1
|
Warchoł J, Tetych A, Tomaszewski R, Kowalczyk B, Olchowik G. Virtual Reality-Induced Modification of Vestibulo-Ocular Reflex Gain in Posturography Tests. J Clin Med 2024; 13:2742. [PMID: 38792284 PMCID: PMC11122614 DOI: 10.3390/jcm13102742] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/08/2024] [Revised: 05/03/2024] [Accepted: 05/05/2024] [Indexed: 05/26/2024] Open
Abstract
Background: The aim of the study was to demonstrate the influence of virtual reality (VR) exposure on postural stability and determine the mechanism of this influence. Methods: Twenty-six male participants aged 21-23 years were included, who underwent postural stability assessment twice before and after a few minute of single VR exposure. The VR projection was a computer-generated simulation of the surrounding scenery. Postural stability was assessed using the Sensory Organization Test (SOT), using Computerized Dynamic Posturography (CDP). Results: The findings indicated that VR exposure affects the visual and vestibular systems. Significant differences (p < 0.05) in results before and after VR exposure were observed in tests on an unstable surface. It was confirmed that VR exposure has a positive influence on postural stability, attributed to an increase in the sensory weight of the vestibular system. Partial evidence suggested that the reduction in vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR) reinforcement may result in an adaptive shift to the optokinetic reflex (OKR). Conclusions: By modifying the process of environmental perception through artificial sensory simulation, the influence of VR on postural stability has been demonstrated. The validity of this type of research is determined by the effectiveness of VR techniques in the field of vestibular rehabilitation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jan Warchoł
- Department of Biophysics, Medical University of Lublin, K. Jaczewskiego 4, 20-090 Lublin, Poland; (A.T.); (B.K.); (G.O.)
| | - Anna Tetych
- Department of Biophysics, Medical University of Lublin, K. Jaczewskiego 4, 20-090 Lublin, Poland; (A.T.); (B.K.); (G.O.)
| | - Robert Tomaszewski
- Department of Computer Science, University of Applied Sciences in Biala Podlaska, Sidorska 95/97, 21-500 Biala Podlaska, Poland;
| | - Bartłomiej Kowalczyk
- Department of Biophysics, Medical University of Lublin, K. Jaczewskiego 4, 20-090 Lublin, Poland; (A.T.); (B.K.); (G.O.)
| | - Grażyna Olchowik
- Department of Biophysics, Medical University of Lublin, K. Jaczewskiego 4, 20-090 Lublin, Poland; (A.T.); (B.K.); (G.O.)
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Zhou J, Zhuang W. Physically active undergraduates perform better on executive-related oculomotor control: Evidence from the antisaccade task and pupillometry. Psych J 2023; 12:17-24. [PMID: 36109011 DOI: 10.1002/pchj.596] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/11/2021] [Accepted: 08/07/2022] [Indexed: 02/04/2023]
Abstract
Previous studies have shown that exercise can improve executive function in young and older adults. However, it remains controversial whether a sufficient amount of physical activity leads to higher-level executive function. To examine the effect of physical activity on executive function, we used eye-tracking technology and the antisaccade task in 41 young undergraduates with various levels of physical activity. Moreover, we also investigated their differences in cognitive ability by examining their pupil size during the antisaccade task. Eye-tracking results showed that physically active individuals showed shorter saccade latency and higher accuracy in the antisaccade task than their physically inactive counterparts. Furthermore, the former showed larger pupil size during the preparatory period of antisaccade. These findings suggest that individuals with higher-level physical activity have higher-level executive function. The larger pupil sizes of physically active individuals may imply that their locus coeruleus-norepinephrine system and executive-related prefrontal cortex are more active, which contributes to their higher-level cognitive ability.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Junyi Zhou
- School of Physical Education and Sport Sciences, Fujian Normal University, Fuzhou, China.,Provincial University Key Laboratory of Sport and Health Science, Fujian Normal University, Fuzhou, China.,CAS Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology, Beijing, China
| | - Wenjie Zhuang
- School of Physical Education and Sport Sciences, Fujian Normal University, Fuzhou, China
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Munoz MJ, Reilly JL, Pal GD, Verhagen Metman L, Rivera YM, Drane QH, Corcos DM, David FJ, Goelz LC. Medication adversely impacts visually-guided eye movements in Parkinson's disease. Clin Neurophysiol 2022; 143:145-153. [PMID: 35995722 DOI: 10.1016/j.clinph.2022.07.505] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/27/2022] [Revised: 07/14/2022] [Accepted: 07/18/2022] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE We examined whether previous inconsistent findings about the effect of anti-Parkinsonian medication on visually-guided saccades (VGS) were due to the use of different paradigms, which change the timing of fixation offset and target onset, or different target eccentricities. METHODS Thirty-three participants with Parkinson's disease (PD) completed the VGS tasks OFF and ON medication, along with 13 healthy controls. Performance on 3 paradigms (gap, step, and overlap) and 2 target eccentricities was recorded. We used mixed models to determine the effect of medication, paradigm, and target eccentricity on saccade latency, gain, and peak velocity. RESULTS First, we confirmed known paradigm effects on latency, and target eccentricity effects on gain and peak velocity in participants with PD. Second, latency was positively associated with OFF medication Movement Disorders Society - Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale (MDS-UPDRS) motor score in PD. Third, medication prolonged latency for the larger target eccentricity across the 3 paradigms, while decreasing gain and peak velocity in the step paradigm across target eccentricities. CONCLUSIONS Medication adversely affected and was not therapeutically beneficial for VGS. Previous inconsistencies may have resulted from chosen target eccentricity. SIGNIFICANCE The negative medication effect on VGS may be clinically significant, as many activities in daily life require oculomotor control, inhibitory control, and visually-guided shifts of attention.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Miranda J Munoz
- Department of Physical Therapy and Human Movement Sciences, Northwestern University, Chicago, IL, USA.
| | - James L Reilly
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Northwestern University Feinberg School of Medicine, Chicago, IL, USA
| | - Gian D Pal
- Department of Neurology, Rutgers University, New Brunswick, NJ, USA; Department of Neurological Sciences, Section of Parkinson Disease and Movement Disorders, Rush University Medical Center, Chicago, IL, USA
| | - Leo Verhagen Metman
- Department of Neurological Sciences, Section of Parkinson Disease and Movement Disorders, Rush University Medical Center, Chicago, IL, USA
| | - Yessenia M Rivera
- Department of Physical Therapy and Human Movement Sciences, Northwestern University, Chicago, IL, USA
| | - Quentin H Drane
- Department of Physical Therapy and Human Movement Sciences, Northwestern University, Chicago, IL, USA
| | - Daniel M Corcos
- Department of Physical Therapy and Human Movement Sciences, Northwestern University, Chicago, IL, USA
| | - Fabian J David
- Department of Physical Therapy and Human Movement Sciences, Northwestern University, Chicago, IL, USA
| | - Lisa C Goelz
- Department of Physical Therapy and Human Movement Sciences, Northwestern University, Chicago, IL, USA; Department of Kinesiology and Nutrition, University of Illinois at Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Opwonya J, Doan DNT, Kim SG, Kim JI, Ku B, Kim S, Park S, Kim JU. Saccadic Eye Movement in Mild Cognitive Impairment and Alzheimer's Disease: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Neuropsychol Rev 2022; 32:193-227. [PMID: 33959887 PMCID: PMC9090874 DOI: 10.1007/s11065-021-09495-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 43] [Impact Index Per Article: 21.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/06/2020] [Accepted: 03/01/2021] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
Alzheimer's disease (AD) is the leading cause of dementia, and mild cognitive impairment (MCI) is considered the transitional state to AD dementia (ADD) and other types of dementia, whose symptoms are accompanied by altered eye movement. In this work, we reviewed the existing literature and conducted a meta-analysis to extract relevant eye movement parameters that are significantly altered owing to ADD and MCI. We conducted a systematic review of 35 eligible original publications in saccade paradigms and a meta-analysis of 27 articles with specified task conditions, which used mainly gap and overlap conditions in both prosaccade and antisaccade paradigms. The meta-analysis revealed that prosaccade and antisaccade latencies and frequency of antisaccade errors showed significant alterations for both MCI and ADD. First, both prosaccade and antisaccade paradigms differentiated patients with ADD and MCI from controls, however, antisaccade paradigms was more effective than prosaccade paradigms in distinguishing patients from controls. Second, during prosaccade in the gap and overlap conditions, patients with ADD had significantly longer latencies than patients with MCI, and the trend was similar during antisaccade in the gap condition as patients with ADD had significantly more errors than patients with MCI. The anti-effect magnitude was similar between controls and patients, and the magnitude of the latency of the gap effect varied among healthy controls and MCI and ADD subjects, but the effect size of the latency remained large in both patients. These findings suggest that, using gap effect, anti-effect, and specific choices of saccade paradigms and conditions, distinctions could be made between MCI and ADD patients as well as between patients and controls.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Julius Opwonya
- Future Medicine Division, Korea Institute of Oriental Medicine, Daejeon, Republic of Korea
- Korean Convergence Medicine, University of Science and Technology, Daejeon, Republic of Korea
| | - Dieu Ni Thi Doan
- Future Medicine Division, Korea Institute of Oriental Medicine, Daejeon, Republic of Korea
- Korean Convergence Medicine, University of Science and Technology, Daejeon, Republic of Korea
| | - Seul Gee Kim
- Future Medicine Division, Korea Institute of Oriental Medicine, Daejeon, Republic of Korea
| | - Joong Il Kim
- Future Medicine Division, Korea Institute of Oriental Medicine, Daejeon, Republic of Korea
| | - Boncho Ku
- Future Medicine Division, Korea Institute of Oriental Medicine, Daejeon, Republic of Korea
| | - Soochan Kim
- Department of Electrical and Electronic Engineering, Hankyong National University, Anseong, Republic of Korea
| | - Sunju Park
- Department of Preventive Medicine, College of Korean Medicine, Daejeon University, Daejeon, Republic of Korea.
| | - Jaeuk U Kim
- Future Medicine Division, Korea Institute of Oriental Medicine, Daejeon, Republic of Korea.
- Korean Convergence Medicine, University of Science and Technology, Daejeon, Republic of Korea.
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Eye Movement Alterations in Post-COVID-19 Condition: A Proof-of-Concept Study. SENSORS 2022; 22:s22041481. [PMID: 35214383 PMCID: PMC8875414 DOI: 10.3390/s22041481] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/07/2022] [Revised: 02/07/2022] [Accepted: 02/10/2022] [Indexed: 02/04/2023]
Abstract
There is much evidence pointing out eye movement alterations in several neurological diseases. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first video-oculography study describing potential alterations of eye movements in the post-COVID-19 condition. Visually guided saccades, memory-guided saccades, and antisaccades in horizontal axis were measured. In all visual tests, the stimulus was deployed with a gap condition. The duration of the test was between 5 and 7 min per participant. A group of n=9 patients with the post-COVID-19 condition was included in this study. Values were compared with a group (n=9) of healthy volunteers whom the SARS-CoV-2 virus had not infected. Features such as centripetal and centrifugal latencies, success rates in memory saccades, antisaccades, and blinks were computed. We found that patients with the post-COVID-19 condition had eye movement alterations mainly in centripetal latency in visually guided saccades, the success rate in memory-guided saccade test, latency in antisaccades, and its standard deviation, which suggests the involvement of frontoparietal networks. Further work is required to understand these eye movements' alterations and their functional consequences.
Collapse
|
6
|
Zhou J. Differences on Prosaccade Task in Skilled and Less Skilled Female Adolescent Soccer Players. Front Psychol 2021; 12:711420. [PMID: 34721156 PMCID: PMC8551357 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.711420] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/18/2021] [Accepted: 09/23/2021] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Although the relationship between cognitive processes and saccadic eye movements has been outlined, the relationship between specific cognitive processes underlying saccadic eye movements and skill level of soccer players remains unclear. Present study used the prosaccade task as a tool to investigate the difference in saccadic eye movements in skilled and less skilled Chinese female adolescent soccer players. Fifty-six healthy female adolescent soccer players (range: 14–18years, mean age: 16.5years) from Fujian Youth Football Training Base (Fujian Province, China) took part in the experiment. In the prosaccade task, participants were instructed to fixate at the cross at the center of the screen as long as the target appeared peripherally. They were told to saccade to the target as quickly and accurately as possible once it appeared. The results indicated that skilled soccer players exhibited shorter saccade latency (p=0.031), decreased variability of saccade latency (p=0.013), and higher spatial accuracy of saccade (p=0.032) than their less skilled counterparts. The shorter saccade latency and decreased variability of saccade latency may imply that the attentional system of skilled soccer player is superior which leads to smaller attention fluctuation and less attentional lapse. Additionally, higher spatial accuracy of saccade may imply potential structural differences in brain underlying saccadic eye movement between skilled and less skilled soccer players. More importantly, the results of the present study demonstrated that soccer players’ cognitive capacities vary as a function of their skill levels. The limitations of the present study and future directions of research were discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Junyi Zhou
- School of Physical Education and Sport Sciences, Fujian Normal University, Fuzhou, China.,Provincial University Key Laboratory of Sport and Health Science, Fujian Normal University, Fuzhou, China.,Key Laboratory of Kinesiological Evaluation General Administration of Sport of China, Fujian Normal University, Fuzhou, China
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Koçoğlu K, Hodgson TL, Eraslan Boz H, Akdal G. Deficits in saccadic eye movements differ between subtypes of patients with mild cognitive impairment. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2021; 43:187-198. [PMID: 33792489 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2021.1900077] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
Abstract
Introduction: Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) is known to be heterogeneous in its cognitive features and course of progression. Whilst memory impairment is characteristic of amnestic MCI (aMCI), cognitive deficits other than memory can occur in both aMCI and non-amnestic MCI (naMCI) and accurate assessment of the subtypes of MCI is difficult for clinicians without the application of extensive neuropsychological testing. In this study, we examine metrics derived from recording of reflexive and voluntary saccadic eye movements as a potential alternative method for discriminating between subtypes and assessing cognitive functions in MCI.Method: A total of 29 MCI patients and 29 age- and education-matched healthy controls (HCs) participated in the cross-sectional study. We recorded horizontal and vertical pro-saccades and anti-saccade responses. All the participants also completed a comprehensive neuropsychological tests battery.Results: Significant differences in saccadic eye movement were found between the subtypes of MCI and HCs. Patients with aMCI had a higher percentage of short latency "express" saccades than HCs. We found strong associations between saccadic reaction times and cognitive domains, including executive functions and attention. The mini-mental state examination (MMSE) was also found to correlate with uncorrected errors in the anti-saccade task.Conclusions: The increased proportion of saccades in the express latency range in aMCI may be indicative of problems with cognitive inhibitory control in these patients. A focus on this and other saccade metrics in the preclinical and prodromal stages of dementia may help to predict the clinical progression of the disease and direct interventions for the management of MCI. The clinical significance of saccadic eye movement impairments in MCI is not yet fully understood and should be investigated in further studies using larger samples.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Koray Koçoğlu
- Department of Neurosciences, Institute of Health Sciences, Dokuz Eylül University, İzmir, Turkey
| | | | - Hatice Eraslan Boz
- Department of Neurosciences, Institute of Health Sciences, Dokuz Eylül University, İzmir, Turkey.,Department of Neurology, Faculty of Medicine, Dokuz Eylül University, İzmir, Turkey
| | - Gülden Akdal
- Department of Neurosciences, Institute of Health Sciences, Dokuz Eylül University, İzmir, Turkey.,Department of Neurology, Faculty of Medicine, Dokuz Eylül University, İzmir, Turkey
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Blue-light effects on saccadic eye movements and attentional disengagement. Atten Percept Psychophys 2021; 83:1713-1728. [PMID: 33751450 DOI: 10.3758/s13414-021-02250-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 01/11/2021] [Indexed: 12/19/2022]
Abstract
People are constantly exposed to high-energy blue light as they spend considerable amounts of time reading and browsing materials on electronic products like computers and cellphones. Recent studies suggest that the stimulation of intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells (ipRGCs)-a newly discovered type of photoreceptor shown to be particularly sensitive to blue light-activates brain regions related to eye movements and attentional orienting (e.g., frontal eye fields). It remains unclear, however, whether and how blue light affects eye movements and attention behaviorally. We examined this by adopting the gap paradigm in which participants made saccades to a peripheral target as quickly and accurately as possible while the fixation sign vanished (i.e., the gap condition) or remained visible. Participants were exposed to blue and orange light on two separate days. Faster saccade latency under blue light was found across two experiments, and the results indicate that blue light shortened saccade latency when attention and eye movements operate simultaneously. Our findings provide evidence for the blue-light facilitatory effect on eye movements and attentional disengagement, and suggest that blue light can enhance the speed of saccadic eye movements.
Collapse
|
9
|
Abstract
Successful goal-directed visual behavior depends on efficient disengagement of attention. Attention must be withdrawn from its current focus before being redeployed to a new object or internal process. Previous research has demonstrated that occupying cognitive processes with a secondary cellular phone conversation impairs attentional functioning and driving behavior. For example, attentional processing is significantly impacted by concurrent cell phone use, resulting in decreased explicit memory for on-road information. Here, we examined the impact of a critical component of cell-phone use-active listening-on the effectiveness of attentional disengagement. In the gap task-a saccadic manipulation of attentional disengagement-we measured saccade latencies while participants performed a secondary active listening task. Saccadic latencies significantly increased under an active listening load only when attention needed to be disengaged, indicating that active listening delays a disengagement operation. Simple dual-task interference did not account for the observed results. Rather, active cognitive engagement is required for measurable disengagement slowing to be observed. These results have implications for investigations of attention, gaze behavior, and distracted driving. Secondary tasks such as active listening or cell-phone conversations can have wide-ranging impacts on cognitive functioning, potentially impairing relatively elementary operations of attentional function, including disengagement.
Collapse
|
10
|
|
11
|
Kim K, Lee C. Activity of primate V1 neurons during the gap saccade task. J Neurophysiol 2017; 118:1361-1375. [PMID: 28615338 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00758.2016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/20/2016] [Revised: 06/14/2017] [Accepted: 06/14/2017] [Indexed: 12/18/2022] Open
Abstract
When a saccadic eye movement is made toward a visual stimulus, the variability in accompanying primary visual cortex (V1) activity is related to saccade latency in both humans and simians. To understand the nature of this relationship, we examined the functional link between V1 activity and the initiation of visually guided saccades during the gap saccade task, in which a brief temporal gap is inserted between the turning off of a fixation stimulus and the appearance of a saccadic target. The insertion of such a gap robustly reduces saccade latency and facilitates the occurrence of extremely short-latency (express) saccades. Here we recorded single-cell activity from macaque V1 while monkeys performed the gap saccade task. In parallel with the gap effect on saccade latency the neural latency (time of first spike) of V1 response elicited by the saccade target became shorter, and the firing rate increased as the gap duration increased. Similarly, neural latency was shorter and firing rate was higher before express saccades relative to regular-latency saccades. In addition to these posttarget changes, the level of spontaneous spike activity during the pretarget period was negatively correlated with both neural and saccade latencies. These results demonstrate that V1 activity correlates with the gap effect and indicate that trial-to-trial variability in the state of V1 accompanies the variability of neural and behavioral latencies.NEW & NOTEWORTHY The link between neural activity in monkey primary visual cortex (V1) and visually guided behavioral response is confirmed with the gap saccade paradigm. Results indicated that the variability in neural latency of V1 spike activity correlates with the gap effect on saccade latency and that the trial-to-trial variability in the state of V1 before the onset of saccade target correlates with the variability in neural and behavioral latencies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kayeon Kim
- Department of Psychology, Seoul National University, Kwanak, Seoul, Republic of Korea
| | - Choongkil Lee
- Department of Psychology, Seoul National University, Kwanak, Seoul, Republic of Korea
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Noiret N, Carvalho N, Laurent É, Chopard G, Binetruy M, Nicolier M, Monnin J, Magnin E, Vandel P. Saccadic Eye Movements and Attentional Control in Alzheimer's Disease. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2017; 33:1-13. [DOI: 10.1093/arclin/acx044] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/17/2016] [Accepted: 05/09/2017] [Indexed: 11/14/2022] Open
|
13
|
Noiret N, Vigneron B, Diogo M, Vandel P, Laurent É. Saccadic eye movements: what do they tell us about aging cognition? AGING NEUROPSYCHOLOGY AND COGNITION 2016; 24:575-599. [DOI: 10.1080/13825585.2016.1237613] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Nicolas Noiret
- Laboratoire de Psychologie EA-3188, Université Bourgogne Franche-Comté, Besançon, France
- Centre Mémoire de Ressources et de Recherche (CMRR), Centre Hospitalier Régional Universitaire (CHRU) Jean Minjoz, Besançon, France
| | - Blanche Vigneron
- Laboratoire de Psychologie EA-3188, Université Bourgogne Franche-Comté, Besançon, France
| | - Marine Diogo
- Laboratoire de Psychologie EA-3188, Université Bourgogne Franche-Comté, Besançon, France
| | - Pierre Vandel
- Centre Mémoire de Ressources et de Recherche (CMRR), Centre Hospitalier Régional Universitaire (CHRU) Jean Minjoz, Besançon, France
- Laboratoire de Neurosciences EA-481, Université Bourgogne Franche-Comté, Besançon, France
| | - Éric Laurent
- Laboratoire de Psychologie EA-3188, Université Bourgogne Franche-Comté, Besançon, France
- Maison des Sciences de l’Homme et de l’Environnement (UMSR-3124), CNRS & Université Bourgogne Franche-Comté, Besançon, France
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Kleberg JL, Thorup E, Falck-Ytter T. Reduced visual disengagement but intact phasic alerting in young children with autism. Autism Res 2016; 10:539-545. [PMID: 27696688 PMCID: PMC5396274 DOI: 10.1002/aur.1675] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/03/2016] [Accepted: 06/27/2016] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
Abstract
Children with autism may have difficulties with visual disengagement-that is, inhibiting current fixations and orienting to new stimuli in the periphery. These difficulties may limit these children's ability to flexibly monitor the environment, regulate their internal states, and interact with others. In typical development, visual disengagement is influenced by a phasic alerting network that increases the processing speed of the visual system after salient events. The role of the phasic alerting effect in the putative atypical disengagement performance in autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is not known. Here, we compared visual disengagement in six-year-old children with autism (N = 18) and typically developing children (N = 17) matched for age and nonverbal IQ. We manipulated phasic alerting during a visual disengagement task by adding spatially nonpredictive sounds shortly before the onset of the visual peripheral targets. Children with ASD showed evidence of delayed disengagement compared to the control group. Sounds facilitated visual disengagement similarly in both groups, suggesting typical modulation by phasic alerting in ASD in the context of this task. These results support the view that atypical visual disengagement in ASD is related to other factors than atypicalities in the alerting network. Autism Res 2017, 10: 539-545. © 2016 The Authors Autism Research published by Wiley Periodicals, Inc. on behalf of International Society for Autism Research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Emilia Thorup
- From the Department of Psychology, Uppsala University, Uppsala, Sweden
| | - Terje Falck-Ytter
- From the Department of Psychology, Uppsala University, Uppsala, Sweden.,Center of Neurodevelopmental Disorders at Karolinska Institutet (KIND), Karolinska institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
A mathematical model of saccadic reaction time as a function of the fixation point brightness gain. Atten Percept Psychophys 2015; 77:2153-65. [PMID: 25962456 DOI: 10.3758/s13414-015-0902-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
The gap effect refers to a reduction in saccadic reaction time (SRT) to an eccentric target, when the fixation point is removed before the target onset. Though it is known that the gap effect peaks when the fixation point is offset about 200 ms before the onset of the eccentric target, it is unknown how this effect is modulated by stimulus variations. In this paper, we propose and investigate a model of saccadic reaction time as a function of the fixation point brightness gain. The brightness gain is defined as the ratio of the final and initial intensities of the stimulus. We have conducted a typical gap effect experiment with 15 participants, where the brightness of the fixation point was manipulated under four conditions and two gap intervals, at the same time and 200 ms before the onset of the eccentric target. The conditions included removing the fixation point (offset), leaving it with constant brightness (overlap), reducing, and increasing its brightness (lower and higher brightness conditions). Experimental data showed a significant gap effect in the offset and lower brightness conditions when compared to the overlap condition. On the other hand, the SRT was significantly longer for the higher brightness condition than the SRT for the overlap condition. Linear regression analysis using ten values of brightness gain shows that our model fits the data well for the 0- and 200-ms gap, with a coefficient of determination of .89 and .94, respectively.
Collapse
|
16
|
Action and perception are temporally coupled by a common mechanism that leads to a timing misperception. J Neurosci 2015; 35:1493-504. [PMID: 25632126 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.2054-14.2015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/08/2023] Open
Abstract
We move our eyes to explore the world, but visual areas determining where to look next (action) are different from those determining what we are seeing (perception). Whether, or how, action and perception are temporally coordinated is not known. The preparation time course of an action (e.g., a saccade) has been widely studied with the gap/overlap paradigm with temporal asynchronies (TA) between peripheral target onset and fixation point offset (gap, synchronous, or overlap). However, whether the subjects perceive the gap or overlap, and when they perceive it, has not been studied. We adapted the gap/overlap paradigm to study the temporal coupling of action and perception. Human subjects made saccades to targets with different TAs with respect to fixation point offset and reported whether they perceived the stimuli as separated by a gap or overlapped in time. Both saccadic and perceptual report reaction times changed in the same way as a function of TA. The TA dependencies of the time change for action and perception were very similar, suggesting a common neural substrate. Unexpectedly, in the perceptual task, subjects misperceived lights overlapping by less than ∼100 ms as separated in time (overlap seen as gap). We present an attention-perception model with a map of prominence in the superior colliculus that modulates the stimulus signal's effectiveness in the action and perception pathways. This common source of modulation determines how competition between stimuli is resolved, causes the TA dependence of action and perception to be the same, and causes the misperception.
Collapse
|
17
|
Valuch C, Pflüger LS, Wallner B, Laeng B, Ansorge U. Using eye tracking to test for individual differences in attention to attractive faces. Front Psychol 2015; 6:42. [PMID: 25698993 PMCID: PMC4313586 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00042] [Citation(s) in RCA: 33] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/26/2014] [Accepted: 01/09/2015] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
We assessed individual differences in visual attention toward faces in relation to their attractiveness via saccadic reaction times. Motivated by the aim to understand individual differences in attention to faces, we tested three hypotheses: (a) Attractive faces hold or capture attention more effectively than less attractive faces; (b) men show a stronger bias toward attractive opposite-sex faces than women; and (c) blue-eyed men show a stronger bias toward blue-eyed than brown-eyed feminine faces. The latter test was included because prior research suggested a high effect size. Our data supported hypotheses (a) and (b) but not (c). By conducting separate tests for disengagement of attention and attention capture, we found that individual differences exist at distinct stages of attentional processing but these differences are of varying robustness and importance. In our conclusion, we also advocate the use of linear mixed effects models as the most appropriate statistical approach for studying inter-individual differences in visual attention with naturalistic stimuli.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Christian Valuch
- Cognitive Science Research Platform, University of Vienna Vienna, Austria ; Department of Basic Psychological Research and Research Methods, Faculty of Psychology, University of Vienna Vienna, Austria
| | - Lena S Pflüger
- Cognitive Science Research Platform, University of Vienna Vienna, Austria ; Department of Anthropology, Faculty of Life Sciences, University of Vienna Vienna, Austria
| | - Bernard Wallner
- Department of Anthropology, Faculty of Life Sciences, University of Vienna Vienna, Austria ; Department of Behavioural Biology, Faculty of Life Sciences, University of Vienna Vienna, Austria
| | - Bruno Laeng
- Department of Psychology, Faculty of Social Sciences, University of Oslo Oslo, Norway
| | - Ulrich Ansorge
- Department of Basic Psychological Research and Research Methods, Faculty of Psychology, University of Vienna Vienna, Austria
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
The influence of attention and target identification on saccadic eye movements depends on prior target location. J Ophthalmol 2014; 2014:850606. [PMID: 24719754 PMCID: PMC3955594 DOI: 10.1155/2014/850606] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/11/2013] [Revised: 12/17/2013] [Accepted: 01/06/2014] [Indexed: 11/17/2022] Open
Abstract
Saccadic latency is reduced by a temporal gap between fixation point and target, by identification of a target feature, and by movement in a new direction (inhibition of saccadic return, ISR). A simple additive model was compared with a shared resources model that predicts a three-way interaction. Twenty naïve participants made horizontal saccades to targets left and right of fixation in a randomised block design. There was a significant three-way interaction among the factors on saccade latency. This was revealed in a two-way interaction between feature identification and the gap versus no gap factor which was only apparent when the saccade was in the same direction as the previous saccade. No interaction was apparent when the saccade was in the opposite direction. This result supports an attentional inhibitory effect that is present during ISR to a previous location which is only partly released by the facilitative effect of feature identification and gap. Together, anticipatory error data and saccade latency interactions suggest a source of ISR at a higher level of attention, possibly localised in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and involving tonic activation.
Collapse
|
19
|
Effects of direct and averted gaze on the subsequent saccadic response. Atten Percept Psychophys 2014; 76:1085-92. [DOI: 10.3758/s13414-014-0660-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
|
20
|
Tajik-Parvinchi DJ, Sandor P. Enhanced antisaccade abilities in children with Tourette syndrome: the Gap-effect Reversal. Front Hum Neurosci 2013; 7:768. [PMID: 24312038 PMCID: PMC3826111 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00768] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/08/2013] [Accepted: 10/25/2013] [Indexed: 11/25/2022] Open
Abstract
Tourette Syndrome (TS) is a childhood onset disorder of motor and vocal tics. The neural networks underlying TS overlap with those of saccade eye movements. Thus, deviations on saccadic tasks can provide important information about psychopathology of TS. Tourette syndrome often coexists with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and Obsessive Compulsive Disorder (OCD). Hence, we manipulated various components of a saccade task to measure its effects on saccades of children with TS-only, TS+ADHD, TS+ADHD+OCD and healthy controls. Children looked toward (prosaccade) or in the opposite direction (antisaccade) of a peripheral target as soon as it appeared. The prosaccade and antisaccade tasks were presented in three conditions. In the Gap200 condition, the fixation dot disappeared 200 ms prior to the appearance of the peripheral target, In the Gap800 condition, the fixation dot disappeared 800 ms prior to the appearance of the peripheral target and in Overlap200 the fixation dot disappeared 200 ms after the appearance of the peripheral target. Fixation-offset manipulations had different effects on each group's antisaccades. The TS+ADHD+OCD group's antisaccade latencies and error rates remained relatively unchanged in the three conditions and displayed a pattern of eye movements that can be interpreted as enhanced. Alternatively, the TS+ADHD group displayed an overall pattern of longer saccadic latencies. Findings corroborate the hypothesis that the combination of tic disorder and ADHD results in unique behavioral profiles. It is plausible that a subgroup of children with TS develop an adaptive ability to control their tics which generalizes to enhanced volitional control of saccadic behavior as well. Supporting evidence and other findings are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Diana J Tajik-Parvinchi
- Department of Psychology, Centre for Vision Research, York University Toronto, ON, Canada ; Tourette Syndrome Neurodevelopmental Clinic and Toronto Western Research Institute, University Health Network Toronto, ON, Canada
| | | |
Collapse
|
21
|
Ueda H, Takahashi K, Watanabe K. Influence of removal of invisible fixation on the saccadic and manual gap effect. Exp Brain Res 2013; 232:329-36. [DOI: 10.1007/s00221-013-3743-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/24/2013] [Accepted: 10/11/2013] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
|
22
|
Remillard G. The study of sequence learning in individuals with schizophrenia: a critical review of the literature. J Neuropsychol 2013; 8:231-45. [PMID: 23714117 DOI: 10.1111/jnp.12022] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/19/2012] [Revised: 02/20/2013] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
The serial reaction time task (SRTT) has been used extensively to study implicit sequence learning. A number of studies have used the SRTT to examine sequence learning in schizophrenia patients. Despite these studies, it remains unclear whether sequence learning is impaired in patients, whether antipsychotic medications affect sequence learning, and what types of sequential information patients might have difficulty learning. Methodological limitations have made it difficult to obtain good answers to these questions. Methodological innovations from the general SRTT literature that have not yet been adopted in the schizophrenia literature could provide better answers.
Collapse
|
23
|
Ueda H, Takahashi K, Watanabe K. Contributions of retinal input and phenomenal representation of a fixation object to the saccadic gap effect. Vision Res 2013; 82:52-7. [DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2013.02.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/10/2012] [Revised: 02/06/2013] [Accepted: 02/11/2013] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
|
24
|
Jin Z, Reeves A, Watamaniuk SNJ, Heinen SJ. Shared attention for smooth pursuit and saccades. J Vis 2013; 13:7. [PMID: 23487159 PMCID: PMC3598381 DOI: 10.1167/13.4.7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/25/2012] [Accepted: 01/09/2013] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
Identification of brief luminance decrements on parafoveal stimuli presented during smooth pursuit improves when a spot pursuit target is surrounded by a larger random dot cinematogram (RDC) that moves with it (Heinen, Jin, & Watamaniuk, 2011). This was hypothesized to occur because the RDC provided an alternative, less attention-demanding pursuit drive, and therefore released attentional resources for visual perception tasks that are shared with those used to pursue the spot. Here, we used the RDC as a tool to probe whether spot pursuit also shares attentional resources with the saccadic system. To this end, we set out to determine if the RDC could release attention from pursuit of the spot to perform a saccade task. Observers made a saccade to one of four parafoveal targets that moved with the spot pursuit stimulus. The targets either moved alone or were surrounded by an RDC (100% coherence). Saccade latency decreased with the RDC, suggesting that the RDC released attention needed to pursue the spot, which was then used for the saccade task. Additional evidence that attention was released by the RDC was obtained in an experiment in which attention was anchored to the fovea by requiring observers to detect a brief color change applied 130 ms before the saccade target appeared. This manipulation eliminated the RDC advantage. The results imply that attentional resources used by the pursuit and saccadic eye movement control systems are shared.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Zhenlan Jin
- The Smith-Kettlewell Eye Research Institute, San Francisco, CA
- Present Address: School of Life Sciences and Technology, University of Electronic Science and Technology of China, Chengdu, China
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
25
|
Eye-Hand Coordination in Children with High Functioning Autism and Asperger’s Disorder Using a Gap-Overlap Paradigm. J Autism Dev Disord 2012; 43:841-50. [DOI: 10.1007/s10803-012-1623-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/28/2022]
|
26
|
Attentional capture decreases when distractors remain visible during rapid serial visual presentations. Atten Percept Psychophys 2010; 72:939-50. [DOI: 10.3758/app.72.4.939] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
|
27
|
Huestegge L, Koch I. Fixation disengagement enhances peripheral perceptual processing: evidence for a perceptual gap effect. Exp Brain Res 2009; 201:631-40. [DOI: 10.1007/s00221-009-2080-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/24/2009] [Accepted: 10/30/2009] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
|
28
|
Jin Z, Reeves A. Attentional release in the saccadic gap effect. Vision Res 2009; 49:2045-55. [DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2009.02.015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 34] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/11/2008] [Revised: 02/21/2009] [Accepted: 02/24/2009] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
|
29
|
Souto D, Kerzel D. Evidence for an attentional component in saccadic inhibition of return. Exp Brain Res 2009; 195:531-40. [PMID: 19424686 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-009-1824-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/14/2008] [Accepted: 04/18/2009] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
After presentation of a peripheral cue, facilitation at the cued location is followed by inhibition of return (IOR). It has been recently proposed that IOR may originate at different processing stages for manual and ocular responses, with manual IOR resulting from inhibited attentional orienting, and ocular IOR resulting form inhibited motor preparation. Contrary to this interpretation, we found an effect of target contrast on saccadic IOR. The effect of contrast decreased with increasing reaction times (RTs) for saccades, but not for manual key-press responses. This may have masked the effect of contrast on IOR with saccades in previous studies (Hunt and Kingstone in J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 29:1068-1074, 2003) because only mean RTs were considered. We also found that background luminance strongly influenced the effects of gap and target contrast on IOR.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- David Souto
- Faculté de Psychologie et Sciences de l'Education, Université de Genève, Geneva, Switzerland.
| | | |
Collapse
|
30
|
Yang Q, Lê TT, Debay E, Orssaud C, Magnier G, Kapoula Z. Rare express saccades in elderly fallers. Clin Interv Aging 2009; 3:691-8. [PMID: 19281061 PMCID: PMC2682401 DOI: 10.2147/cia.s2431] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/23/2022] Open
Abstract
OBJECTIVE To examine horizontal saccades in elderly subjects with falling history; prior extensive screening was done to recruit subjects with falling history in the absence of pathology. METHODS Twelve elderly with falling history were tested. Two testing conditions were used: the gap (fixation target extinguishes prior to target onset) and the overlap (fixation stays on after target onset) paradigms. Each condition was run at three viewing distances--20 cm, 40 cm, and 150 cm, corresponding to convergence angle at 17.1 degrees, 8.6 degrees, and 2.3 degrees, respectively. Eye movements were recorded with the photoelectric IRIS (Skalar medical). RESULTS (i) like in healthy elderly subjects, elderly with falling history produce shorter latencies in the gap paradigm than in the overlap paradigm; (ii) their latencies are shorter at near distances (20 and 40 cm) relative to 150 cm for both paradigms; (iii) the novel result is that they fail to produce express latencies even in the conditions (near viewing distance and the gap task) known to promote high rates of express in adults (25%) or in healthy elderly (20%). Seven from the 10 healthy elderly produced express saccades at rates >12%, while 9 of the 12 older subjects with falling history showed no express saccades at all; the remaining 3 subjects showed low rates <12%. CONCLUSION The quasi paucity of express saccades could be due to the disequilibrium of complex cortical/subcortical networks needed for making express saccades. The results support models suggesting specific network for express saccades; missing of such optomotor reflex may go along with missing other reflexes as well increasing the chances of falling.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Q Yang
- Groupe IRIS Vision and Motricité Binoculaire, CNRS, Service d'Ophtalmologie-ORL-Stomatologie, Hôpital Européen Georges Pompidou, 20 rue Leblanc, 75908, Paris, France.
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
31
|
Abstract
The saccadic eye movement system provides researchers with a powerful tool with which to explore the cognitive control of behaviour. It is a behavioural system whose limited output can be measured with exceptional precision, and whose input can be controlled and manipulated in subtle ways. A range of cognitive processes (notably those involved in working memory and attention) have been shown to influence saccade parameters. Researchers interested in the relationship between cognitive function and psychiatric disorders have made extensive use of saccadic eye movement tasks to draw inferences as to the cognitive deficits associated with particular psychopathologies. The purpose of this review is to provide researchers with an overview of the research literature documenting cognitive involvement in saccadic tasks in healthy controls. An appreciation of this literature provides a solid background against which to interpret the deficits on saccadic tasks demonstrated in patient populations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- S B Hutton
- Department of Psychology, University of Sussex, Pevensey 1 1c03, Brighton BN1 9RH, UK.
| |
Collapse
|
32
|
Anderson AJ, Carpenter RHS. The effect of stimuli that isolate S-cones on early saccades and the gap effect. Proc Biol Sci 2008; 275:335-44. [PMID: 18055389 DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2007.1394] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Disappearance of the fixation spot before the appearance of a peripheral target typically reduces average saccadic reaction times (the gap effect) and may also produce a separate population of early or express saccades. The superior colliculus (SC) is generally believed to be critically involved in generating both effects. As the direct sensory input to the SC does not encode colour information, to determine whether this input was critical in generating the gap effect or express saccades we used coloured targets which this pathway cannot distinguish. Our observers still made early saccades to colour-defined targets, but these were anticipations in response to the offset of the non-coloured fixation target. We also show that a gap effect still occurs when either the fixation target or the peripheral target is colour defined, suggesting that direct sensory input to the SC is not required and that information about the location of colour-defined targets is abstracted prior to processing within the SC.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- A J Anderson
- Department of Optometry and Vision Sciences, The University of Melbourne, Victoria 3010, Australia.
| | | |
Collapse
|
33
|
The presence of visual gap affects the duration of stopping process. Exp Brain Res 2008; 192:199-209. [PMID: 18797854 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-008-1570-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/05/2008] [Accepted: 09/03/2008] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
Abstract
A milestone on which relies the voluntary control of behavior is the ability to shape our motor output to meet the needs of the context which we are continuously facing. Even though it is solidly established that contextual information influence movement generation few studies have so far explored their effects on inhibitory processes. We compared the inhibitory control of arm movements of ten healthy right-handed volunteers in a countermanding reaching paradigm with and without the presence of a temporal gap between the offset of the central target and the peripheral target appearance. We found that this perceptual gap reduces the reaction times of hand movements and, at the same time, increases the duration of the stop process, the stop signal reaction time. The two effects are not correlated implying that inhibition and execution of reaching movement are two independent processes influenced by a common factor: the disengagement of selective attention from the central target. Therefore our results support the idea of the existence of a link between spatial selective attention and inhibitory processes.
Collapse
|
34
|
Latency of saccades and vergence eye movements in dyslexic children. Exp Brain Res 2008; 188:1-12. [DOI: 10.1007/s00221-008-1345-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 35] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/17/2007] [Accepted: 02/20/2008] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
|
35
|
Fehd HM, Seiffert AE. Eye movements during multiple object tracking: where do participants look? Cognition 2007; 108:201-9. [PMID: 18096148 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.11.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 100] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/22/2006] [Revised: 10/22/2007] [Accepted: 11/05/2007] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
Abstract
Similar to the eye movements you might make when viewing a sports game, this experiment investigated where participants tend to look while keeping track of multiple objects. While eye movements were recorded, participants tracked either 1 or 3 of 8 red dots that moved randomly within a square box on a black background. Results indicated that participants fixated closer to targets more often than to distractors. However, on 3-target trials, fixation was closer to the center of the triangle formed by the targets more often than to any individual targets. This center-looking strategy seemed to reflect that people were grouping the targets into a single object rather than simultaneously minimizing all target eccentricities. Here we find that observers deliberately focus their eyes on a location that is different from the objects they are attending, perhaps as a consequence of representing those objects as a group.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hilda M Fehd
- Vanderbilt University, Department of Psychology, 111 21st Avenue South, Nashville 37203, United States.
| | | |
Collapse
|