learned correlation between two pairs of features (e.g., A and B, A and C) is generalized to the noncontiguous features (i.e., B and C)-to make causal inferences. Previous findings showed that 20- and 26-month-old children can use second-order correlation learning to learn about static and dynamic features in category and noncategory contexts. The current behavioral study and computational model extend these findings to show that 2- and 3-year-olds can detect the second-order correlation between an object's surface feature and its capacity to activate a novel machine, but only if the children had encoded the first-order correlations on which the second-order correlation was based. These results have implications for children's developing information-processing capacities on their ability to use second-order correlations to infer causal relations in the world.
Collapse
|
9
|
Bazhydai M, Silverstein P, Parise E, Westermann G. Two-year-old children preferentially transmit simple actions but not pedagogically demonstrated actions. Dev Sci 2020; 23:e12941. [PMID: 31981382 DOI: 10.1111/desc.12941] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/26/2019] [Revised: 10/25/2019] [Accepted: 01/14/2020] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
Children are sensitive to both social and non-social aspects of the learning environment. Among social cues, pedagogical communication has been shown to not only play a role in children's learning, but also in their own active transmission of knowledge. Vredenburgh, Kushnir and Casasola, Developmental Science, 2015, 18, 645 showed that 2-year-olds are more likely to demonstrate an action to a naive adult after learning it in a pedagogical than in a non-pedagogical context. This finding was interpreted as evidence that pedagogically transmitted information has a special status as culturally relevant. Here we test the limits of this claim by setting it in contrast with an explanation in which the relevance of information is the outcome of multiple interacting social (e.g., pedagogical demonstration) and non-social properties (e.g., action complexity). To test these competing hypotheses, we varied both pedagogical cues and action complexity in an information transmission paradigm with 2-year-old children. In Experiment 1, children preferentially transmitted simple non-pedagogically demonstrated actions over pedagogically demonstrated more complex actions. In Experiment 2, when both actions were matched for complexity, we found no evidence of preferential transmission of pedagogically demonstrated actions. We discuss possible reasons for the discrepancy between our results and previous literature showing an effect of pedagogical cues on cultural transmission, and conclude that our results are compatible with the view that pedagogical and other cues interact, but incompatible with the theory of a privileged role for pedagogical cues.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Priya Silverstein
- Psychology Department, Lancaster University, Lancaster, UK.,School of Psychology, University of Surrey, Guildford, UK
| | - Eugenio Parise
- Psychology Department, Lancaster University, Lancaster, UK
| | | |
Collapse
|
10
|
Parish-Morris J, Pallathra AA, Ferguson E, Maddox BB, Pomykacz A, Perez LS, Bateman L, Pandey J, Schultz RT, Brodkin ES. Adaptation to different communicative contexts: an eye tracking study of autistic adults. J Neurodev Disord 2019; 11:5. [PMID: 30981277 PMCID: PMC6461820 DOI: 10.1186/s11689-019-9265-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/06/2018] [Accepted: 03/28/2019] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
Background Learning through social observation (i.e., watching other people interact) lays the foundation for later social skills and social cognition. However, social situations are often complex, and humans are only capable of attending to one aspect of a scene at a time. How do people choose where to allocate their visual resources when viewing complex social scenarios? For typically developing (TD) individuals, faces are often given priority. Depending upon context, however, it may be more useful to attend to other aspects of the environment, such as hands, tools, or background objects. Previous studies reported reduced face looking in individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD), but modulation of visual attention in response to contextual differences (e.g., according to social richness, or the presence/absence of communicative behaviors between two people) has only briefly been explored. In this study, we used eye-tracking technology to test the extent to which ASD adults and TD adults use social context to guide their gaze behavior. Methods Fifty-five adults participated (28 with ASD). The location and duration of participants’ gaze were recorded while they watched a series of naturalistic social videos. Half of the videos depicted two people engaging in non-verbal communication (rich social scenes) while playing with toys. The other half depicted two people playing with toys separately, not interacting with each other (lean social scenes). Results ASD and TD adults both increased their attention to faces in communicative contexts (rich social scenes) as compared to non-communicative contexts (lean social scenes). However, TD adults increased their attention to faces significantly more when watching two people communicate than did ASD adults, who increased their attention to a lesser degree. Further analysis revealed that ASD adults persisted in looking at hands and toys, even when observing two people communicate in a rich social scene. Conclusions Diminished gaze to faces when observing two people communicating may lead to fewer opportunities for social learning and subsequent reductions in social knowledge. Naturalistic measures of contextual modulation could help identify areas of need for individuals learning about the social world and could become treatment targets to improve everyday social learning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Julia Parish-Morris
- Center for Autism Research, Children's Hospital of Philadelphia, 5th Floor 2716 South Street, Philadelphia, PA, 19146, USA. .,Department of Psychiatry, Perelman School of Medicine at the University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, 19104, USA.
| | - Ashley A Pallathra
- Department of Psychology, Catholic University of America, Washington, DC, 20064, USA
| | - Emily Ferguson
- Center for Autism Research, Children's Hospital of Philadelphia, 5th Floor 2716 South Street, Philadelphia, PA, 19146, USA
| | - Brenna B Maddox
- Center for Autism Research, Children's Hospital of Philadelphia, 5th Floor 2716 South Street, Philadelphia, PA, 19146, USA
| | - Alison Pomykacz
- Center for Autism Research, Children's Hospital of Philadelphia, 5th Floor 2716 South Street, Philadelphia, PA, 19146, USA
| | - Leat S Perez
- Department of Psychiatry, Perelman School of Medicine at the University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, 19104, USA
| | - Leila Bateman
- Center for Autism Research, Children's Hospital of Philadelphia, 5th Floor 2716 South Street, Philadelphia, PA, 19146, USA
| | - Juhi Pandey
- Center for Autism Research, Children's Hospital of Philadelphia, 5th Floor 2716 South Street, Philadelphia, PA, 19146, USA
| | - Robert T Schultz
- Center for Autism Research, Children's Hospital of Philadelphia, 5th Floor 2716 South Street, Philadelphia, PA, 19146, USA.,Department of Pediatrics, Perelman School of Medicine at the University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, 19104, USA
| | - Edward S Brodkin
- Department of Psychiatry, Perelman School of Medicine at the University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, 19104, USA
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
|
12
|
Cerchiaro Ceballos EL, Puche-Navarro R. Funcionamientos Inferenciales en Niños Caminadores: un Acercamiento al Microdesarrollo en una Tarea de Resolución de Problemas. REVISTA COLOMBIANA DE PSICOLOGÍA 2018. [DOI: 10.15446/rcp.v27n2.66054] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/09/2022] Open
Abstract
Se exponen los resultados de un estudio sobre la emergencia de la capacidad de niños caminadores para resolver problemas, centrado en la manera como niños de 25 meses de edad acceden a la comprensión de un problema de compuertas, cuya solución les exige funcionamientos inferenciales distintos. Se aplicó una metodología microgenética, en la cual se utiliza una situación de resolución de problemas (SRP), en tres ensayos de una misma sesión de observación. El análisis se dirige a las variaciones interindividuales, a partir de las cuales se identifican tendencias en los desempeños de los niños. Los resultados ponen en evidencia la capacidad resolutoria de niños caminadores, manifestada en acciones sistemáticas, organizadas, automotivadas y en el uso de procesos inferenciales complejos.
Collapse
|
13
|
Wang Z, Williamson RA, Meltzoff AN. Preschool physics: Using the invisible property of weight in causal reasoning tasks. PLoS One 2018; 13:e0192054. [PMID: 29561840 PMCID: PMC5862406 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0192054] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/28/2017] [Accepted: 01/16/2018] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Causal reasoning is an important aspect of scientific thinking. Even young human children can use causal reasoning to explain observations, make predictions, and design actions to bring about specific outcomes in the physical world. Weight is an interesting type of cause because it is an invisible property. Here, we tested preschool children with causal problem-solving tasks that assessed their understanding of weight. In an experimental setting, 2- to 5-year-old children completed three different tasks in which they had to use weight to produce physical effects-an object displacement task, a balance-scale task, and a tower-building task. The results showed that the children's understanding of how to use object weight to produce specific object-to-object causal outcomes improved as a function of age, with 4- and 5-year-olds showing above-chance performance on all three tasks. The younger children's performance was more variable. The pattern of results provides theoretical insights into which aspects of weight processing are particularly difficult for preschool children and why they find it difficult.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Zhidan Wang
- School of Educational Science, Jiangsu Normal University, Xuzhou, Jiangsu, The People's Republic of China
- * E-mail:
| | - Rebecca A. Williamson
- Department of Psychology, Georgia State University, Atlanta, Georgia, United States of America
| | - Andrew N. Meltzoff
- Institute for Learning & Brain Sciences, University of Washington, Seattle, Washington, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Bolenz F, Reiter AMF, Eppinger B. Developmental Changes in Learning: Computational Mechanisms and Social Influences. Front Psychol 2017; 8:2048. [PMID: 29250006 PMCID: PMC5715389 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02048] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/15/2017] [Accepted: 11/09/2017] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Our ability to learn from the outcomes of our actions and to adapt our decisions accordingly changes over the course of the human lifespan. In recent years, there has been an increasing interest in using computational models to understand developmental changes in learning and decision-making. Moreover, extensions of these models are currently applied to study socio-emotional influences on learning in different age groups, a topic that is of great relevance for applications in education and health psychology. In this article, we aim to provide an introduction to basic ideas underlying computational models of reinforcement learning and focus on parameters and model variants that might be of interest to developmental scientists. We then highlight recent attempts to use reinforcement learning models to study the influence of social information on learning across development. The aim of this review is to illustrate how computational models can be applied in developmental science, what they can add to our understanding of developmental mechanisms and how they can be used to bridge the gap between psychological and neurobiological theories of development.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Florian Bolenz
- Chair of Lifespan Developmental Neuroscience, Department of Psychology, Technische Universität Dresden, Dresden, Germany
| | - Andrea M F Reiter
- Chair of Lifespan Developmental Neuroscience, Department of Psychology, Technische Universität Dresden, Dresden, Germany.,Department of Neurology, Max-Planck-Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany
| | - Ben Eppinger
- Chair of Lifespan Developmental Neuroscience, Department of Psychology, Technische Universität Dresden, Dresden, Germany.,Department of Psychology, Concordia University, Montreal, QC, Canada.,PERFORM Centre, Concordia University, Montreal, QC, Canada
| |
Collapse
|