1
|
Sharp N, Hepworth LR. Treating Visual Inattention in Acute Stroke Survivors Using a Therapy Scanning Wall: A Proof-of-Concept Study. Br Ir Orthopt J 2023; 19:71-77. [PMID: 37577068 PMCID: PMC10417920 DOI: 10.22599/bioj.311] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/20/2023] [Accepted: 07/14/2023] [Indexed: 08/15/2023] Open
Abstract
Background Visual inattention is common following right hemisphere stroke, with up to 80% of patients being affected. Visual inattention following stroke is linked to poorer outcomes. There is no clear evidence for how visual inattention should be treated in the hospital inpatient setting. Objective To explore the practical implications and possible benefits of using a visual scanning wall in a stroke rehabilitation unit as an assessment and treatment tool for visual inattention. Methods This proof-of-concept study recruited stroke survivors with visual inattention. Participants used the scanning wall for scanning training five days a week for two weeks. Assessments using the scanning wall and modified Albert's test were conducted at baseline and at day 14. Both participants and staff delivering the training were asked to complete an acceptability questionnaire. Results All participants demonstrated an improvement in the number of pictures identified from baseline to day 14. There was a mean improvement of 9.20 (95% CI 4.77 to 13.63) in the 14 days. This is a statistically significant improvement in the scanning wall score between baseline line and day 14 (p = 0.01). All participants and staff reported the scanning wall as acceptable to use. Conclusion This proof-of-concept study has demonstrated the scanning wall could be used to assess for visual inattention in extra personal space. Also, it could be beneficial and is acceptable for the treatment of visual inattention within a hospital inpatient setting for acute stroke survivors.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Natalie Sharp
- Leighton Hospital, Mid Cheshire Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust, Crewe, UK
| | | |
Collapse
|
2
|
Moore MJ, Driscoll R, Colwell M, Hewitt O, Demeyere N. Aligning formal and functional assessments of Visuospatial Neglect: A mixed-methods study. Neuropsychol Rehabil 2022; 32:2560-2579. [PMID: 34392812 DOI: 10.1080/09602011.2021.1967172] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/30/2022]
Abstract
ABSTRACTThe occurrence of visuospatial neglect acts as a key predictor of recovery outcome following stroke. However, the specific behavioural profiles associated with various neglect subtypes are not well understood. This study aims to identify real-world functional impairments associated with neglect, to determine whether functional impairment profiles differ across patients with egocentric and allocentric neglect, and to investigate how neglect severity predicts functional impairments.Notes from 290 stroke patients' occupational therapy functional assessments were qualitatively and quantitatively analysed in the context of neglect type and severity as reported by the OCS Cancellation Task. Overall, neglect patients had more references to having difficulty initiating tasks, finding items, exhibiting spatial inattention, and having difficulty using both arms than patients without neglect. The proportion of theme references did not differ significantly across patients with egocentric and allocentric neglect. The quantitative severity of egocentric neglect was acted as a significant predictor of reference occurrence over and above stroke severity within difficulty finding items, spatial inattention, body inattention, and upper limb use.This study expands on previous findings by identifying real-world functional impairments differentiating patients with and without neglect. This data provides novel insight into the impact of neglect on functional abilities.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Margaret Jane Moore
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Radcliffe Observatory Quarter, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
| | - Rebecca Driscoll
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Radcliffe Observatory Quarter, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
| | - Michael Colwell
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Radcliffe Observatory Quarter, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
| | - Olivia Hewitt
- Oxford Institute of Clinical Psychology Training and Research, Isis Education Centre, Warneford Hospital, Oxford, UK
| | - Nele Demeyere
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Radcliffe Observatory Quarter, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Kwon S, Fahrenthold BK, Cavanaugh MR, Huxlin KR, Mitchell JF. Perceptual restoration fails to recover unconscious processing for smooth eye movements after occipital stroke. eLife 2022; 11:67573. [PMID: 35730931 PMCID: PMC9255960 DOI: 10.7554/elife.67573] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/15/2021] [Accepted: 06/21/2022] [Indexed: 11/28/2022] Open
Abstract
The visual pathways that guide actions do not necessarily mediate conscious perception. Patients with primary visual cortex (V1) damage lose conscious perception but often retain unconscious abilities (e.g. blindsight). Here, we asked if saccade accuracy and post-saccadic following responses (PFRs) that automatically track target motion upon saccade landing are retained when conscious perception is lost. We contrasted these behaviors in the blind and intact fields of 11 chronic V1-stroke patients, and in 8 visually intact controls. Saccade accuracy was relatively normal in all cases. Stroke patients also had normal PFR in their intact fields, but no PFR in their blind fields. Thus, V1 damage did not spare the unconscious visual processing necessary for automatic, post-saccadic smooth eye movements. Importantly, visual training that recovered motion perception in the blind field did not restore the PFR, suggesting a clear dissociation between pathways mediating perceptual restoration and automatic actions in the V1-damaged visual system.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sunwoo Kwon
- Herbert Wertheim School of Optometry and Vision Science, University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, United States
| | | | - Matthew R Cavanaugh
- Center for Visual Science, University of Rochester, Rochester, United States
| | - Krystel R Huxlin
- Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, University of Rochester, Rochester, United States
| | - Jude F Mitchell
- Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, University of Rochester, Rochester, United States
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Unilateral Stroke: Computer-based Assessment Uncovers Non-Lateralized and Contralesional Visuoattentive Deficits. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2021; 27:959-969. [PMID: 33551012 DOI: 10.1017/s1355617720001393] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE Patients with unilateral stroke commonly show hemispatial neglect or milder contralesional visuoattentive deficits, but spatially non-lateralized visuoattentive deficits have also been reported. The aim of the present study was to compare spatially lateralized (i.e., contralesional) and non-lateralized (i.e., general) visuoattentive deficits in left and right hemisphere stroke patients. METHOD Participants included 40 patients with chronic unilateral stroke in either the left hemisphere (LH group, n = 20) or the right hemisphere (RH group, n = 20) and 20 healthy controls. To assess the contralesional deficits, we used a traditional paper-and-pencil cancellation task (the Bells Test) and a Lateralized Targets Computer Task. To assess the non-lateralized deficits, we developed a novel large-screen (173 × 277 cm) computer method, the Ball Rain task, with moving visual stimuli and fast-paced requirements for selective attention. RESULTS There were no contralesional visuoattentive deficits according to the cancellation task. However, in the Lateralized Targets Computer Task, RH patients missed significantly more left-sided than right-sided targets in bilateral trials. This omission distribution differed significantly from those of the controls and LH patients. In the assessment of non-lateralized attention, RH and LH patients missed significantly more Ball Rain targets than controls in both the left and right hemifields. CONCLUSIONS Computer-based assessment sensitively reveals various aspects of visuoattentive deficits in unilateral stroke. Patients with either right or left hemisphere stroke demonstrate non-lateralized visual inattention. In right hemisphere stroke, these symptoms can be accompanied by subtle contralesional visuoattentive deficits that have remained unnoticed in cancellation task.
Collapse
|
5
|
Lugtmeijer S, Lammers NA, de Haan EHF, de Leeuw FE, Kessels RPC. Post-Stroke Working Memory Dysfunction: A Meta-Analysis and Systematic Review. Neuropsychol Rev 2020; 31:202-219. [PMID: 33230717 PMCID: PMC7889582 DOI: 10.1007/s11065-020-09462-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/17/2019] [Accepted: 10/07/2020] [Indexed: 12/16/2022]
Abstract
This review investigates the severity and nature of post-stroke working memory deficits with reference to the multi-component model of working memory. We conducted a systematic search in PubMed up to March 2019 with search terms for stroke and memory. Studies on adult stroke patients, that included a control group, and assessed working memory function, were selected. Effect sizes (Hedges' g) were extracted from 50 studies (in total 3,084 stroke patients) based on the sample size, mean and standard deviation of patients and controls. Performance of stroke patients was compared to healthy controls on low-load (i.e. capacity) and high-load (executively demanding) working memory tasks, grouped by modality (verbal, non-verbal). A separate analysis compared patients in the sub-acute and the chronic stage. Longitudinal studies and effects of lesion location were systematically reviewed. Stroke patients demonstrated significant deficits in working memory with a moderate effect size for both low-load (Hedges' g = -.58 [-.82 to -.43]) and high-load (Hedges' g = -.59 [-.73 to -.45]) tasks. The effect sizes were comparable for verbal and non-verbal material. Systematically reviewing the literature showed that working memory deficits remain prominent in the chronic stage of stroke. Lesions in a widespread fronto-parietal network are associated with working memory deficits. Stroke patients show decrements of moderate magnitude in all subsystems of working memory. This review clearly demonstrates the global nature of the impairment in working memory post-stroke.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Selma Lugtmeijer
- University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands. .,Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University, Nijmegen, the Netherlands.
| | | | | | - Frank-Erik de Leeuw
- Radboud University Medical Center, Department of Neurology, Nijmegen, the Netherlands
| | - Roy P C Kessels
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University, Nijmegen, the Netherlands.,Department of Medical Psychology, Radboud University Medical Center, Nijmegen, the Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Tamura M, Shirakawa M, Luo ZW, Noda K, Tanemura R. Qualitative assessment for extrapersonal neglect in patients with stroke using a virtual reality system task. COGENT MEDICINE 2019. [DOI: 10.1080/2331205x.2019.1687272] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/25/2022] Open
Affiliation(s)
- Masaki Tamura
- Graduate School of Health Sciences, Kobe University, Kobe, Hyogo 654-0142, Japan
- Department of Physical Therapy, Asahi College of Medical and Health Professionals, Okayama, Okayama 700-0026, Japan
| | - Makoto Shirakawa
- Graduate School of System Informatics, Kobe University, Kobe, Hyogo 657-8501, Japan
| | - Zhi-wei Luo
- Graduate School of System Informatics, Kobe University, Kobe, Hyogo 657-8501, Japan
| | - Kazue Noda
- Graduate School of Health Sciences, Kobe University, Kobe, Hyogo 654-0142, Japan
| | - Rumi Tanemura
- Graduate School of Health Sciences, Kobe University, Kobe, Hyogo 654-0142, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
No direction specific costs in trans-saccadic memory. Neuropsychologia 2019; 125:23-29. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2019.01.014] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/26/2018] [Revised: 01/04/2019] [Accepted: 01/24/2019] [Indexed: 11/21/2022]
|
8
|
Occurrence and Recovery of Different Neglect-Related Symptoms in Right Hemisphere Infarct Patients during a 1-Year Follow-Up. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2018; 24:617-628. [PMID: 29611492 DOI: 10.1017/s1355617718000176] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
Abstract
OBJECTIVES To examine the occurrence of and recovery from visual neglect-related symptoms with the focus on neglect laterality, ipsilateral orienting bias, and slowed processing speed in right hemisphere (RH) infarct patients during a 1-year follow-up. Furthermore, to propose guidelines for assessing processing speed alongside the Behavioural Inattention Test (BIT). METHODS We studied three RH patient groups: neglect (N+), mild left inattention (MLI+), and non-neglect (N-) patients, and healthy controls. The BIT with some additional analyses was conducted at the acute phase and at 6 and 12 months. RESULTS The N+ group's BIT score increased and originally lateralized omissions became more evenly distributed during the follow-up. The N+ and MLI+ groups' starting points were more rightward located than the healthy group's at the acute phase and at 6, and partly at 12 months. Patient groups were slower than the controls in performing cancellation tests at the acute phase. The N+ and MLI+ groups remained slower than the controls throughout the follow-up. CONCLUSIONS During the first year after RH infarct, originally left-sided manifestation of neglect shifted toward milder non-lateralized attentional deficit. Ipsilateral orienting bias and slowed processing speed appeared to be rather persistent neglect-related symptoms both in neglect patients and patients with initially milder inattention. We propose some effortless, tentative ways of examining processing speed and ipsilateral orienting bias alongside the BIT to better recognize these neglect-related symptoms, and highlight the need to assess and treat patients with initially milder inattention, who have been under-recognized and under-treated in clinical work. (JINS, 2018, 24, 617-628).
Collapse
|
9
|
Emerson RL, García-Molina A, López Carballo J, García Fernández J, Aparicio-López C, Novo J, Sánchez-Carrión R, Enseñat-Cantallops A, Peña-Casanova J. Visual search in unilateral spatial neglect: The effects of distractors on a dynamic visual search task. APPLIED NEUROPSYCHOLOGY-ADULT 2018; 26:401-410. [PMID: 29469619 DOI: 10.1080/23279095.2018.1434522] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
Abstract
The objective of this study was to examine visual scanning performance in patients with Unilateral Spatial Neglect (USN) in a visual search task. Thirty-one right hemisphere stroke patients with USN were recruited. They performed a dynamic visual search task with two conditions, with and without distractors, while eye movements were monitored with an eye-tracker. The main goal of the task was to select target stimuli that appeared from the top of the screen and moved vertically downward. Target detection and visual scanning percentage were assessed over two hemispaces (right, left) on two conditions (distractor, no distractor). Most Scanned Regions (MSR) were calculated to analyze the areas of the screen where most points of fixation were directed to. Higher target detection rate and visual scanning percentages were found on the right hemispace on both conditions. From the MSRs we found that participants with a center of attention further to the right of the screen also presented smaller overall MSRs. Right hemisphere stroke patients with USN presented not only a significant rightward bias but reduced overall search areas, implying hyperattention does not only restrict search on the horizontal (right-left) axis but the vertical axis (top-bottom) too.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rebeca Lauren Emerson
- a Àrea de Rehabilitació NeuroPiscoSocial, Institut Guttmann, Institut Universitari de Neurorehabilitació adscrit a la UAB , Barcelona , Spain.,b Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona , Barcelona , Bellaterra , Spain.,c Fundació Institut d'Investigació en Ciències de la Salut Germans Trias i Pujol , Barcelona , Spain
| | - Alberto García-Molina
- a Àrea de Rehabilitació NeuroPiscoSocial, Institut Guttmann, Institut Universitari de Neurorehabilitació adscrit a la UAB , Barcelona , Spain.,b Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona , Barcelona , Bellaterra , Spain.,c Fundació Institut d'Investigació en Ciències de la Salut Germans Trias i Pujol , Barcelona , Spain
| | - Jaume López Carballo
- a Àrea de Rehabilitació NeuroPiscoSocial, Institut Guttmann, Institut Universitari de Neurorehabilitació adscrit a la UAB , Barcelona , Spain.,b Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona , Barcelona , Bellaterra , Spain.,c Fundació Institut d'Investigació en Ciències de la Salut Germans Trias i Pujol , Barcelona , Spain
| | - Juan García Fernández
- a Àrea de Rehabilitació NeuroPiscoSocial, Institut Guttmann, Institut Universitari de Neurorehabilitació adscrit a la UAB , Barcelona , Spain.,b Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona , Barcelona , Bellaterra , Spain.,c Fundació Institut d'Investigació en Ciències de la Salut Germans Trias i Pujol , Barcelona , Spain
| | - Celeste Aparicio-López
- a Àrea de Rehabilitació NeuroPiscoSocial, Institut Guttmann, Institut Universitari de Neurorehabilitació adscrit a la UAB , Barcelona , Spain.,b Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona , Barcelona , Bellaterra , Spain.,c Fundació Institut d'Investigació en Ciències de la Salut Germans Trias i Pujol , Barcelona , Spain
| | - Junquera Novo
- a Àrea de Rehabilitació NeuroPiscoSocial, Institut Guttmann, Institut Universitari de Neurorehabilitació adscrit a la UAB , Barcelona , Spain.,b Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona , Barcelona , Bellaterra , Spain.,c Fundació Institut d'Investigació en Ciències de la Salut Germans Trias i Pujol , Barcelona , Spain
| | - Rocío Sánchez-Carrión
- a Àrea de Rehabilitació NeuroPiscoSocial, Institut Guttmann, Institut Universitari de Neurorehabilitació adscrit a la UAB , Barcelona , Spain.,b Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona , Barcelona , Bellaterra , Spain.,c Fundació Institut d'Investigació en Ciències de la Salut Germans Trias i Pujol , Barcelona , Spain
| | - Antonia Enseñat-Cantallops
- a Àrea de Rehabilitació NeuroPiscoSocial, Institut Guttmann, Institut Universitari de Neurorehabilitació adscrit a la UAB , Barcelona , Spain.,b Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona , Barcelona , Bellaterra , Spain.,c Fundació Institut d'Investigació en Ciències de la Salut Germans Trias i Pujol , Barcelona , Spain
| | - Jordi Peña-Casanova
- d Behavioral Neurology Research Group, Mar Institute of Medical Research Foundation (FIMIM) , Barcelona , Spain.,e Department of Psychiatry and Legal Medicine, Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona , Barcelona , Spain.,f Section of Behavioral Neurology and Dementias, Hospital del Mar, Parc Salut Mar , Barcelona , Spain
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Takamura Y, Imanishi M, Osaka M, Ohmatsu S, Tominaga T, Yamanaka K, Morioka S, Kawashima N. Intentional gaze shift to neglected space: a compensatory strategy during recovery after unilateral spatial neglect. Brain 2016; 139:2970-2982. [DOI: 10.1093/brain/aww226] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/14/2016] [Accepted: 07/23/2016] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
|
11
|
Tsai CL, Chen FC, Pan CY, Tseng YT. The Neurocognitive Performance of Visuospatial Attention in Children with Obesity. Front Psychol 2016; 7:1033. [PMID: 27458421 PMCID: PMC4933706 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01033] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/18/2016] [Accepted: 06/24/2016] [Indexed: 12/11/2022] Open
Abstract
The present study investigates the behavioral performance and event-related potentials (ERPs) in children with obesity and healthy weight children when performing a visuospatial attention task. Twenty-six children with obesity (obese group) and 26 healthy weight children (control group) were recruited. Their behavioral performance during a variant of the Posner paradigm was measured, and brain ERPs were recorded concurrently. The behavioral data revealed that the obese group responded more slowly, especially in the invalid condition, and exhibited a deficit in attentional inhibition capacity as compared to the control group. In terms of cognitive electrophysiological performance, although the obese group did not show significant differences on P3 latency elicited by the target stimuli when compared to the control group, they exhibited smaller P3 amplitudes when performing the visuospatial attention task. These results broaden previous findings, and indicate that childhood obesity is associated with a reduced ability to modulate the executive function network which supports visuospatial attention.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chia-Liang Tsai
- Lab of Cognitive Neurophysiology, Institute of Physical Education, Health and Leisure Studies, National Cheng Kung University Tainan, Taiwan
| | - Fu-Chen Chen
- Department of Recreational Sport and Health Promotion, National Pingtung University of Science and Technology Tainan, Taiwan
| | - Chien-Yu Pan
- Department of Physical Education, National Kaohsiung Normal University Kaohsiung, Taiwan
| | - Yu-Ting Tseng
- Lab of Cognitive Neurophysiology, Institute of Physical Education, Health and Leisure Studies, National Cheng Kung UniversityTainan, Taiwan; School of Kinesiology, University of Minnesota, MinneapolisMN, USA
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Rinaldi L, Di Luca S, Henik A, Girelli L. Reading direction shifts visuospatial attention: an Interactive Account of attentional biases. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2014; 151:98-105. [PMID: 24968311 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.05.018] [Citation(s) in RCA: 73] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/07/2014] [Revised: 04/17/2014] [Accepted: 05/12/2014] [Indexed: 11/16/2022] Open
Abstract
A growing amount of evidence confirms the influence of reading and writing habits on visuospatial processing, although this phenomenon has been so far testified mainly as a lateralized shift of a single behavioral sign (e.g., line bisection), with lack of proof from pure right-to-left readers. The present study contributed to this issue by analyzing multiple attentional and motor indexes in monolingual Italian (i.e., reading from left-to-right), and monolingual (i.e., reading from right-to-left) and bilingual Israeli (i.e., reading from right-to-left in Hebrew but also from left-to-right in English) participants' visuospatial performance. Subjects were administered a computerized standard star cancellation task and a modified version in which English letters and words were replaced by Hebrew ones. Tasks were presented on a graphics tablet, allowing recording of both chronometric and spatial parameters (i.e., measured in (x, y) vector coordinates). Results showed that reading direction modulated the on-line visuomotor performance (i.e., left-to-right vs. right-to-left shifts) from the beginning (i.e., first mark) to the end of the task (i.e., spatial distribution of omissions and subjective epicenter). Additionally, the spatial bias observed in a computerized line bisection task was also related to the participants' habitual reading direction. Overall, the results favor the proposal of an Interactive Account of visuospatial asymmetries, according to which both cultural factors, such as the directional scanning associated with language processing, and biological factors, such as hemispheric specialization, modulate visuospatial processing. Results are discussed in light of recent behavioral and neuroanatomical findings.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Luca Rinaldi
- Department of Psychology, University of Milano-Bicocca, Milano, Italy.
| | - Samuel Di Luca
- Centre de Neuroscience Système et Cognition, Institut de Recherche en Sciences Psychologiques, Université Catholique de Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium
| | - Avishai Henik
- Department of Psychology and Zlotowski Center for Neuroscience, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beer-Sheva, Israel
| | - Luisa Girelli
- Department of Psychology, University of Milano-Bicocca, Milano, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Mahayana IT, Liu CL, Chang CF, Hung DL, Tzeng OJL, Juan CH, Muggleton NG. Far-space neglect in conjunction but not feature search following transcranial magnetic stimulation over right posterior parietal cortex. J Neurophysiol 2014; 111:705-14. [DOI: 10.1152/jn.00492.2013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Near- and far-space coding in the human brain is a dynamic process. Areas in dorsal, as well as ventral visual association cortex, including right posterior parietal cortex (rPPC), right frontal eye field (rFEF), and right ventral occipital cortex (rVO), have been shown to be important in visuospatial processing, but the involvement of these areas when the information is in near or far space remains unclear. There is a need for investigations of these representations to help explain the pathophysiology of hemispatial neglect, and the role of near and far space is crucial to this. We used a conjunction visual search task using an elliptical array to investigate the effects of transcranial magnetic stimulation delivered over rFEF, rPPC, and rVO on the processing of targets in near and far space and at a range of horizontal eccentricities. As in previous studies, we found that rVO was involved in far-space search, and rFEF was involved regardless of the distance to the array. It was found that rPPC was involved in search only in far space, with a neglect-like effect when the target was located in the most eccentric locations. No effects were seen for any site for a feature search task. As the search arrays had higher predictability with respect to target location than is often the case, these data may form a basis for clarifying both the role of PPC in visual search and its contribution to neglect, as well as the importance of near and far space in these.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Indra T. Mahayana
- Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, National Central University, Jhongli, Taiwan
| | - Chia-Lun Liu
- Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, National Central University, Jhongli, Taiwan
| | - Chi Fu Chang
- Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, National Central University, Jhongli, Taiwan
| | - Daisy L. Hung
- Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, National Central University, Jhongli, Taiwan
- Institute of Neuroscience, National Yang-Ming University, Taipei, Taiwan
- Laboratories for Cognitive Neuroscience, National Yang-Ming University, Taipei, Taiwan
| | - Ovid J. L. Tzeng
- Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, National Central University, Jhongli, Taiwan
- Institute of Neuroscience, National Yang-Ming University, Taipei, Taiwan
- Laboratories for Cognitive Neuroscience, National Yang-Ming University, Taipei, Taiwan
- Institute of Linguistics, Academia Sinica, Taipei, Taiwan
| | - Chi-Hung Juan
- Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, National Central University, Jhongli, Taiwan
- Laboratories for Cognitive Neuroscience, National Yang-Ming University, Taipei, Taiwan
| | - Neil G. Muggleton
- Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, National Central University, Jhongli, Taiwan
- Laboratories for Cognitive Neuroscience, National Yang-Ming University, Taipei, Taiwan
- Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, London, United Kingdom; and
- Department of Psychology, Goldsmiths, University of London, New Cross, London, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Abstract
AbstractNeuropsychological studies of patients with visuospatial neglect have shown differences in perceptual deficits for information in near space (i.e. near to the body) and information in far space. It has been suggested that among the many areas of the human brain, a number of areas are associated with a set of spatial maps specialized for visuospatial control related to this spatial distinction. This paper reviews how parietal cortex is thought to be involved in visuospatial neglect in relation to its control of visuospatial attention in the left and right visual fields and at different viewing distances. In particular, the importance of regions of the parietal cortex in the pathogenesis of neglect and in spatial attention and perception is discussed. Parietal cortex may control different distributions of attention across space by allocating specific attentional resources in near and far space while also showing attentional asymmetry across visual fields. Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) as a technique offers the advantage of examining the direct behavioral effect of disruption of many of these areas with excellent temporal and spatial resolution. We discuss the use of TMS and the insights it may offer regarding the roles of these areas in neglect as well as normal visuospatial perception.
Collapse
|
15
|
Nemmi F, Boccia M, Piccardi L, Galati G, Guariglia C. Segregation of neural circuits involved in spatial learning in reaching and navigational space. Neuropsychologia 2013; 51:1561-70. [PMID: 23615031 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.03.031] [Citation(s) in RCA: 66] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/01/2012] [Revised: 03/22/2013] [Accepted: 03/23/2013] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Federico Nemmi
- Neuropsychology Center, Santa Lucia Foundation, Rome, Italy.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
16
|
Changes in trunk orientation do not induce asymmetries in covert orienting. Atten Percept Psychophys 2013; 75:1193-205. [PMID: 23666630 DOI: 10.3758/s13414-013-0470-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
We explored the effect of trunk orientation on responses to visual targets in five experiments, following work suggesting a disengage deficit in covert orienting related to changes in the trunk orientation of healthy participants. In two experiments, participants responded to the color of a target appearing in the left or right visual field following a peripheral visual cue that was informative about target location. In three additional experiments, participants responded to the location (left/right) of a target using a spatially compatible motor response. In none of the experiments did trunk orientation interact with spatial-cuing effects, suggesting that orienting behavior is not affected by the rotation of the body relative to the head. Theoretical implications are discussed.
Collapse
|
17
|
Searching in space and in time. NEBRASKA SYMPOSIUM ON MOTIVATION. NEBRASKA SYMPOSIUM ON MOTIVATION 2013; 59:5-22. [PMID: 23437628 DOI: 10.1007/978-1-4614-4794-8_2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/13/2023]
Abstract
Our conception of attention is intricately linked to limited processing capacity and the consequent requirement to select, in both space and time, what objects and actions will have access to these limited resources. Seminal studies by Treisman (Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97-136, 1980) and Broadbent (Perception and Psychophysics, 42, 105-113, 1987; Raymond et al. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 849-860, 1992) offered the field tasks for exploring the properties of attention when searching in space and time. After describing the natural history of a search episode we briefly review some of these properties. We end with the question: Is there one attentional "beam" that operates in both space and time to integrate features into objects? We sought an answer by exploring the distribution of errors when the same participant searched for targets presented at the same location with items distributed over time (McLean et al. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 35A, 171-186, 1982) and presented all at once with items distributed over space (Snyder Journal of Experimental Psychology; 92, 428-431, 1972). Preliminary results revealed a null correlation between spatial and temporal slippage suggesting separate selection mechanisms in these two domains.
Collapse
|
18
|
Chechlacz M, Rotshtein P, Humphreys GW. Neuroanatomical Dissections of Unilateral Visual Neglect Symptoms: ALE Meta-Analysis of Lesion-Symptom Mapping. Front Hum Neurosci 2012; 6:230. [PMID: 22907997 PMCID: PMC3415822 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00230] [Citation(s) in RCA: 99] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/06/2012] [Accepted: 07/18/2012] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Unilateral visual neglect is commonly defined as impaired ability to attend to stimuli presented on the side of visual space contralateral to the brain lesion. However, behavioral analyses indicate that different neglect symptoms can dissociate. The neuroanatomy of the syndrome has been hotly debated. Some groups have argued that the syndrome is linked to posterior parietal cortex lesions, while others report damage within regions including the superior temporal gyrus, insula, and basal ganglia. Several recent neuroimaging studies provide evidence that heterogeneity in the behavioral symptoms of neglect can be matched by variations in the brain lesions, and that some of the discrepancies across earlier findings might have resulted from the use of different neuropsychological tests and/or varied measures within the same task for diagnosing neglect. In this paper, we review the evidence for dissociations between both the symptoms and the neural substrates of unilateral visual neglect, drawing on ALE (anatomic likelihood estimation) meta-analyses of lesion-symptom mapping studies. Specifically, we examine dissociations between neglect symptoms associated with impaired control of attention across space (in an egocentric frame of reference) and within objects (in an allocentric frame of reference). Results of ALE meta-analyses indicated that, while egocentric symptoms are associated with damage within perisylvian network (pre- and postcentral, supramarginal, and superior temporal gyri) and damage within sub-cortical structures, more posterior lesions including the angular, middle temporal, and middle occipital gyri are associated with allocentric symptoms. Furthermore, there was high concurrence in deficits associated with white matter lesions within long association (superior longitudinal, inferior fronto-occipital, and inferior longitudinal fasciculi) and projection (corona radiata and thalamic radiation) pathways, supporting a disconnection account of the syndrome. Using this evidence we argue that different forms of neglect link to both distinct and common patterns of gray and white matter lesions. The findings are discussed in terms of functional accounts of neglect and theoretical models based on computational studies of both normal and impaired attention functions.
Collapse
|
19
|
Tsai CL, Wang CH, Tseng YT. Effects of exercise intervention on event-related potential and task performance indices of attention networks in children with developmental coordination disorder. Brain Cogn 2012; 79:12-22. [DOI: 10.1016/j.bandc.2012.02.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 42] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/22/2011] [Revised: 10/02/2011] [Accepted: 02/06/2012] [Indexed: 12/31/2022]
|
20
|
Lane AR, Ball K, Smith DT, Schenk T, Ellison A. Near and far space: Understanding the neural mechanisms of spatial attention. Hum Brain Mapp 2011; 34:356-66. [PMID: 22042759 DOI: 10.1002/hbm.21433] [Citation(s) in RCA: 33] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/10/2011] [Accepted: 07/26/2011] [Indexed: 11/07/2022] Open
Abstract
Visuospatial neglect is a multicomponent syndrome, and one dissociation reported is between neglect for near (peripersonal) and far (extrapersonal) space. Owing to patient heterogeneity and extensive lesions, it is difficult to determine the precise neural mechanisms underlying this dissociation using clinical methodology. In this study, transcranial magnetic stimulation was used to examine the involvement of three areas in the undamaged brain, while participants completed a conjunction search task in near and far space. The brain areas investigated were right posterior parietal cortex (rPPC), right frontal eye field (rFEF), and right ventral occipital cortex (rVO), each of which has been implicated in visuospatial processing. The results revealed a double dissociation, whereby rPPC was involved for search in near space only, whilst rVO only became necessary when the task was completed in far space. These data provide clear evidence for a dorsal and ventral dissociation between the processing of near and far space, which is compatible with the functional roles previously attributed to the two streams. For example, the involvement of the dorsal stream in near space reflects its role in vision for action, because it is within this spatial location that actions can be performed. The results also revealed that rFEF is involved in the processing of visual search in both near and far space and may contribute to visuospatial attention and/or the control of eye-movements irrespective of spatial frame. We discuss our results with respect to their clear ramifications for clinical diagnosis and neurorehabilitation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alison R Lane
- Cognitive Neuroscience Research Unit, Durham University, United Kingdom.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
21
|
Skakoon-Sparling SP, Vasquez BP, Hano K, Danckert J. Impairments in tactile search following superior parietal damage. Brain Cogn 2011; 76:341-8. [PMID: 21600685 DOI: 10.1016/j.bandc.2011.04.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/15/2010] [Revised: 04/26/2011] [Accepted: 04/27/2011] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Abstract
The superior parietal cortex is critical for the control of visually guided actions. Research suggests that visual stimuli relevant to actions are preferentially processed when they are in peripersonal space. One recent study demonstrated that visually guided movements towards the body were more impaired in a patient with damage to superior parietal cortex. Whereas past studies have explored disordered movement in optic ataxic patients, there has been less exploration of space perception in terms of search capacity in this population. In addition, there is some debate concerning the relationship between deficits of visuomotor control and impaired attention/perception in optic ataxia. Given that the dorsal stream has been implicated in the spatial processing of stimuli in peripersonal space, and damage to this region is known to cause optic ataxia, we felt that further investigation was warranted. We examined tactile search behavior in the fronto-parallel and radial planes in a patient with right superior parietal damage and optic ataxia. We used a pegboard with removable cylindrical pegs that allowed for the reorganization of targets between trials. To better characterize three-dimensional search behavior, we included both horizontal and vertical search conditions. Results showed that the patient spent more time searching, was more accurate and revisited more targets in right versus left space. Interestingly, the patient spent the majority of her time specifically searching the lower right quadrant of the stimulus array. Further analysis revealed lower target detection rates along the outer borders of the pegboard on all sides. The search pattern observed here is unusual considering that all targets were within arm's reach. The present experiment demonstrates that damage to superior parietal cortex impairs tactile search and biases exploration towards lower right peripersonal space.
Collapse
|
22
|
Right hemisphere infarct patients and healthy controls: evaluation of starting points in cancellation tasks. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2010; 16:902-9. [PMID: 20624331 DOI: 10.1017/s1355617710000792] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
Abstract
Patients with visual neglect (VN) tend to start cancellation tasks from the right. This exceptional initial rightward bias is also seen in some right hemisphere (RH) stroke patients who do not meet the criteria of VN in conventional tests. The present study compared RH infarct patients' (examined on average 4 days post-stroke) and healthy controls' starting points (SPs) in three cancellation tasks of the Behavioural Inattention Test (BIT). Furthermore, task-specific guideline values were defined for a normal SP to differentiate the performance of healthy subjects from that of patients with subclinical inattention. Conventional tests indicated that 15 of the 70 RH infarct patients had VN. The control group comprised 44 healthy volunteers. In each task, the VN group started the cancellations mainly from the right. The non-neglect and healthy groups initiated most cancellations from the left, more so in the healthy group. Starting more than one BIT task outside the guideline value indicated pathological inattention, as this was typical among the VN patients, but exceptional among the healthy subjects. One-third of the non-neglect patients showed pathological inattention by starting more than one task outside the guideline value. Clinical assessment of VN should, therefore, include an evaluation of the SPs to detect this subtle form of neglect.
Collapse
|