1
|
Boswell AM, Kohler PJ, McCarthy JD, Caplovitz GP. Perceived group size is determined by the centroids of the component elements. J Vis 2021; 21:1. [PMID: 34851391 PMCID: PMC8648053 DOI: 10.1167/jov.21.13.1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
To accomplish the deceptively simple task of perceiving the size of objects in the visual scene, the visual system combines information about the retinal size of the object with several other cues, including perceived distance, relative size, and prior knowledge. When local component elements are perceptually grouped to form objects, the task is further complicated because a grouped object does not have a continuous contour from which retinal size can be estimated. Here, we investigate how the visual system solves this problem and makes it possible for observers to judge the size of perceptually grouped objects. We systematically vary the shape and orientation of the component elements in a two-alternative forced-choice task and find that the perceived size of the array of component objects can be almost perfectly predicted from the distance between the centroids of the component elements and the center of the array. This is true whether the global contour forms a circle or a square. When elements were positioned such that the centroids along the global contour were at different distances from the center, perceived size was based on the average distance. These results indicate that perceived size does not depend on the size of individual elements, and that smooth contours formed by the outer edges of the component elements are not used to estimate size. The current study adds to a growing literature highlighting the importance of centroids in visual perception and may have implications for how size is estimated for ensembles of different objects.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Peter J Kohler
- Department of Psychology, York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada.,Centre for Vision Research, York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada.,
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
2
|
Mamassian P, Zannoli M. Sensory loss due to object formation. Vision Res 2020; 174:22-40. [DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2020.05.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/31/2019] [Revised: 05/14/2020] [Accepted: 05/20/2020] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
|
3
|
Danilova MV, Takahashi C, Mollon JD. How does the human visual system compare the speeds of spatially separated objects? PLoS One 2020; 15:e0231959. [PMID: 32352993 PMCID: PMC7192430 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0231959] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/02/2019] [Accepted: 04/04/2020] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
We measured psychophysical thresholds for discriminating the speeds of two arrays of moving dots. The arrays could be juxtaposed or could be spatially separated by up to 10 degrees of visual angle, eccentricity being held constant. We found that the precision of the judgments varied little with separation. Moreover, the function relating threshold to separation was similar whether the arrays moved in the same, in opposite or in orthogonal directions. And there was no significant difference in threshold whether the two stimuli were initially presented to the same cerebral hemisphere or to opposite ones. How are human observers able to compare stimuli that fall at well separated positions in the visual field? We consider two classes of explanation: (i) Observers' judgments might be based directly on the signals of dedicated 'comparator neurons', i.e. neurons drawing inputs of opposite sign from local regions of the visual field. (ii) Signals about local features might be transmitted to the site of comparison by a shared 'cerebral bus', where the same physical substrate carries different information from moment to moment. The minimal effects of proximity and direction (which might be expected to influence local detectors of relative motion), and the combinatorial explosion in the number of comparator neurons that would be required by (i), lead us to favor models of type (ii).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- M. V. Danilova
- Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, England, United Kingdom
- I. P. Pavlov Institute of Physiology, St. Petersburg, Russian Federation, Giessen, Germany
| | - C. Takahashi
- Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, England, United Kingdom
| | - J. D. Mollon
- Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, England, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Abstract
Motion information is essential in daily life because it provides cues to depth, timing, object identification, and self-motion, as well as input to the oculomotor system. As the peripheral visual field is exquisitely sensitive to motion, we investigated the periphery of individuals with central visual field loss (CFL) to determine whether speed and direction discrimination are intact in this population. We compared CFL participants' (N = 8), older (N = 6), and young controls' (N = 6) ability to discriminate motion speed and direction in a two-spatial-alternative forced-choice design. Participants viewed moving dots on the left and right of a fixation marker and judged which side had the faster speed or more clockwise direction. For the young control group, we repeated the experiment with the stimulus limited to thin strips of fixed width at eccentricities of 5°, 10°, and 15°. There was no significant difference in mean speed or direction discrimination thresholds of CFL participants and older controls for either velocity. Young controls had significantly lower thresholds than the CFL group for both tasks. We did not find an effect of visual acuity, viewing eccentricity, or scotoma location on individuals' ability to discriminate speed or direction. Our results indicate that for high-visibility stimuli moving at 5°-10°/s, speed and direction discrimination are intact in the periphery of individuals with CFL.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Natela Shanidze
- Smith-Kettlewell Eye Research Institute, San Francisco, CA, USA
| | - Preeti Verghese
- Smith-Kettlewell Eye Research Institute, San Francisco, CA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Lee ARI, Ales JM, Harris JM. Speed change discrimination for motion in depth using constant world and retinal speeds. PLoS One 2019; 14:e0214766. [PMID: 30943269 PMCID: PMC6447190 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0214766] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/15/2019] [Accepted: 03/19/2019] [Indexed: 12/04/2022] Open
Abstract
Motion at constant speed in the world maps into retinal motion very differently for lateral motion and motion in depth. The former is close to linear, for the latter, constant speed objects accelerate on the retina as they approach. Motion in depth is frequently studied using speeds that are constant on the retina, and are thus not consistent with real-world constant motion. Our aim here was to test whether this matters: are we more sensitive to real-world motion? We measured speed change discrimination for objects undergoing accelerating retinal motion in depth (consistent with constant real-world speed), and constant retinal motion in depth (consistent with real-world deceleration). Our stimuli contained both looming and binocular disparity cues to motion in depth. We used a speed change discrimination task to obtain thresholds for conditions with and without binocular and looming motion in depth cues. We found that speed change discrimination thresholds were similar for accelerating retinal speed and constant retinal speed and were notably poor compared to classic speed discrimination thresholds. We conclude that the ecologically valid retinal acceleration in our stimuli neither helps, nor hinders, our ability to make judgements in a speed change discrimination task.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Abigail R. I. Lee
- School of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of St Andrews, St Andrews, Fife, United Kingdom
- * E-mail:
| | - Justin M. Ales
- School of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of St Andrews, St Andrews, Fife, United Kingdom
| | - Julie M. Harris
- School of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of St Andrews, St Andrews, Fife, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Abstract
The ability to judge speed is a fundamental aspect of visual motion processing. Speed judgments are generally assumed to depend on signals in motion-sensitive, directionally selective, neurons in areas such as V1 and MT. Speed comparisons might therefore be expected to be most accurate when they use information within a common set of directionally tuned neurons. However, there does not appear to be any published evidence on how well speeds can be compared for movements in different directions. We tested speed discrimination judgments between pairs of random-dot stimuli presented side-by-side in a series of four experiments (n = 65). Participants judged which appeared faster of a reference stimulus moving along the cardinal or oblique axis and a comparison stimulus moving either in the same direction or in a different direction. The bias (point of subjective equality) and sensitivity (Weber fraction) were estimated from individual psychometric functions fitted for each condition. There was considerable between-participants variability in psychophysical estimates across conditions. Nonetheless, participants generally made more acute comparisons between stimuli moving in the same direction than those moving in different directions, at least for conditions with an upwards reference (∼20% difference in Weber fractions). We also showed evidence for an oblique effect in speed discrimination when comparing stimuli moving in the same direction, and a bias whereby oblique motion tended to be perceived as moving faster than cardinal motion. These results demonstrate interactions between speed and direction processing, thus informing our understanding of how they are represented in the brain.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Catherine Manning
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
| | | | - Oliver Braddick
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Cocci G, Barbieri D, Citti G, Sarti A. Cortical spatiotemporal dimensionality reduction for visual grouping. Neural Comput 2015; 27:1252-93. [PMID: 25826020 DOI: 10.1162/neco_a_00738] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/04/2022]
Abstract
The visual systems of many mammals, including humans, are able to integrate the geometric information of visual stimuli and perform cognitive tasks at the first stages of the cortical processing. This is thought to be the result of a combination of mechanisms, which include feature extraction at the single cell level and geometric processing by means of cell connectivity. We present a geometric model of such connectivities in the space of detected features associated with spatiotemporal visual stimuli and show how they can be used to obtain low-level object segmentation. The main idea is to define a spectral clustering procedure with anisotropic affinities over data sets consisting of embeddings of the visual stimuli into higher-dimensional spaces. Neural plausibility of the proposed arguments will be discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Giacomo Cocci
- Department of Electrical, Electronic, and Information Engineering "Guglielmo Marconi," University of Bologna, 40136 Bologna, Italy
| | - Davide Barbieri
- Department of Mathematics, Autonomous University of Madrid, Facultad de Ciencias, 28049 Madrid, Spain
| | - Giovanna Citti
- Department of Mathematics, University of Bologna, 40126 Bologna, Italy
| | - Alessandro Sarti
- Centre d'Analyse et de Mathématique Sociales, EHESS, 75244 Paris, France
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
The global slowdown effect: why does perceptual grouping reduce perceived speed? Atten Percept Psychophys 2014; 76:780-92. [PMID: 24448695 DOI: 10.3758/s13414-013-0607-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
The percept of four rotating dot pairs is bistable. The "local percept" is of four pairs of dots rotating independently. The "global percept" is of two large squares translating over one another (Anstis & Kim 2011). We have previously demonstrated (Kohler, Caplovitz, & Tse 2009) that the global percept appears to move more slowly than the local percept. Here, we investigate and rule out several hypotheses for why this may be the case. First, we demonstrate that the global slowdown effect does not occur because the global percept is of larger objects than the local percept. Second, we show that the global slowdown effect is not related to rotation-specific detectors that may be more active in the local than in the global percept. Third, we find that the effect is also not due to a reduction of image elements during grouping and can occur with a stimulus very different from the one used previously. This suggests that the effect may reflect a general property of perceptual grouping. Having ruled out these possibilities, we suggest that the global slowdown effect may arise from emergent motion signals that are generated by the moving dots, which are interpreted as the ends of "barbell bars" in the local percept or the corners of the illusory squares in the global percept. Alternatively, the effect could be the result of noisy sources of motion information that arise from perceptual grouping that, in turn, increase the influence of Bayesian priors toward slow motion (Weiss, Simoncelli, & Adelson 2002).
Collapse
|
9
|
Edwards M, Cassanello CR, Badcock DR, Nishida S. Effect of form cues on 1D and 2D motion pooling. Vision Res 2013; 76:94-104. [PMID: 23131840 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2012.10.015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/23/2012] [Revised: 09/28/2012] [Accepted: 10/25/2012] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
Abstract
Local-motion information can provide either 1-dimensional (1D) or 2-dimensional (2D) solutions. 1D signals occur when the aperture problem has not been solved, so each signal is an estimate of the local-orthogonal component of the object's motion. 2D signals occur when the aperture problem has been solved, so each signal is an estimate of the object's motion. Previous research (JoV, 2009, 9, 1-25) has shown that 1D and 2D signals are pooled differently, via intersection-of-constraints (IOC) and vector-average processes, respectively. Previous research (e.g. Vis. Res., 2003, 2290-2301) has also indicated that form cues can influence how motion signals are perceived. We investigated whether forms cues can affect the pooling of motion signals and whether they differentially affect the pooling of 1D and 2D signals. Global-Gabor (GG) and global-plaid (GP) stimuli were used. These stimuli consist of multiple apertures that contain either Gabors or plaids, respectively. In the GG stimulus the global solution is defined by having the Gabor carriers move (1D signals) such that they are consistent with a single IOC-defined solution. In the GP stimuli the plaid motion (2D signals) are consistent with a vector-average solution defined by a Gaussian distribution. Form cues can be introduced by adding orientation information to the apertures that is either consistent (aligned with) or inconsistent (orthogonal to) with the global-solution. With the 1D stimuli, form cues affect how the motion signals are pooled, with motion being perceived in the direction defined by the orientation cue. Orientation cues had no direct effect on the pooling of the 2D signals.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mark Edwards
- Department of Psychology, The Australian National University, Canberra, ACT, Australia.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
10
|
Schwarzkopf DS, Sterzer P, Rees G. Decoding of coherent but not incoherent motion signals in early dorsal visual cortex. Neuroimage 2010; 56:688-98. [PMID: 20385243 PMCID: PMC3084455 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.04.011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/10/2009] [Revised: 03/08/2010] [Accepted: 04/05/2010] [Indexed: 11/15/2022] Open
Abstract
When several scattered grating elements are arranged in such a way that their directions of motion are consistent with a common path, observers perceive them as belonging to a globally coherent moving object. Here we investigated how this coherence changes the representation of motion signals in human visual cortex using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and multivariate voxel pattern decoding, which have the potential to reveal how well a stimulus is encoded in different contexts. Only during globally coherent motion was it possible to reliably distinguish fMRI signals evoked by different directions of motion in early visual cortex. This effect was specific to the retinotopic representation of the visual field quadrant in V1 traversed by the coherent element path and could not simply be attributed to a general increase in signal strength. Decoding was more reliable for cortical areas corresponding to the lower visual field. Because some previous studies observed poorer speed discrimination when motion was grouped, we also conducted behavioural experiments to investigate this with our stimuli, but did not reveal a consistent relationship between coherence and perceived speed. Taken together, these data show that neuronal populations in early visual cortex represent information that could be used for interpreting motion signals as unified objects.
Collapse
|
11
|
Rasche C, Gegenfurtner KR. Precision of speed discrimination and smooth pursuit eye movements. Vision Res 2009; 49:514-23. [DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2008.12.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 32] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/21/2007] [Revised: 11/21/2008] [Accepted: 12/08/2008] [Indexed: 01/01/2023]
|
12
|
Parovel G, Casco C. The psychophysical law of speed estimation in Michotte’s causal events. Vision Res 2006; 46:4134-42. [PMID: 17007898 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2006.08.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/09/2005] [Revised: 07/27/2006] [Accepted: 08/01/2006] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
Observers saw an event in which a computer-animated square moved up to and made contact with another, which after a short delay moved off, its motion appearing to be caused by launch by the first square. Observers chose whether the second (launched) square was faster in this causal event than when presented following a long delay (non-causal event). The speed of the second object in causal events was overestimated for a wide range of speeds of the first object (launcher), but accurately assessed in non-causal events. Experiments 2 and 3 showed that overestimation occurred also in other causal displays in which the trajectories were overlapping, successive, spatially separated or inverted but did not occurred with consecutive speeds that did not produce causal percepts. We also found that if the first object in a causal event was faster, then Weber's law holds and overestimation of the launched object speed was proportional to the speed of the launcher. In contrast, if the second object was faster, overestimation was constant, i.e. independent of the launcher. We propose that the particular speed integration of causal display results in overestimation and that the way overestimation depends on V1 phenomenally affects the attribution of the source of V2 motion: either in V1 (in launching) or in V2 (in triggering).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Giulia Parovel
- Department of Communication Sciences, University of Siena, via Roma 56, 53100 Siena, Italy.
| | | |
Collapse
|