1
|
Zuniga-Montanez C, Krott A. Late Talkers can generalise trained labels by object shape similarities, but not unfamiliar labels. JOURNAL OF CHILD LANGUAGE 2024:1-24. [PMID: 39359168 DOI: 10.1017/s0305000924000163] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/04/2024]
Abstract
Late talkers (LTs) exhibit delayed vocabulary development, which might stem from a lack of a typical word learning strategy to generalise object labels by shape, called the 'shape bias'. We investigated whether LTs can acquire a shape bias and whether this accelerates vocabulary learning. Fourteen LTs were randomly allocated to either a shape training group (Mage = 2.76 years, 6 males), which was taught that objects similar in shape have the same name, or a control group (Mage = 2.61 years, 4 males), which was taught real words without any focus on object shape. After seven training sessions, children in the shape training group generalised trained labels by shape (d = 1.28), but not unfamiliar labels. Children in the control group extended all labels randomly. Training did not affect expressive vocabulary.
Collapse
|
2
|
Lam JHY, Leachman MA, Pratt AS. A systematic review of factors that impact reading comprehension in children with developmental language disorders. RESEARCH IN DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES 2024; 149:104731. [PMID: 38663332 DOI: 10.1016/j.ridd.2024.104731] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/17/2023] [Revised: 03/25/2024] [Accepted: 04/07/2024] [Indexed: 05/21/2024]
Abstract
Children with developmental language disorder (DLD) have a high rate of co-occurring reading difficulties. The current study aims to (i) examine which factors within the Active View of Reading (AVR; Duke & Cartwright, 2021) apply to individuals with DLD and (ii) investigate other possible factors that relate to reading comprehension ability in individuals with DLD, outside the components in the AVR. Electronic database search and journal hand-search yielded 5058 studies published before March 2022 related to reading comprehension in children with DLD. 4802 articles were excluded during abstract screening, yielding 256 studies eligible for full-text review. Following full-text review, 44 studies were included and further coded for demographics, language of assessment, description of reported disabilities, behavioral assessment, and reading comprehension assessment. While the results aligned with the AVR model, three additional factors were identified as significantly relating to reading comprehension abilities in children with DLD: expressive language (oral and written), question types of reading assessment, and language disorder history. Specifically, expressive language was positively associated with reading comprehension ability, while resolved DLD showed higher reading comprehension abilities than persistent DLD. Furthermore, children with DLD may face additional difficulties in comprehending inference-based questions. This study provides factors for researchers, educators, and clinical professionals to consider when evaluating the reading comprehension of individuals with DLD. Future research should further explore the relative importance of factors of the AVR to reading comprehension outcomes throughout development.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Amy S Pratt
- Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, OH, USA
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Gósy M, Bunta F, Pregitzer M. Speech processing performance of Hungarian-speaking twins and singletons. CLINICAL LINGUISTICS & PHONETICS 2023; 37:979-995. [PMID: 36052433 DOI: 10.1080/02699206.2022.2111274] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/28/2020] [Revised: 05/30/2022] [Accepted: 08/01/2022] [Indexed: 06/15/2023]
Abstract
Studying speech processing in twins versus their singleton peers provides opportunities to study both genetic and environmental effects on how children acquire these aspects of their speech and - by extension - their phonological systems. Our study focused on speech processing in typically developing Hungarian-speaking twins and their singleton peers between 5 and 9 years of age. Participants included 384 monolingual Hungarian-speaking children (192 twins, and 192 singletons). Data from four tasks - repetition of synthesised monosyllables, nonsense words, well-formed noisy sentences, and well-formed phonologically complex sentences - were analysed. There was a main effect for birth status, and singletons outperformed their twin peers on the majority of the speech processing tasks. Age and task also had effects on the performance of the participants, and there was a three-way task by age by twin versus singleton status indicating that the speech processing performance of twins versus singletons is interdependent with the type of task and age. Our results also indicate that monolingual Hungarian-speaking twins may be at higher risk for developmental speech delays relative to their singleton peers.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mária Gósy
- Department of Phonetics, Linguistics Institute ELKH and ELTE University, Budapest, Hungary
| | - Ferenc Bunta
- Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, University of Houston, Houston, Texas, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
4
|
Reilly S, McKean C. Creating the conditions for robust early language development for all-Part 1: Evidence-informed child language surveillance in the early years. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF LANGUAGE & COMMUNICATION DISORDERS 2023; 58:2222-2241. [PMID: 37432035 DOI: 10.1111/1460-6984.12929] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/11/2023] [Accepted: 06/14/2023] [Indexed: 07/12/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND The emergence of language in the early years is a major developmental accomplishment that underpins learning, enables social interaction and, later, is an indicator of well-being. Learning language is an effortless process for most, but can be challenging for others. There is a need to act early. First, because there are several social, environmental and family factors known to influence how language develops during the critical early years. Second, there is a robust association between a child's socio-economic circumstances and language outcomes. Put simply, children living in less advantaged circumstance have poorer language outcomes, which are apparent very early and persist across the lifespan. Third, children with demonstrated weaknesses in language learning in early childhood have poorer educational, employment, mental health and quality-of-life outcomes across the lifespan. Acting early to counter these impacts is important; however, there are several well-documented challenges in accurately identifying in the early years children who are at later risk of developmental language disorder (DLD) and to deliver prevention and intervention programmes to scale. This is critical because many services do not currently reach those who need them most; as many as 50% of children in need may not be receiving support. AIM To determine whether an improved surveillance system, based on best evidence, could be developed for the early years. METHODS & PROCEDURES We summarised findings from longitudinal, population or community studies that: (1) adopted bioecological models, (2) repeatedly measured language (including the early years) and (3) adopted similar methodologies, to identify factors that influence language outcomes. MAIN CONTRIBUTION The evidence confirmed that language development is not always stable but is characterized by distinct trajectories and each has distinguishing social, environmental features. Children in the change or fluctuating groups tend to live in less advantageous circumstances that may not always support and enable language development. Risk factors tend to cluster and accumulate across the early years and beyond, thereby markedly increasing the likelihood of poorer language outcomes later in life. CONCLUSIONS & IMPLICATIONS In this the first of two papers, designed to be read together, we integrate research on the social determinants of child language and propose they be embedded into surveillance models. This has the potential to reach more children and those living in disadvantaged circumstances. In the accompanying paper we combine this information with evidence-informed early prevention/intervention approaches and propose the design and implementation of an early language public health framework. WHAT THIS PAPER ADDS What is already known on the subject There are several well-documented challenges in accurately identifying in the early years children who are at later risk of DLD and reaching those most in need of language support. What this study adds to existing knowledge A combination of child, family and environmental determinants, collectively and cumulatively, play out over time and dramatically increase the risk of later language problems, in particular those children living in disadvantaged circumstances. We propose an improved surveillance system that incorporates these determinants be developed and that this be part of a whole of system approach to child language in the early years. What are the potential or actual clinical implications of this work? Clinicians intuitively act to prioritize children with multiple features or risks; however, they can only do so for those who present or are identified to be at risk. Given many children with language problems are not being reached by many early language services, it is reasonable to ask if this knowledge can be integrated to improve reach. Or is a different surveillance model required?
Collapse
|
5
|
Surakka S, Vehkavuori SM, Saaristo-Helin K, Stolt S. What kind of information do early parental report instruments provide on language ability at 3;6 when used at 2;0? A longitudinal comparison study. Front Psychol 2023; 14:1206949. [PMID: 37546451 PMCID: PMC10400438 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1206949] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/16/2023] [Accepted: 06/16/2023] [Indexed: 08/08/2023] Open
Abstract
Introduction Various parental report instruments are available for assessing children's language skills at the end of the second year. However, comparison studies on their usability are lacking, and it is also open to question what kind of information the instruments provide when used in a parallel manner. This longitudinal study investigated which of the available three parental report instruments, when used at 2;0 (year;month), provides the most representative information on language development at 3;6. In addition, since most of the parental report instruments available focus specifically on expressive language, the role of receptive language ability was also investigated when analyzing the explanatory value of parental report instruments. Methods The participants were 68 typically developing children. At 2;0, language skills were measured using the following measures: the Infant-Toddler Checklist of the Communication and Symbolic Behavior Scales Developmental Profile (ITC), the Short Form and Long Form versions of the Finnish Communicative Development Inventories (FinCDI-SF, FinCDI-LF), and the Reynell Developmental Language Scales III (RDLS). The outcome measures were receptive/expressive/ general language ability at 3;6 measured using RDLS. Results The results of parental report instruments were significantly and positively associated with language ability at 3;6. The correlation between the combined value of ITC and FinCDI-SF and later language ability was stronger than correlations for each measure separately. The regression models with the results of parental report instruments as predictors explained 18-22% (p < 0.00) of the variability in the total RDLS score. However, when receptive language ability at 2;0 was included in the models as a predictor, R2 increased considerably (46-48%, p < 0.00). Discussion The results adduce the usability of parental report measures along with the importance of measuring receptive language skills at 2 years of age. In summary, this study provides important insights into the clinical evaluation of early language ability.
Collapse
|
6
|
Capelli E, Silibello G, Ajmone PF, Altamore E, Lalatta F, Vizziello PG, Costantino MA, Zampini L. Language Development in Sex Chromosome Trisomies: Developmental Profiles at 2 and 4 Years of Age, and Predictive Measures. Dev Neurorehabil 2022; 25:337-348. [PMID: 34983283 DOI: 10.1080/17518423.2021.2020925] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/03/2022]
Abstract
PURPOSE Describing language development in children with sex chromosome trisomies (SCT) and testing the predictive value of early language measures on later outcomes. METHOD Thirteen children with SCT were followed longitudinally. Their developmental profile was assessed, with particular attention to language, at 2 and 4 years. The predictive value of direct (spontaneous speech analysis) and indirect (communicative development inventory) language measures at 2 on performances at 4 was tested. RESULTS Language performances at both ages were lower than non-verbal development. At 2, more than 50% of the group produced less than 50 words. At 4, impaired performances were observed in speech sound development and expressive morpho-syntax. Direct measures of Pre-syntactic development predicted later global language outcomes and Sentence Repetition. The number of consonants used at 2 was significantly related to Nonword Repetition at 4. CONCLUSIONS The study highlights the importance of early detection and careful follow-up for children with SCT.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Gaia Silibello
- Foundation Irccs Ca' Granda Ospedale Maggiore Policlinico, Milan, Italy
| | | | - Elena Altamore
- Foundation Irccs Ca' Granda Ospedale Maggiore Policlinico, Milan, Italy
| | - Faustina Lalatta
- Foundation Irccs Ca' Granda Ospedale Maggiore Policlinico, Milan, Italy
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
7
|
Price KM, Wigg KG, Misener VL, Clarke A, Yeung N, Blokland K, Wilkinson M, Kerr EN, Guger SL, Lovett MW, Barr CL. Language Difficulties in School-Age Children With Developmental Dyslexia. JOURNAL OF LEARNING DISABILITIES 2022; 55:200-212. [PMID: 33890525 PMCID: PMC8996296 DOI: 10.1177/00222194211006207] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/28/2023]
Abstract
Developmental dyslexia (DD) is a common reading disability, affecting 5% to 11% of children in North America. Children classified as having DD often have a history of early language delay (ELD) or language impairments. Nevertheless, studies have reported conflicting results as to the association between DD-ELD and the extent of current language difficulties in children with DD. To examine these relationships, we queried the parents of school-age children with reading difficulties on their child's early and current language ability. Siblings were also examined. Children were directly assessed using quantitative tests of language and reading skills. To compare this study with the literature, we divided the sample (N = 674) into three groups: DD, intermediate readers (IR), and skilled readers (SR). We found a significant association between DD and ELD, with parents of children in the DD/IR groups reporting their children put words together later than the SR group. We also found a significant association between DD and language difficulties, with children with low reading skills having low expressive/receptive language abilities. Finally, we identified early language predicted current language, which predicted reading skills. These data contribute to research indicating that children with DD experience language difficulties, suggesting early recognition may help identify reading problems.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kaitlyn M. Price
- University Health Network, Toronto,
Ontario, Canada
- The Hospital for Sick Children,
Toronto, Ontario, Canada
- University of Toronto, Ontario,
Canada
| | | | | | - Antoine Clarke
- The Hospital for Sick Children,
Toronto, Ontario, Canada
| | - Natalie Yeung
- The Hospital for Sick Children,
Toronto, Ontario, Canada
| | | | | | - Elizabeth N. Kerr
- The Hospital for Sick Children,
Toronto, Ontario, Canada
- University of Toronto, Ontario,
Canada
| | | | - Maureen W. Lovett
- The Hospital for Sick Children,
Toronto, Ontario, Canada
- University of Toronto, Ontario,
Canada
| | - Cathy L. Barr
- University Health Network, Toronto,
Ontario, Canada
- The Hospital for Sick Children,
Toronto, Ontario, Canada
- University of Toronto, Ontario,
Canada
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Holzinger D, Weber C, Fellinger J. Validity and feasibility of a predictive language screening tool in 2-year-old children in primary pediatric care. Front Pediatr 2022; 10:865457. [PMID: 36147818 PMCID: PMC9485630 DOI: 10.3389/fped.2022.865457] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/29/2022] [Accepted: 08/12/2022] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
OBJECTIVE To assess the predictive validity and feasibility of the newly developed language screening tool, SPES-2 (Sprachentwicklungsscreening), for 2-year-old children in pediatric primary care. METHODS A prospective cohort study recruited 2,044 non-selected German-speaking children undergoing a regular well-baby check-up at the age of 2 years. Thirty primary care pediatricians spread over urban and rural areas screened the children using a short parent-reported questionnaire and direct assessment of word comprehension. To validate the screening tool, language skills were assessed using a standardized language screening tool in the complete sample 1 year later. Data of a random sample of 621 children were analyzed. Feasibility of the screening tool was evaluated using questionnaires completed by the participating pediatricians. RESULTS The new screening tool, SPES-2, demonstrated good diagnostic accuracy with AUC (Area under the Roc Curve) of 0.885, a sensitivity of 0.74, and specificity of 0.86, using a parent-reported questionnaire (expressive vocabulary, two-word combinations, parental concerns) as stage 1, followed by a stage 2 direct assessment of word comprehension by the pediatrician. The second stage was restricted to children who failed the parental screening. The screening identified children with high, moderate, and low risk of significant language deficits (SLD) at the age of 3 years, permitting tailored follow-up assessment and parental counseling. Practicality and acceptability of the screening were mostly rated as high. Pediatricians regarded the availability of follow-up diagnostic services and parent guidance as most important for a general implementation of the new instrument. CONCLUSION The language screening tool, SPES-2, was valid for the identification of significant language deficits 1 year later, and considered as feasible within primary pediatric care.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Daniel Holzinger
- Institute of Neurology of Senses and Language, Hospital of St. John of God, Linz, Austria.,Research Institute for Developmental Medicine, Johannes Kepler University Linz, Linz, Austria.,Institute of Linguistics, University of Graz, Graz, Austria
| | - Christoph Weber
- Research Institute for Developmental Medicine, Johannes Kepler University Linz, Linz, Austria.,Department for Inclusive Education, University of Education Upper Austria, Linz, Austria
| | - Johannes Fellinger
- Institute of Neurology of Senses and Language, Hospital of St. John of God, Linz, Austria.,Research Institute for Developmental Medicine, Johannes Kepler University Linz, Linz, Austria.,Division of Social Psychiatry, University Clinic for Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Medical University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Eadie P, Levickis P, McKean C, Westrupp E, Bavin EL, Ware RS, Gerner B, Reilly S. Developing Preschool Language Surveillance Models - Cumulative and Clustering Patterns of Early Life Factors in the Early Language in Victoria Study Cohort. Front Pediatr 2022; 10:826817. [PMID: 35186809 PMCID: PMC8854765 DOI: 10.3389/fped.2022.826817] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/01/2021] [Accepted: 01/10/2022] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND Screening and surveillance of development are integral to ensuring effective early identification and intervention strategies for children with vulnerabilities. However, not all developmental skills have reliable screening processes, such as early language ability. METHOD We describe how a set of early life factors used in a large, prospective community cohort from Australia are associated with language abilities across the preschool years, and determine if either an accumulation of risk factors or a clustering of risk factors provide a feasible approach to surveillance of language development in preschool children. RESULTS There were 1,208 children with a 7-year language outcome. The accumulation of early life factors increased the likelihood of children having low language skills at 7-years. Over a third of children with typical language skills (36.6%) had ≤ two risks and half of the children with low language (50%) had six or more risks. As the number of factors increases the risk of having low language at 7-years increases, for example, children with six or more risks had 17 times greater risk, compared to those with ≤ two risks. Data collected from 1,910 children at 8- to 12-months were used in the latent class modeling. Four profile classes (or groups) were identified. The largest group was developmentally enabled with a supportive home learning environment (56.2%, n = 1,073). The second group was vulnerable, both developmentally and in their home learning environment (31.2%, n = 596); the third group was socially disadvantaged with a vulnerable home learning environment (7.4%, n = 142); the final group featured maternal mental health problems and vulnerable child socio-emotional adjustment (5.2%, n = 99). Compared to developmentally enabled children, the risk of low language at 7-years was greater for children in the three other groups. CONCLUSION The cumulative and cluster risk analyses demonstrate the potential to use developmental surveillance to identify children within the first years of life who are at risk of language difficulties. Importantly, parent-child interaction and the home learning environment emerged as a consistent cluster. We recommend they be adopted as the common focus for early intervention and universal language promotion programs.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Patricia Eadie
- Melbourne Graduate School of Education, University of Melbourne, Melbourne, VIC, Australia
| | - Penny Levickis
- Melbourne Graduate School of Education, University of Melbourne, Melbourne, VIC, Australia.,Genetics, Murdoch Children's Research Institute, Melbourne, VIC, Australia
| | - Cristina McKean
- School of Education, Communication and Language Sciences, Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne, United Kingdom
| | - Elizabeth Westrupp
- Deakin University, Centre for Social and Early Emotional Development, School of Psychology, Geelong, VIC, Australia.,Judith Lumley Centre, La Trobe University, Melbourne, VIC, Australia
| | - Edith L Bavin
- Genetics, Murdoch Children's Research Institute, Melbourne, VIC, Australia.,School of Psychology and Public Health La Trobe University, Melbourne, VIC, Australia
| | - Robert S Ware
- Menzies Health Institute Queensland, Griffith University, Gold Coast, QLD, Australia
| | - Bibi Gerner
- Melbourne Graduate School of Education, University of Melbourne, Melbourne, VIC, Australia.,Genetics, Murdoch Children's Research Institute, Melbourne, VIC, Australia
| | - Sheena Reilly
- Menzies Health Institute Queensland, Griffith University, Gold Coast, QLD, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Caglar-Ryeng Ø, Eklund K, Nergård-Nilssen T. School-entry language outcomes in late talkers with and without a family risk of dyslexia. DYSLEXIA (CHICHESTER, ENGLAND) 2021; 27:29-49. [PMID: 32181543 DOI: 10.1002/dys.1656] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/30/2018] [Revised: 02/18/2020] [Accepted: 03/02/2020] [Indexed: 06/10/2023]
Abstract
Children with familial risk (FR) of dyslexia and children with early language delay are known to be at risk for later language and literacy difficulties. However, research addressing long-term outcomes in children with both risk factors is scarce. This study tracked FR and No-FR children identified as late talkers at 2 years of age and reports development from 4;6 through 6 years. We examined the possible effects of FR-status and late talking (LT) status, respectively, on language skills at school entry, and whether FR-status moderated the associations between 4;6-year and 6-year language scores. Results indicated an effect of LT status on language at both ages, while FR status affected language skills at 6 years only. The interaction between LT and FR statuses was not significant, implying that LT status affected language skills independently of the child's FR status. A proportion of late talkers developed typical language at 6 years of age, while some FR children with typical vocabulary skills in toddlerhood had emerging developmental language disorder by school entry. FR status had a moderating effect on the association between expressive grammar at ages 4;6 and 6 years. Possible explanations for the effect of FR status on language skills are discussed. We highlight limitations in the study size and suggest how these preliminary findings can inform future research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ømur Caglar-Ryeng
- Department of Education, UiT-The Arctic University of Norway, Tromsø, Norway
| | - Kenneth Eklund
- Department of Psychology, University of Jyväskylä, Jyväskylä, Finland
| | | |
Collapse
|
11
|
Duncan LG, Gollek C, Potter DD. eLIPS: Development and Validation of an Observational Tool for Examining Early Language in Play Settings. Front Psychol 2020; 11:1813. [PMID: 32849070 PMCID: PMC7412886 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01813] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/30/2020] [Accepted: 07/01/2020] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Intervention in the early years can help to mitigate the risks that early language and communication difficulties pose for later learning and well-being. Critical to this is the capacity of early years educators to evaluate language development accurately in the classroom in order to target individual support effectively. This article reports on the development and testing of the Early Language in Play Settings (eLIPS) tool, an observational measure of child language. An action research model was used in the design of the tool with the result that the methodology adopted was compatible with an early years child-centered approach. Observations of children in play settings were used to gather information about early language through subscales measuring social communication, receptive and expressive language. A series of preliminary trials with 3- to 5-year-olds, established that the eLIPS measures have concurrent validity with scores from a standardized language assessment, the Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals-Preschool 2 UK . Investigation of internal consistency showed reliability for use by researchers and early years educators together with inter-rater reliability across these groups. It was concluded that eLIPS has potential as a tool to assist early years educators in understanding individual patterns of language acquisition in a play-based environment and for framing team discussions about approaches to early language support.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lynne G Duncan
- Psychology, School of Social Sciences, University of Dundee, Dundee, United Kingdom
| | - Conny Gollek
- School of Education and Social Sciences, University of the West of Scotland, Paisley, United Kingdom
| | - Douglas D Potter
- Psychology, School of Social Sciences, University of Dundee, Dundee, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
van den Bedem NP, Dockrell JE, van Alphen PM, Rieffe C. Emotional Competence Mediates the Relationship between Communication Problems and Reactive Externalizing Problems in Children with and without Developmental Language Disorder: A Longitudinal Study. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF ENVIRONMENTAL RESEARCH AND PUBLIC HEALTH 2020; 17:ijerph17166008. [PMID: 32824870 PMCID: PMC7459595 DOI: 10.3390/ijerph17166008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/29/2020] [Revised: 07/17/2020] [Accepted: 07/23/2020] [Indexed: 01/09/2023]
Abstract
Language problems are a risk factor for externalizing problems, but the developmental path remains unclear. Emotional competence may mediate the relationship, especially when externalizing problems are reactive in nature, such as in Oppositional Deviant Disorder (ODD) and reactive aggression. We examined the development of reactive and proactive externalizing problems in children with (n = 98) and without (n = 156) Developmental Language Disorder (DLD; age: 8-16 years) over 18 months. Relationships with communicative risk factors (structural, pragmatic and emotion communication) and the mediating role of emotional competence (emotion recognition and anger dysregulation) were examined. Multi-level analyses showed that increasing emotion recognition and decreasing anger dysregulation were longitudinally related to decreasing ODD symptoms in both groups, whereas anger dysregulation was related to more reactive aggression in children with DLD alone. Pragmatic and emotion communication problems were related to more reactive externalizing problems, but these relationships were mediated by emotional competence, suggesting that problems in emotional competence explain the communication problems of children with DLD. Therefore, in addition to interventions for communication skills, there is a need to address the emotional competence of children with DLD, as this decreases the risk for reactive externalizing problems.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Neeltje P. van den Bedem
- Developmental Psychology, Leiden University, Wassenaarseweg 52, 2333AK Leiden, The Netherlands;
- Correspondence:
| | - Julie E. Dockrell
- Department of Psychology and Human Development, University College London, 25 Woburn Square, London WC1H 0AA, UK;
| | - Petra M. van Alphen
- Royal Dutch Kentalis, Theerestraat 42, 5271 GD Sint-Michielsgestel, The Netherlands;
| | - Carolien Rieffe
- Developmental Psychology, Leiden University, Wassenaarseweg 52, 2333AK Leiden, The Netherlands;
- Department of Psychology and Human Development, University College London, 25 Woburn Square, London WC1H 0AA, UK;
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Del Tufo SN, Earle FS, Cutting LE. The impact of expressive language development and the left inferior longitudinal fasciculus on listening and reading comprehension. J Neurodev Disord 2019; 11:37. [PMID: 31838999 PMCID: PMC6912995 DOI: 10.1186/s11689-019-9296-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/05/2019] [Accepted: 11/11/2019] [Indexed: 12/25/2022] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND During the first 3-years of life, as the brain undergoes dramatic growth, children begin to develop speech and language. Hallmarks of this progression are seen when children reach developmental milestones, forming the foundation of language. Expressive language milestones, such as the production of a child's first word, are delayed in 5-8% of children. While for some children delays in reaching these milestones are harbingers of developmental disorders, for others expressive language delays appear to resolve. Regardless of whether or not early language skills appear resolved, difficulty with later comprehension is a likely outcome. Whether this heightened risk for poor comprehension differs based on text features, individual characteristics, or receipt of intervention remains unknown. Moreover, this relationship between expressive language development and comprehension is not yet linked to neurobiology, though the inferior longitudinal fasciculus (ILF) is a potential neurobiological correlate. Therefore, we investigated the impact of, and interactions between, expressive language development, early intervention, and the ILF on comprehension. METHODS Longitudinal recurrent survival analyses predicted the risk of answering a comprehension question incorrectly. Predictors of comprehension included expressive language development, passage features, participant characteristics, fractional anisotropy, receipt of early intervention, and later diagnosis of speech or language disorders. RESULTS Children with later expressive language milestones had poorer comprehension. When comprehension text features were examined, children with later milestones had poorer listening and reading comprehension, and poorer narrative and expository comprehension. The left ILF acted as a neurodevelopmental correlate, one that moderated the relationship between expressive language milestones and comprehension. Specifically, the left ILF exacerbated the relationship for those who did not receive early intervention and buffered the relationship for those who received intervention services. Early intervention decreased the risk of poor comprehension by 39% for children later diagnosed with a speech or language disorder. CONCLUSIONS Early intervention should be provided for children with delayed expressive language milestones, particularly those who are at risk for speech or language disorders. The ILF plays a critical role in the relationship between expressive language development and comprehension, which may be that of a protective factor for children with the most severe early issues with speech and language.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Stephanie N Del Tufo
- Peabody College of Education and Human Development, Vanderbilt University, 416C One Magnolia Circle, Box 228, Nashville, TN, 37203, USA.,Vanderbilt Brain Institute, Vanderbilt University School of Medicine, 6133 Medical Research Building III, 465 21st Avenue South, Nashville, TN, 37232, USA.,Vanderbilt Kennedy Center, Vanderbilt University, 110 Magnolia Circle, Nashville, TN, 37203, USA.,College of Education and Human Development, University of Delaware, 106 Alison Hall West, Newark, DE, 19716, USA
| | - F Sayako Earle
- Communication Sciences and Disorders, University of Delaware, 100 Discovery Boulevard, Newark, DE, 19713, USA
| | - Laurie E Cutting
- Peabody College of Education and Human Development, Vanderbilt University, 416C One Magnolia Circle, Box 228, Nashville, TN, 37203, USA. .,Vanderbilt Brain Institute, Vanderbilt University School of Medicine, 6133 Medical Research Building III, 465 21st Avenue South, Nashville, TN, 37232, USA. .,Vanderbilt Kennedy Center, Vanderbilt University, 110 Magnolia Circle, Nashville, TN, 37203, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Armstrong R, Symons M, Scott JG, Arnott WL, Copland DA, McMahon KL, Whitehouse AJO. Predicting Language Difficulties in Middle Childhood From Early Developmental Milestones: A Comparison of Traditional Regression and Machine Learning Techniques. JOURNAL OF SPEECH, LANGUAGE, AND HEARING RESEARCH : JSLHR 2018; 61:1926-1944. [PMID: 30073346 DOI: 10.1044/2018_jslhr-l-17-0210] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/01/2017] [Accepted: 01/15/2018] [Indexed: 06/08/2023]
Abstract
PURPOSE The current study aimed to compare traditional logistic regression models with machine learning algorithms to investigate the predictive ability of (a) communication performance at 3 years old on language outcomes at 10 years old and (b) broader developmental skills (motor, social, and adaptive) at 3 years old on language outcomes at 10 years old. METHOD Participants (N = 1,322) were drawn from the Western Australian Pregnancy Cohort (Raine) Study (Straker et al., 2017). A general developmental screener, the Infant Monitoring Questionnaire (Squires, Bricker, & Potter, 1990), was completed by caregivers at the 3-year follow-up. Language ability at 10 years old was assessed using the Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals-Third Edition (Semel, Wiig, & Secord, 1995). Logistic regression models and interpretable machine learning algorithms were used to assess predictive abilities of early developmental milestones for later language outcomes. RESULTS Overall, the findings showed that prediction accuracies were comparable between logistic regression and machine learning models using communication-only performance as well as performance on communication and broader developmental domains to predict language performance at 10 years old. Decision trees are incorporated to visually present these findings but must be interpreted with caution because of the poor accuracy of the models overall. CONCLUSIONS The current study provides preliminary evidence that machine learning algorithms provide equivalent predictive accuracy to traditional methods. Furthermore, the inclusion of broader developmental skills did not improve predictive capability. Assessment of language at more than 1 time point is necessary to ensure children whose language delays emerge later are identified and supported. SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.6879719.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rebecca Armstrong
- School of Health and Rehabilitation Sciences, University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia
- Centre for Clinical Research, University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia
- Centre for Advanced Imaging, University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia
| | - Martyn Symons
- Telethon Kids Institute, University of Western Australia, Perth
- National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC) Fetal Alcohol Spectrum Disorder (FASD) Research Australia, Centre of Research Excellence, Perth
| | - James G Scott
- Centre for Clinical Research, University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia
- Metro North Mental Health, Royal Brisbane and Women's Hospital, Australia
| | - Wendy L Arnott
- School of Health and Rehabilitation Sciences, University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia
- Hear and Say, Brisbane, Australia
| | - David A Copland
- School of Health and Rehabilitation Sciences, University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia
- Centre for Clinical Research, University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia
| | - Katie L McMahon
- Centre for Advanced Imaging, University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia
| | | |
Collapse
|
15
|
Psyridou M, Eklund K, Poikkeus AM, Torppa M. Reading outcomes of children with delayed early vocabulary: A follow-up from age 2-16. RESEARCH IN DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES 2018; 78:114-124. [PMID: 29805034 DOI: 10.1016/j.ridd.2018.05.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/14/2017] [Revised: 02/24/2018] [Accepted: 05/10/2018] [Indexed: 06/08/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Delays in expressive vocabulary have been associated with lower outcomes in reading. AIM The aim is to conduct a long-term follow-up study to investigate if early expressive vocabulary delay (late talking) predicts reading development in participants age 16 and under. We examine further if the prediction is different in the presence of family risk for dyslexia (FR) and early receptive vocabulary delay. METHODS Expressive and receptive vocabulary skills were assessed at the age of 2-2.5 years, and reading skills in Grades 2, 3, 8 and 9 (age 8-16). The longitudinal sample consisted of 200 Finnish-speaking children, of which 108 had FR for dyslexia and 92 came from families without reading difficulties. We compared the reading development of five subgroups: 1) FR and no vocabulary delay; 2) FR and late talkers, 3) FR, late talkers and co-existing receptive vocabulary delay; 4) no FR and late talkers; and 5) no FR and no vocabulary delay. RESULTS The group with FR and expressive and receptive vocabulary delay had difficulties in reading comprehension, but not in reading fluency. The late talkers without receptive vocabulary difficulties tended to become typical readers. CONCLUSIONS AND IMPLICATIONS Delays in early vocabulary can lead to a reading comprehension deficit, with the specification that expressive vocabulary deficit alone can alleviate in time, whereas the combined deficit is a stronger risk marker.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Maria Psyridou
- Department of Teacher Education, Faculty of Education and Psychology, University of Jyväskylä, P.O. Box 35, Ruusupuisto, Alvar Aallon katu 9, 40014-FIN, Finland.
| | - Kenneth Eklund
- Department of Psychology, Faculty of Education and Psychology, University of Jyväskylä, P.O. Box 35, Kärki, Mattilanniemi 6, 40014-FIN, Finland.
| | - Anna-Maija Poikkeus
- Department of Teacher Education, Faculty of Education and Psychology, University of Jyväskylä, P.O. Box 35, Ruusupuisto, Alvar Aallon katu 9, 40014-FIN, Finland.
| | - Minna Torppa
- Department of Teacher Education, Faculty of Education and Psychology, University of Jyväskylä, P.O. Box 35, Ruusupuisto, Alvar Aallon katu 9, 40014-FIN, Finland.
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Abstract
From a speech-language pathology perspective, there is a gap in interprofessional education/practice (IPE/IPP) that leads to a wait-and-see approach with late talkers (LT). In line with the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association's Strategic Pathway to Excellence, this article attempts to bridge the gap, reexamining the panoptic view that most LT "catch up" to their peers. The LT who persist with language disorder should not be overlooked. Late talking can impact socialization and school readiness, and can place some toddlers at risk for life-long disability. Each state's early intervention program has an established IPP infrastructure. Parent-implemented intervention addresses risks and maximizes protective factors.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nina Capone Singleton
- Department of Speech-Language Pathology, School of Health and Medical Sciences, Seton Hall University, 400 South Orange Avenue, South Orange, NJ 07079, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Rudolph JM. Case History Risk Factors for Specific Language Impairment: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. AMERICAN JOURNAL OF SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY 2017; 26:991-1010. [PMID: 28672377 DOI: 10.1044/2016_ajslp-15-0181] [Citation(s) in RCA: 41] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/18/2015] [Accepted: 12/13/2016] [Indexed: 06/07/2023]
Abstract
PURPOSE Research suggests that the best approach to early identification of children with specific language impairment (SLI) should include assessment of risk factors. However, previous attempts to develop a list for this purpose have been unsuccessful. In this study, systematic review and meta-analytic procedures were used to determine whether any case history factors can be used to identify toddlers at risk of developing SLI. METHOD Epidemiological studies that examined the association between risk factors and SLI were identified. Results across studies were aggregated to determine more precisely the strength of association between each risk factor and the development of SLI. The clinical significance of these factors was established via comparison to late talker status. RESULTS Eleven risk factors were found to be statistically significant predictors of SLI. Among these, maternal education level, 5-min Apgar score, birth order, and biological sex met criteria for clinical significance. CONCLUSIONS At least 4 case history factors are as predictive as late talker status in the context of early identification of toddlers at risk for SLI. The findings of this review highlight the importance of taking a child's genetic and environmental context into consideration when deciding whether further evaluation and early intervention services are warranted. SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIALS https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.5150122.
Collapse
|
18
|
Armstrong R, Scott JG, Whitehouse AJO, Copland DA, Mcmahon KL, Arnott W. Late talkers and later language outcomes: Predicting the different language trajectories. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY 2017; 19:237-250. [PMID: 28440674 DOI: 10.1080/17549507.2017.1296191] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/14/2016] [Accepted: 02/11/2017] [Indexed: 06/07/2023]
Abstract
PURPOSE The aim of the current study was to investigate the risk factors present at 2 years for children who showed language difficulties that persisted from 2 to 10 years and difficulties that emerged later, at 10 years. METHOD Participants (n = 783) were drawn from the Raine Study in Western Australia. Patterns of change from 2 to 10 years were identified based on child performance on the Language Development Survey and the Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals, respectively. Logistic regression analyses were conducted to test whether parental, family and child characteristics present at 2 years predicted poorer language outcomes at age 10. RESULT Across the 8 year period, 5.6% of the children displayed consistently low language skills, 5.9% improved skills and 23.2% deteriorated skills. Compared to children with consistently typical skills, the deteriorated group was more likely to have mothers who smoked during pregnancy, fathers with incomplete secondary education, low family income, poor early literacy environment and be male. Children showing consistently low language skills were more likely to have mothers who smoked during pregnancy than late talkers whose early delays improved. CONCLUSION Results provide evidence of some modifiable risk factors at 2 years which are associated with language outcome.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rebecca Armstrong
- a School of Health and Rehabilitation Sciences, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia
- b Centre for Clinical Research, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia
- c Centre for Advanced Imaging, The University of Queensland , Brisbane , Australia
| | - James G Scott
- b Centre for Clinical Research, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia
- d Metro North Mental Health , The Royal Brisbane and Women's Hospital , Brisbane , Australia
| | - Andrew J O Whitehouse
- e Telethon Kids Institute, University of Western Australia , Perth , Australia , and
| | - David A Copland
- a School of Health and Rehabilitation Sciences, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia
- b Centre for Clinical Research, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia
| | - Katie L Mcmahon
- c Centre for Advanced Imaging, The University of Queensland , Brisbane , Australia
| | - Wendy Arnott
- a School of Health and Rehabilitation Sciences, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia
- f Hear and Say Centre , Brisbane , Australia
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Hammer CS, Morgan P, Farkas G, Hillemeier M, Bitetti D, Maczuga S. Late Talkers: A Population-Based Study of Risk Factors and School Readiness Consequences. JOURNAL OF SPEECH, LANGUAGE, AND HEARING RESEARCH : JSLHR 2017; 60:607-626. [PMID: 28257586 PMCID: PMC5962923 DOI: 10.1044/2016_jslhr-l-15-0417] [Citation(s) in RCA: 39] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/03/2015] [Revised: 07/30/2016] [Accepted: 08/08/2016] [Indexed: 05/19/2023]
Abstract
PURPOSE This study was designed to (a) identify sociodemographic, pregnancy and birth, family health, and parenting and child care risk factors for being a late talker at 24 months of age; (b) determine whether late talkers continue to have low vocabulary at 48 months; and (c) investigate whether being a late talker plays a unique role in children's school readiness at 60 months. METHOD We analyzed data from the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, a population-based sample of 9,600 children. Data were gathered when the children were 9, 24, 48, and 60 months old. RESULTS The risk of being a late talker at 24 months was significantly associated with being a boy, lower socioeconomic status, being a nonsingleton, older maternal age at birth, moderately low birth weight, lower quality parenting, receipt of day care for less than 10 hr/week, and attention problems. Being a late talker increased children's risk of having low vocabulary at 48 months and low school readiness at 60 months. Family socioeconomic status had the largest and most profound effect on children's school readiness. CONCLUSIONS Limited vocabulary knowledge at 24 and 48 months is uniquely predictive of later school readiness. Young children with low vocabularies require additional supports prior to school entry.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Paul Morgan
- The Pennsylvania State University, University Park
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
20
|
Ladányi E, Lukács Á. Lexical conflict resolution in children with specific language impairment. JOURNAL OF COMMUNICATION DISORDERS 2016; 61:119-130. [PMID: 27135369 DOI: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2016.04.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/26/2015] [Revised: 04/06/2016] [Accepted: 04/06/2016] [Indexed: 06/05/2023]
Abstract
The aim of our study is to examine the effect of conflict on naming latencies in children with specific language impairment (SLI) and typically developing (TD) children and to explore whether deficits in conflict resolution contribute to lexical problems in SLI. In light of previous results showing difficulties with inhibitory functions in SLI, we expected higher semantic conflict effect in the SLI than in the TD group. To investigate this question 13 children with SLI and 13 age- and gender-matched TD children performed a picture naming task in which the level of conflict was manipulated and naming latencies were measured. Children took longer to name pictures in high conflict conditions than in low conflict conditions. This effect was equally present in the SLI and TD groups. Our results suggest that word production is more effortful for children when conflict resolution is required but children with SLI manage competing lexical representations as efficiently as TD children. This result contradicts studies, which found difficulties with inhibitory functions and is in line with findings of intact inhibitory abilities in children with SLI. Further studies should rule out the possibility that in SLI lower level of conflict resulting from weaker lexical representations masks impairments in inhibition, and investigate the effect of linguistic conflict in other areas.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Enikő Ladányi
- Paris Descartes University, Laboratoire Psychologie de la Perception, 45 Rue des Saints-Peres, 75006 Paris, France; BME Department of Cognitive Science, Budapest University of Technology and Economics, Egry József u. 1, H-1111 Budapest, Hungary.
| | - Ágnes Lukács
- BME Department of Cognitive Science, Budapest University of Technology and Economics, Egry József u. 1, H-1111 Budapest, Hungary.
| |
Collapse
|
21
|
Korpilahti P, Kaljonen A, Jansson-Verkasalo E. Population-Based Screening for Language Delay: Let’s Talk STEPS Study. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2016. [DOI: 10.4236/psych.2016.72023] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
|
22
|
Duff FJ, Reen G, Plunkett K, Nation K. Do infant vocabulary skills predict school-age language and literacy outcomes? J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2015; 56:848-56. [PMID: 25557322 PMCID: PMC4674965 DOI: 10.1111/jcpp.12378] [Citation(s) in RCA: 125] [Impact Index Per Article: 13.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 12/02/2014] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Strong associations between infant vocabulary and school-age language and literacy skills would have important practical and theoretical implications: Preschool assessment of vocabulary skills could be used to identify children at risk of reading and language difficulties, and vocabulary could be viewed as a cognitive foundation for reading. However, evidence to date suggests predictive ability from infant vocabulary to later language and literacy is low. This study provides an investigation into, and interpretation of, the magnitude of such infant to school-age relationships. METHODS Three hundred British infants whose vocabularies were assessed by parent report in the 2nd year of life (between 16 and 24 months) were followed up on average 5 years later (ages ranged from 4 to 9 years), when their vocabulary, phonological and reading skills were measured. RESULTS Structural equation modelling of age-regressed scores was used to assess the strength of longitudinal relationships. Infant vocabulary (a latent factor of receptive and expressive vocabulary) was a statistically significant predictor of later vocabulary, phonological awareness, reading accuracy and reading comprehension (accounting for between 4% and 18% of variance). Family risk for language or literacy difficulties explained additional variance in reading (approximately 10%) but not language outcomes. CONCLUSIONS Significant longitudinal relationships between preliteracy vocabulary knowledge and subsequent reading support the theory that vocabulary is a cognitive foundation of both reading accuracy and reading comprehension. Importantly however, the stability of vocabulary skills from infancy to later childhood is too low to be sufficiently predictive of language outcomes at an individual level - a finding that fits well with the observation that the majority of 'late talkers' resolve their early language difficulties. For reading outcomes, prediction of future difficulties is likely to be improved when considering family history of language/literacy difficulties alongside infant vocabulary levels.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Fiona J. Duff
- Department of Experimental Psychology University of Oxford Oxford UK
| | - Gurpreet Reen
- Department of Experimental Psychology University of Oxford Oxford UK
| | - Kim Plunkett
- Department of Experimental Psychology University of Oxford Oxford UK
| | - Kate Nation
- Department of Experimental Psychology University of Oxford Oxford UK
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Executive functions among Egyptian children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and reading disabilities. MIDDLE EAST CURRENT PSYCHIATRY 2015. [DOI: 10.1097/01.xme.0000461749.64397.af] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/26/2022] Open
|
24
|
Graduate student research on individual differences. LEARNING AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES 2013. [DOI: 10.1016/j.lindif.2013.07.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/21/2022]
|