1
|
Ceple I, Svede A, Serpa E, Kassaliete E, Volberga L, Mikelsone R, Berzina A, Ganebnaya A, Krauze L, Krumina G. The Prevalence of Accommodative and Binocular Dysfunctions in Children with Reading Difficulties. Life (Basel) 2024; 15:7. [PMID: 39859947 PMCID: PMC11766850 DOI: 10.3390/life15010007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/12/2024] [Revised: 12/22/2024] [Accepted: 12/23/2024] [Indexed: 01/27/2025] Open
Abstract
Uncorrected refractive error and unsatisfactory performance on several clinical accommodation and binocular vision tests are more common in children who struggle with reading. The aim of the current study is to explore the prevalence of accommodative and binocular dysfunctions in children with and without reading difficulties. Reading performance was assessed with the Acadience Reading (formerly DIBELS Next) test adjusted and validated for the Latvian language. Children with (N = 39) and without (N = 43) reading difficulties underwent thorough assessment of their subjective refraction, as well as binocular and accommodation functions. The results demonstrate no difference in the prevalence of complaints between children with and without reading difficulties (26% and 23%, respectively). However, children with reading difficulties more frequently present with significant uncorrected refractive errors and/or accommodative and binocular dysfunctions than children without reading difficulties (69% and 47%, respectively). According to the findings, even in cases where a child does not exhibit any ocular or visual complaints, a comprehensive visual function evaluation should be required for any child who struggles with reading.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ilze Ceple
- Department of Optometry and Vision Science, Faculty of Science and Technology, University of Latvia, Jelgavas Street 1, LV-1004 Riga, Latvia; (A.S.); (E.S.); (E.K.); (A.G.); (L.K.); (G.K.)
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
2
|
Bartolomeo P, Liu J, Spagna A. Colors in the mind's eye. Cortex 2024; 170:26-31. [PMID: 37926612 DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2023.10.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/28/2023] [Revised: 10/04/2023] [Accepted: 10/05/2023] [Indexed: 11/07/2023]
Abstract
The famous "Piazza del Duomo" paper, published in Cortex in 1978, inspired a considerable amount of research on visual mental imagery in brain-damaged patients. As a consequence, single-case reports featuring dissociations between perceptual and imagery abilities challenged the prevailing model of visual mental imagery. Here we focus on mental imagery for colors. A case study published in Cortex showed perfectly preserved color imagery in a patient with acquired achromatopsia after bilateral lesions at the borders between the occipital and temporal cortex. Subsequent neuroimaging findings in healthy participants extended and specified this result; color imagery elicited activation in both a domain-general region located in the left fusiform gyrus and the anterior color-biased patch within the ventral temporal cortex, but not in more posterior color-biased patches. Detailed studies of individual neurological patients, as those often published in Cortex, are still critical to inspire and constrain neurocognitive research and its theoretical models.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Paolo Bartolomeo
- Sorbonne Université, Institut du Cerveau - Paris Brain Institute - ICM, Inserm, CNRS, AP-HP, Hôpital de la Pitié-Salpêtrière, Paris, France.
| | - Jianghao Liu
- Sorbonne Université, Institut du Cerveau - Paris Brain Institute - ICM, Inserm, CNRS, AP-HP, Hôpital de la Pitié-Salpêtrière, Paris, France; Corporate Research, Dassault Systèmes, Vélizy-Villacoublay, France
| | - Alfredo Spagna
- Department of Psychology, Columbia University in the City of New York, NY, USA
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Chokron S, Dutton GN. From vision to cognition: potential contributions of cerebral visual impairment to neurodevelopmental disorders. J Neural Transm (Vienna) 2023; 130:409-424. [PMID: 36547695 DOI: 10.1007/s00702-022-02572-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/16/2022] [Accepted: 11/28/2022] [Indexed: 12/24/2022]
Abstract
Vision has a crucial role to play in human development and functioning. It is, therefore, not surprising that vision plays a fundamental role in the development of the child. As a consequence, an alteration in visual function is, therefore, likely to hinder the child's development. Although ocular disorders are well known, diagnosed and taken into account, cerebral visual impairments (CVI) resulting from post-chiasmatic damage are largely underdiagnosed. However, among the disorders resulting from an episode of perinatal asphyxia and/or associated with prematurity, or neonatal hypoglycaemia, CVIs are prominent. In this article, we focus on the role of the possible effects of CVI on a child's learning abilities, leading to major difficulty in disentangling the consequences of CVI from other neurodevelopmental disorders (NDD) such as dyslexia, dyscalculia, dysgraphia, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), developmental coordination disorder (DCD) and autism spectrum disorders (ASD). Although we focus here on the possible overlap between children with CVI and children with other NDD, De Witt et al. (Wit et al. Ear Hear 39:1-19, 2018) have raised exactly the same question regarding children with auditory processing disorders (the equivalent of CVI in the auditory modality). We underline how motor, social and cognitive development as well as academic success can be impaired by CVI and raise the question of the need for systematic evaluation for disorders of vision, visual perception and cognition in all children presenting with a NDD and/or previously born under adverse neurological conditions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sylvie Chokron
- INCC, CNRS, UMR8002, Université de Paris-Cité, Paris, France.
- Institut de Neuropsychologie, Neurovision et Neurocognition, Hôpital-Fondation A. de Rothschild, Paris, France.
| | - Gordon N Dutton
- Department of Vision Science, Glasgow Caledonian University, Glasgow, UK
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Chokron S, Kovarski K, Zalla T, Dutton G. The inter-relationships between cerebral visual impairment, autism and intellectual disability. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2020; 114:201-210. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.04.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/18/2019] [Revised: 01/13/2020] [Accepted: 04/06/2020] [Indexed: 12/13/2022]
|
5
|
Chen P, Motisi SE, Cording C, Ward I, Jasey NN. Impact of eliminating visual input on sitting posture and head position in a patient with spatial neglect following cerebral hemorrhage: a case report. Physiother Theory Pract 2019; 37:852-861. [PMID: 31319732 DOI: 10.1080/09593985.2019.1645252] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/26/2022]
Abstract
Background: Spatial neglect is a neurocognitive syndrome. Affected individuals pay little or insufficient attention to the space contralateral to the injured cerebral hemisphere, often resulting in or exacerbating disability following an acquired brain injury. Eliminating visual input may increase attention toward the contralesional side of space, and improve symptoms of spatial neglect; however this has never been examined in a clinical setting. Objective: In this case report, we observed an individual demonstrate immediate and spontaneous postural changes once visual input was eliminated. Methods: The patient, a 53-year-old female, was admitted to a rehabilitation hospital after hemorrhagic stroke affecting her right basal ganglia and surrounding regions in the frontal lobe. She exhibited left-sided spasticity, severe right gaze preference, and stark rightward postural deviation. Neck passive range of motion was normal. Visual field integrity was inconclusive due to poor communication and impaired cognitive function. Contraversive pushing was ruled out. Results:Once visual input was eliminated by applying a blindfold, the patient turned to the left spontaneously, had more buttock contact on the left, and placed more weight toward the left side in a sitting posture. However, she returned to rightward deviation three minutes after blindfold removal. In addition, the patient's rehabilitation team reported that she was able to participate in more therapy activities with binocular occlusion than with eyes open. Conclusion: Binocular occlusion appeared to demonstrate an immediate, albeit transient, improvement in postural symmetry. The results warrant further research and exploration in clinical applicability.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Peii Chen
- Center for Stroke Rehabilitation Research, Kessler Foundation, West Orange, NJ, USA.,Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Rutgers University - New Jersey Medical School, Newark, NJ, USA
| | | | | | - Irene Ward
- Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Rutgers University - New Jersey Medical School, Newark, NJ, USA.,Kessler Institute for Rehabilitation, West Orange, NJ, USA
| | - Neil N Jasey
- Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Rutgers University - New Jersey Medical School, Newark, NJ, USA.,Kessler Institute for Rehabilitation, West Orange, NJ, USA
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Chokron S, Peyrin C, Perez C. Ipsilesional deficit of selective attention in left homonymous hemianopia and left unilateral spatial neglect. Neuropsychologia 2019; 128:305-314. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.03.013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/26/2017] [Revised: 03/06/2018] [Accepted: 03/08/2018] [Indexed: 12/23/2022]
|
7
|
Toba MN, Rabuffetti M, Duret C, Pradat-Diehl P, Gainotti G, Bartolomeo P. Component deficits of visual neglect: “Magnetic” attraction of attention vs. impaired spatial working memory. Neuropsychologia 2018; 109:52-62. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.11.034] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/28/2017] [Revised: 11/07/2017] [Accepted: 11/29/2017] [Indexed: 12/18/2022]
|
8
|
Complexity vs. unity in unilateral spatial neglect. Rev Neurol (Paris) 2017; 173:440-450. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neurol.2017.07.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/14/2017] [Revised: 07/20/2017] [Accepted: 07/21/2017] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
|
9
|
Perception of active head rotation in patients with severe left unilateral spatial neglect. J Clin Neurosci 2017; 41:41-45. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jocn.2017.01.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/14/2016] [Accepted: 01/22/2017] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
|
10
|
Jane Moore M, Demeyere N. Neglect Dyslexia in Relation to Unilateral Visuospatial Neglect: A Review. AIMS Neurosci 2017. [DOI: 10.3934/neuroscience.2017.4.169] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
|
11
|
Jane Moore M, Demeyere N. Neglect Dyslexia in Relation to Unilateral Visuospatial Neglect: A Review. AIMS Neurosci 2017. [DOI: 10.3934/neuroscience.2017.4.148] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
|
12
|
Chokron S, Dutton GN. Impact of Cerebral Visual Impairments on Motor Skills: Implications for Developmental Coordination Disorders. Front Psychol 2016; 7:1471. [PMID: 27757087 PMCID: PMC5048540 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01471] [Citation(s) in RCA: 41] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/23/2016] [Accepted: 09/13/2016] [Indexed: 12/16/2022] Open
Abstract
Cerebral visual impairment (CVI) has become the primary cause of visual impairment and blindness in children in industrialized countries. Its prevalence has increased sharply, due to increased survival rates of children who sustain severe neurological conditions during the perinatal period. Improved diagnosis has probably contributed to this increase. As in adults, the nature and severity of CVI in children relate to the cause, location and extent of damage to the brain. In the present paper, we define CVI and how this impacts on visual function. We then define developmental coordination disorder (DCD) and discuss the link between CVI and DCD. The neuroanatomical correlates and aetiologies of DCD are also presented in relationship with CVI as well as the consequences of perinatal asphyxia (PA) and preterm birth on the occurrence and nature of DCD and CVI. This paper underlines why there are both clinical and theoretical reasons to disentangle CVI and DCD, and to categorize the features with more precision. In order to offer the most appropriate rehabilitation, we propose a systematic and rapid evaluation of visual function in at-risk children who have survived preterm birth or PA whether or not they have been diagnosed with cerebral palsy or DCD.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sylvie Chokron
- Unité Fonctionnelle Vision and Cognition, Fondation Ophtalmologique RothschildParis, France; Laboratoire de Psychologie de la Perception, UMR 8242, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique - Université Paris-DescartesParis, France
| | - Gordon N Dutton
- Department of Vision Science, Glasgow Caledonian University Glasgow, UK
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Semiology of neglect: An update. Ann Phys Rehabil Med 2016; 60:177-185. [PMID: 27103056 DOI: 10.1016/j.rehab.2016.03.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 71] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/27/2015] [Revised: 03/21/2016] [Accepted: 03/21/2016] [Indexed: 11/21/2022]
Abstract
Hemispatial neglect is a common disabling condition following brain damage to the right hemisphere. Generally, it involves behavioral bias directed ipsilaterally to the damaged hemisphere and loss of spatial awareness for the contralesional side. In this syndrome, several clinical subtypes were identified. The objective of this article is to provide a nosological analysis of the recent data from the literature on the different subtypes of neglect (visual, auditory, somatosensory, motor, egocentric, allocentric and representational neglect), associated ipsilesional and contralesional productive manifestations and their anatomical lesion correlates. These different anatomical-clinical subtypes can be associated or dissociated. They reflect the heterogeneity of this unilateral neglect syndrome that cannot be approached or interpreted in a single manner. We propose that these subtypes result from different underlying deficits: exogenous attentional deficit (visual, auditory neglect); representational deficit (personal neglect, representational neglect, hyperschematia); shift of the egocentric reference frame (egocentric neglect); attentional deficit between objects and within objects (allocentric neglect), endogenous attentional deficit (representational neglect) and transsaccadic working memory or spatial remapping deficit (ipsilesional productive manifestations). Taking into account the different facets of the unilateral neglect syndrome should promote the development of more targeted cognitive rehabilitation protocols.
Collapse
|
14
|
Wansard M, Meulemans T, Geurten M. Shedding new light on representational neglect: The importance of dissociating visual and spatial components. Neuropsychologia 2016; 84:150-7. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.02.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/16/2015] [Revised: 12/29/2015] [Accepted: 02/14/2016] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
|
15
|
Fourtassi M, Rode G, Pisella L. Using eye movements to explore mental representations of space. Ann Phys Rehabil Med 2016; 60:160-163. [PMID: 27038772 DOI: 10.1016/j.rehab.2016.03.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/31/2015] [Revised: 03/08/2016] [Accepted: 03/08/2016] [Indexed: 01/12/2023]
Abstract
Visual mental imagery is a cognitive experience characterised by the activation of the mental representation of an object or scene in the absence of the corresponding stimulus. According to the analogical theory, mental representations have a pictorial nature that preserves the spatial characteristics of the environment that is mentally represented. This cognitive experience shares many similarities with the experience of visual perception, including eye movements. The mental visualisation of a scene is accompanied by eye movements that reflect the spatial content of the mental image, and which can mirror the deformations of this mental image with respect to the real image, such as asymmetries or size reduction. The present article offers a concise overview of the main theories explaining the interactions between eye movements and mental representations, with some examples of the studies supporting them. It also aims to explain how ocular-tracking could be a useful tool in exploring the dynamics of spatial mental representations, especially in pathological situations where these representations can be altered, for instance in unilateral spatial neglect.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Maryam Fourtassi
- Inserm, U1028, CNRS, UMR5292, Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, ImpAct, 16, avenue du Doyen-Lépine, 69676 Bron cedex, France; Université Mohamed Premier, Faculté de médecine et de Pharmacie d'Oujda, BP 724 Hay Al Quods, 60000 Oujda, Morocco.
| | - Gilles Rode
- Inserm, U1028, CNRS, UMR5292, Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, ImpAct, 16, avenue du Doyen-Lépine, 69676 Bron cedex, France; Université Claude-Bernard Lyon 1, 69100 Villeurbanne, France; Hospices Civils de Lyon, Hôpital Henry-Gabrielle, Mouvement et Handicap, 69000 Lyon, France
| | - Laure Pisella
- Inserm, U1028, CNRS, UMR5292, Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, ImpAct, 16, avenue du Doyen-Lépine, 69676 Bron cedex, France; Université Claude-Bernard Lyon 1, 69100 Villeurbanne, France
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Tosun S, Vaid J. What Affects Facing Direction in Human Facial Profile Drawing? A Meta-Analytic Inquiry. Perception 2014; 43:1377-92. [DOI: 10.1068/p7805] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
Abstract
Two meta-analyses were conducted to examine two potential sources of spatial orientation biases in human profile drawings by brain-intact individuals. The first examined profile facing direction as function of hand used to draw. The second examined profile facing direction in relation to directional scanning biases related to reading/writing habits. Results of the first meta-analysis, based on 27 study samples with 4171 participants, showed that leftward facing of profiles (from the viewer's perspective) was significantly associated with using the right hand to draw. The reading/writing direction meta-analysis, based on 10 study samples with 1552 participants, suggested a modest relationship between leftward profile facing and primary use of a left-to-right reading/writing direction. These findings suggest that biomechanical and cultural factors jointly influence hand movement preferences and in turn the direction of facing of human profile drawings.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sümeyra Tosun
- Department of Psychology, Süleyman Şah University, Istanbul, Turkey
| | - Jyotsna Vaid
- Department of Psychology, Texas A&M University, College Station, TX 77843-4235, USA
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
van Dijck JP, Gevers W, Lafosse C, Fias W. Right-sided representational neglect after left brain damage in a case without visuospatial working memory deficits. Cortex 2013; 49:2283-93. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2013.03.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/01/2012] [Revised: 12/01/2012] [Accepted: 03/17/2013] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
|
18
|
Chen P, Goedert KM. Clock drawing in spatial neglect: a comprehensive analysis of clock perimeter, placement, and accuracy. J Neuropsychol 2012; 6:270-89. [PMID: 22390278 PMCID: PMC3371137 DOI: 10.1111/j.1748-6653.2012.02028.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
Clock drawings produced by right-brain-damaged (RBD) individuals with spatial neglect often contain an abundance of empty space on the left while numbers and hands are placed on the right. However, the clock perimeter is rarely compromised in neglect patients' drawings. By analysing clock drawings produced by 71 RBD and 40 healthy adults, this study investigated whether the geometric characteristics of the clock perimeter reveal novel insights to understanding spatial neglect. Neglect participants drew smaller clocks than either healthy or non-neglect RBD participants. While healthy participants' clock perimeter was close to circular, RBD participants drew radially extended ellipses. The mechanisms for these phenomena were investigated by examining the relation between clock-drawing characteristics and performance on six subtests of the Behavioral Inattention Test (BIT). The findings indicated that the clock shape was independent of any BIT subtest or the drawing placement on the test sheet and that the clock size was significantly predicted by one BIT subtest: the poorer the figure and shape copying, the smaller the clock perimeter. Further analyses revealed that in all participants, clocks decreased in size as they were placed farther from the centre of the paper. However, even when neglect participants placed their clocks towards the centre of the page, they were smaller than those produced by healthy or non-neglect RBD participants. These results suggest a neglect-specific reduction in the subjectively available workspace for graphic production from memory, consistent with the hypothesis that neglect patients are impaired in the ability to enlarge the attentional aperture.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Peii Chen
- Kessler Foundation Research Center, West Orange, New Jersey 07052, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
19
|
Di Russo F, Bozzacchi C, Matano A, Spinelli D. Hemispheric differences in VEPs to lateralised stimuli are a marker of recovery from neglect. Cortex 2012; 49:931-9. [PMID: 22664139 DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.04.017] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/28/2011] [Revised: 04/01/2012] [Accepted: 04/23/2012] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
Visual-evoked potentials (VEPs) were recorded in seventeen patients with unilateral lesions of the right hemisphere (RH) and visuospatial neglect. Hemispheric differences were detected for VEP components in the time window from 130 to 280 msec; this result replicates data from a previous study using a larger group of patients (Di Russo et al., 2008). Three patients were tested twice; their hemispheric differences, i.e., the differences in latency and amplitude of VEPs to ipsilesional and contralesional stimuli, were evaluated at the beginning and end of visuospatial rehabilitation training for neglect. The hemispheric differences were limited to components anterior N1 (N1a), posterior N1 (N1p) and P2 (not C1 and P1) and showed a significant decrease after training; amelioration at the behavioural level was also observed. Fourteen patients were tested only once, at different steps of their training. For the overall group, we determined the correlation between VEP hemispheric differences and the number of sessions attended by the patients at the time of VEP recording. The correlation was negative, the higher the number of sessions, the lower the hemispheric asymmetry, and high, ranging from .45 to .64, for both the latency and amplitude of the N1p and P2 components, and for the amplitude of the N1a component. The correlation between VEP hemispheric differences and time from onset (TFO) of the pathological event was not significant. Overall, the hemispheric differences between specific components of the VEP responses to lateralised stimuli appear to be a good marker of recovery from neglect.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Francesco Di Russo
- Department of Education in Sports and Human Movement, University of Rome, Foro Italico, Rome, Italy.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
20
|
Bartolomeo P, Thiebaut de Schotten M, Chica AB. Brain networks of visuospatial attention and their disruption in visual neglect. Front Hum Neurosci 2012; 6:110. [PMID: 22586384 PMCID: PMC3343690 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00110] [Citation(s) in RCA: 139] [Impact Index Per Article: 10.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/17/2012] [Accepted: 04/11/2012] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Visual neglect is a multi-component syndrome including prominent attentional disorders. Research on the functional mechanisms of neglect is now moving from the description of dissociations in patients' performance to the identification of the possible component deficits and of their interaction with compensatory strategies. In recent years, the dissection of attentional deficits in neglect has progressed in parallel with increasing comprehension of the anatomy and function of large-scale brain networks implicated in attentional processes. This review focuses on the anatomy and putative functions of attentional circuits in the brain, mainly subserved by fronto-parietal networks, with a peculiar although not yet completely elucidated role for the right hemisphere. Recent results are discussed concerning the influence of a non-spatial attentional function, phasic alertness, on conscious perception in normal participants and on conflict resolution in neglect patients. The rapid rate of expansion of our knowledge of these systems raises hopes for the development of effective strategies to improve the functioning of the attentional networks in brain-damaged patients.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Paolo Bartolomeo
- INSERM - UPMC UMRS 975, Brain and Spine Institute, Groupe Hospitalier Pitié-Salpêtrière Paris, France
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
21
|
Dulin D, Cavezian C, Serrière C, Bachoud-Levi AC, Bartolomeo P, Chokron S. Colour, face, and visuospatial imagery abilities in low-vision individuals with visual field deficits. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 2011; 64:1955-70. [PMID: 21942941 DOI: 10.1080/17470218.2011.608852] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/17/2022]
Abstract
This study investigates to what extent visual perception integrity is necessary for visual mental imagery. Sixteen low-vision participants with severe peripheral visual field loss, 16 with severe central field loss, 6 left brain-damaged patients with right homonymous hemianopia, 6 right brain-damaged patients with left homonymous hemianopia, and 16 normally sighted controls performed perceptual and imagery tasks using colours, faces, and spatial relationships. Results showed that (a) the perceptual and mental image>ry disorders vary according to the type of visual field loss, (b) hemianopics had no more difficulties imagining spatial stimuli in their contralesional hemispace than in their ipsilesional one, and (c) the only hemianopic participant to have perceptual and mental imagery impairments suffered from attentional deficits. Results suggest that (a) visual memory is not definitively established, but rather needs perceptual practice to be maintained, and (b) that visual mental imagery may involve some of the attentional-exploratory mechanisms that are employed in visual behaviour.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- David Dulin
- ERT TREAT VISION, Service de Neurologie, Fondation Ophtalmologique A de Rothschild, 25 rue Manin, Paris, France.
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
22
|
Bourlon C, Oliviero B, Wattiez N, Pouget P, Bartolomeo P. Visual mental imagery: what the head's eye tells the mind's eye. Brain Res 2010; 1367:287-97. [PMID: 20969839 DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.10.039] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/10/2010] [Revised: 10/07/2010] [Accepted: 10/12/2010] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Abstract
The demonstration of an implication of attentional/eye gaze systems in visual mental imagery might help to understand why some patients with visual neglect, who suffer from severe attentional deficits, also show neglect for mental images. When normal participants generate mental images of previously explored visual scenes, their oculomotor behavior resembles that used during visual exploration. However, this could be a case of encoding specificity, whereby the probability of retrieving an event increases if some information encoded with the event (in this case its spatial location) is present at retrieval. In the present study, normal participants were invited to conjure up a mental image of the map of France and to say whether auditorily presented towns or regions were situated left or right of Paris. A perceptual version of the task was administered after the imaginal condition. Thus, in the imaginal condition participants had to retrieve information from long-term memory. Vocal response times and, unbeknownst to participants, also eye movements were recorded. Participants tended to produce similar eye movements on the imaginal and on the perceptual conditions of the task. We concluded that some mechanisms involved in spontaneous oculomotor behavior may be shared in exploration of visuospatial mental images. Deficits of these common processes participating in the oculomotor exploration might contribute to imaginal neglect.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Clémence Bourlon
- Centre de Recherche de l'Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle Epinière, Inserm UMR S975, CNRS 7225, Pavillon Claude Bernard, Hôpital Pitié Salpêtrière (AP-HP), Paris, France.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
23
|
Rode G, Cotton F, Revol P, Jacquin-Courtois S, Rossetti Y, Bartolomeo P. Representation and disconnection in imaginal neglect. Neuropsychologia 2010; 48:2903-11. [PMID: 20621588 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.05.032] [Citation(s) in RCA: 37] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/13/2009] [Revised: 05/01/2010] [Accepted: 05/28/2010] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
Abstract
Patients with neglect failure to detect, orient, or respond to stimuli from a spatially confined region, usually on their left side. Often, the presence of perceptual input increases left omissions, while sensory deprivation decreases them, possibly by removing attention-catching right-sided stimuli (Bartolomeo, 2007). However, such an influence of visual deprivation on representational neglect was not observed in patients while they were imagining a map of France (Rode et al., 2007). Therefore, these patients with imaginal neglect either failed to generate the left side of mental images (Bisiach & Luzzatti, 1978), or suffered from a co-occurrence of deficits in automatic (bottom-up) and voluntary (top-down) orienting of attention. However, in Rode et al.'s experiment visual input was not directly relevant to the task; moreover, distraction from visual input might primarily manifest itself when representation guides somatomotor actions, beyond those involved in the generation and mental exploration of an internal map (Thomas, 1999). To explore these possibilities, we asked a patient with right hemisphere damage, R.D., to explore visual and imagined versions of a map of France in three conditions: (1) 'imagine the map in your mind' (imaginal); (2) 'describe a real map' (visual); and (3) 'list the names of French towns' (propositional). For the imaginal and visual conditions, verbal and manual pointing responses were collected; the task was also given before and after mental rotation of the map by 180 degrees . R.D. mentioned more towns on the right side of the map in the imaginal and visual conditions, but showed no representational deficit in the propositional condition. The rightward inner exploration bias in the imaginal and visual conditions was similar in magnitude and was not influenced by mental rotation or response type (verbal responses or manual pointing to locations on a map), thus suggesting that the representational deficit was robust and independent of perceptual input in R.D. Structural and diffusion MRI demonstrated damage to several white matter tracts in the right hemisphere and to the splenium of corpus callosum. A second right-brain damaged patient (P.P.), who showed signs of visual but not imaginal neglect, had damage to the same intra-hemispheric tracts, but the callosal connections were spared. Imaginal neglect in R.D. may result from fronto-parietal dysfunction impairing orientation towards left-sided items and posterior callosal disconnection preventing the symmetrical processing of spatial information from long-term memory.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- G Rode
- Université de Lyon, Université Lyon 1, INSERM-UMRS 534, Bron, France.
| | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
24
|
Bourlon C, Duret C, Pradat-Diehl P, Azouvi P, Loeper-Jény C, Merat-Blanchard M, Levy C, Chokron S, Bartolomeo P. Vocal response times to real and imagined stimuli in spatial neglect: A group study and single-case report. Cortex 2010; 47:536-46. [PMID: 20451178 DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2010.03.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/17/2009] [Revised: 01/13/2010] [Accepted: 02/19/2010] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
The relationships between spatial neglect for perceptual objects and representational for imagined items are difficult to explore because of several methodological problems, including the dearth of comparable tests for real and imagined scenes. We asked 19 patients with right brain damage and 12 healthy controls to say whether an auditorily presented French geographical location was left or right of Paris, and recorded their vocal response times. Afterwards, participants performed a similar test with visually presented items. Although several patients had asymmetries of performance on the perceptual version of the test, only one patient was more accurate for right-sided than for left-sided imagined stimuli, thus showing evidence for imaginal neglect. However, this patient performed normally on place description and on mental number line bisection, perhaps as a consequence of different strategies he employed for these tasks. Overall, our results confirm previous evidence showing that imaginal neglect is less frequent than, and often occurs in association with, perceptual neglect. Imaginal neglect may result from the contribution of deficits partly distinct from those implicated in perceptual neglect, such as impaired endogenous orienting of attention or deficits of spatial working memory.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Clémence Bourlon
- Service de Rééducation et de Réadaptation Fonctionnelle, Clinique Les Trois Soleils, Boissise le Roi, France.
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
25
|
The role of automatic orienting of attention towards ipsilesional stimuli in non-visual (tactile and auditory) neglect: A critical review. Cortex 2010; 46:150-60. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.04.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/22/2008] [Revised: 02/02/2009] [Accepted: 04/17/2009] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
|
26
|
Peskine A, Urbanski M, Pradat-Diehl P, Bartolomeo P, Azouvi P. Negligenza spaziale unilaterale. Neurologia 2010. [DOI: 10.1016/s1634-7072(10)70492-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/17/2022] Open
|
27
|
Trojano L, Grossi D, Flash T. Cognitive neuroscience of drawing: Contributions of neuropsychological, experimental and neurofunctional studies. Cortex 2009; 45:269-77. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2008.11.015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/06/2008] [Revised: 11/21/2008] [Accepted: 11/21/2008] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
|
28
|
Cristinzio C, Bourlon C, Pradat-Diehl P, Trojano L, Grossi D, Chokron S, Bartolomeo P. Representational neglect in "invisible" drawing from memory. Cortex 2008; 45:313-7. [PMID: 18718580 DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2008.03.013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/14/2007] [Revised: 01/02/2008] [Accepted: 03/11/2008] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
Abstract
We describe the case of a patient with right hemisphere damage and left unilateral neglect. The patient was asked to draw from memory common objects, either with or without visual feedback. In the conditions without visual feedback the patient was either blindfolded or he made "invisible" drawings using a pen with the cap on, the drawings being recorded with carbon paper underneath. Results showed more neglect without than with visual feedback, contrary to previously published cases. This patient's pattern of performance may result from the contribution of a deficit of spatial working memory. Alternatively or in addition, the patient, who was undergoing cognitive rehabilitation for neglect, may have found easier to compensate for his neglect with visual feedback, which allowed him to visually explore the left part of his drawings.
Collapse
|
29
|
Dupierrix E, Alleysson D, Ohlmann T, Chokron S. Spatial bias induced by a non-conflictual task reveals the nature of space perception. Brain Res 2008; 1214:127-35. [DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.01.021] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/08/2008] [Accepted: 01/08/2008] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
|
30
|
La négligence spatiale unilatérale : trente ans de recherches, de découvertes, d’espoirs et (surtout) de questions. Rev Neurol (Paris) 2008; 164 Suppl 3:S134-42. [DOI: 10.1016/s0035-3787(08)73304-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/21/2022]
|
31
|
Rastelli F, Funes MJ, Lupiáñez J, Duret C, Bartolomeo P. Left visual neglect: is the disengage deficit space- or object-based? Exp Brain Res 2008; 187:439-46. [PMID: 18301884 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-008-1316-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/23/2007] [Accepted: 02/11/2008] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
Abstract
Attention can be directed to spatial locations or to objects in space. Patients with left unilateral spatial neglect are slow to respond to a left-sided target when it is preceded by a right-sided "invalid" cue, particularly at short cue-target intervals, suggesting an impairment in disengaging attention from the right side in order to orient it leftward. We wondered whether this deficit is purely spatial, or it is influenced by the presence of a right-sided visual object. To answer this question, we tested 10 right brain-damaged patients with chronic left-neglect and 41 control participants on a cued response time (RT) detection task in which targets could appear in either of two lateral boxes. In different conditions, non-informative peripheral cues either consisted in the brightening of the contour of one lateral box (onset cue condition), or in the complete disappearance of one lateral box (offset cue condition). The target followed the cue at different stimulus-onset asynchronies (SOAs). If the disengagement deficit (DD) is purely space-based, then it should not vary across the two cueing conditions. With onset cues, patients showed a typical DD at short SOAs. With offset cues, however, the DD disappeared. Thus, patients did not show any DD when there was no object from which attention must be disengaged. These findings indicate that the attentional bias in left-neglect does not concern spatial locations per se, but visual objects in space.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Federica Rastelli
- INSERM-UPMC UMRS 610, Pavillon Claude Bernard, Hôpital Salpêtrière, 47 bd de l'Hôpital, 75013, Paris, France.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
32
|
Chapter 19 Visuospatial and visuoconstructive deficits. HANDBOOK OF CLINICAL NEUROLOGY 2008; 88:373-91. [DOI: 10.1016/s0072-9752(07)88019-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/14/2022]
|
33
|
Abstract
PURPOSE OF REVIEW Left visual neglect is a frequent and dramatic consequence of right hemisphere lesions. Diagnosis is important because behavioural and pharmacological treatments are available. Furthermore, neglect raises important issues concerning the brain mechanisms of consciousness, perception and attention. RECENT FINDINGS Recent behavioural findings and new techniques, such as transcranial magnetic stimulation, direct cortical and subcortical stimulation during brain surgery, and diffusion tensor imaging tractography, have provided evidence relevant to the debate concerning the functional mechanisms and the anatomical bases of neglect. SUMMARY Several component deficits appear to interact in producing different forms of neglect. Rather than lesions at single cortical levels, dysfunction of large-scale brain networks, often induced by white matter disconnection, may constitute the crucial antecedent of neglect signs.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Paolo Bartolomeo
- Inserm Unit 610 and Federation of Neurology, Salpêtrière Hospital, University Pierre and Marie Curie - Paris 6, Paris, France.
| |
Collapse
|
34
|
Urbanski M, Bartolomeo P. Line bisection in left neglect: the importance of starting right. Cortex 2007; 44:782-93. [PMID: 18489959 DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2007.04.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 48] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/26/2006] [Revised: 12/23/2006] [Accepted: 04/12/2007] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Abstract
When marking the subjective midpoint of a horizontal line, patients with left unilateral neglect typically deviate rightward. Different accounts of this pattern of performance refer either to a biased competition between the two hemi-segments of the line, with the right part being subjectively perceived as longer than the left part, or to a distortion of a cognitive representation of space, with spatial coordinates progressively relaxing from the right to the left. These accounts make different predictions about the role of the right part of the line, which is crucial in the biased competition account, but less important in the distortion account. To test these predictions, we asked participants to set the endpoints and the centre of perceived and imaginary lines. Contrary to previous studies, we controlled for the direction of performance of the endpoint task, with left-to-right trials and right-to-left trials being performed in separate blocks. Five patients with right-hemisphere lesions and left neglect demonstrated the typical asymmetries when a right-sided stimulus (segment or endpoint) was present, but showed either normal performance or a reversed (leftward) bias while setting the endpoints and the centre of an imaginary line starting from the left side, when no right-sided visual stimulus was present until completion of each trial. We concluded that the right-sided portion of the line has a crucial importance in determining patients' rightward deviations in line bisection, consistent with the biased competition hypothesis and with neurocognitive models of attentional orienting.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marika Urbanski
- INSERM-UPMC UMR S 610, Hôpital de la Salpêtrière, Paris, France
| | | |
Collapse
|
35
|
Di Ferdinando A, Parisi D, Bartolomeo P. Modeling orienting behavior and its disorders with "ecological" neural networks. J Cogn Neurosci 2007; 19:1033-49. [PMID: 17536973 PMCID: PMC2231571 DOI: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.6.1033] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/04/2022]
Abstract
Computational modeling is a useful tool for spelling out hypotheses in cognitive neuroscience and testing their predictions in artificial systems. Here we describe a series of simulations involving neural networks that learned to perform their task by self-organizing their internal connections. The networks controlled artificial agents with an orienting eye and an arm. Agents saw objects with various shapes and locations and learned to press a key appropriate to their shape. The results showed the following: (1) Despite being able to see the entire visual scene without moving their eye, agents learned to orient their eye toward a peripherally presented object. (2) Neural networks whose hidden layers were previously partitioned into units dedicated to eye orienting and units dedicated to arm movements learned the identification task faster and more accurately than did nonmodular networks. (3) Nonetheless, even nonmodular networks developed a similar functional segregation through self-organization of their hidden layer. (4) After partial disconnection of the hidden layer from the input layer, the lesioned agents continued to respond accurately to single stimuli, wherever they occurred, but on double simultaneous stimulation they oriented toward and responded only to the right-sided stimulus, thus simulating extinction/neglect. These results stress the generality of the advantages provided by orienting processes. Hard-wired modularity, reminiscent of the distinct cortical visual streams in the primate brain, provided further evolutionary advantages. Finally, disconnection is likely to be a mechanism of primary importance in the pathogenesis of neglect and extinction symptoms, consistent with recent evidence from animal studies and brain-damaged patients.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Paolo Bartolomeo
- Neuro-anatomie fonctionnelle du comportement et de ses troubles
INSERM : U610IFR70Université Pierre et Marie Curie - Paris VIGH Pitie-Salpetriere
47, Boulevard de L'Hopital
75651 Paris Cedex 13,FR
- Service de neurologie
AP-HPHôpital Pitié-SalpêtrièreUniversité Pierre et Marie Curie - Paris VI47-83, boulevard de l'Hôpital
75651 PARIS Cedex 13,FR
- * Correspondence should be adressed to: Paolo Bartolomeo
| |
Collapse
|
36
|
Chokron S, Dupierrix E, Tabert M, Bartolomeo P. Experimental remission of unilateral spatial neglect. Neuropsychologia 2007; 45:3127-48. [PMID: 17889040 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.08.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 54] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/18/2006] [Revised: 07/18/2007] [Accepted: 08/02/2007] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
Over the past several decades a growing amount of research has focused on the possibility of transiently reducing left neglect signs in right brain-damaged patients by using vestibular and/or visuo-proprioceptive stimulations. Here we review seminal papers dealing with these visuo-vestibulo-proprioceptive stimulations in normal controls, right brain-damaged (RBD) patients, and animals. We discuss these data in terms of clinical implications but also with regards to theoretical frameworks commonly used to explain the unilateral neglect syndrome. We undermine the effect of these stimulations on the position of the egocentric reference and extend the notion that the positive effects of these stimulation techniques may stem from a reorientation of attention towards the neglected side of space or from a recalibration of sensori-motor correlations. We conclude this review with discussing the possible interaction between experimental rehabilitation, models of neglect and basic spatial cognition research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sylvie Chokron
- Laboratoire de Psychologie et NeuroCognition, CNRS, UMR5105, UPMF, Grenoble, France.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
37
|
Urbanski M, Angeli V, Bourlon C, Cristinzio C, Ponticorvo M, Rastelli F, Thiebaut de Schotten M, Bartolomeo P. Négligence spatiale unilatérale : une conséquence dramatique mais souvent négligée des lésions de l’hémisphère droit. Rev Neurol (Paris) 2007; 163:305-22. [PMID: 17404518 DOI: 10.1016/s0035-3787(07)90403-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/27/2023]
Abstract
INTRODUCTION Unilateral Spatial Neglect (USN) is a common consequence of right brain damage. In the most severe cases, behavioral signs of USN can last several years and compromise patients' autonomy and social rehabilitation. These clinical facts stress the need for reliable procedures of diagnosis and rehabilitation. STATE OF THE ART The last 3 decades have witnessed an explosion of studies on USN, which raises issues related to complex cognitive activities such as mental representation, spatial attention and consciousness. USN is probably a heterogeneous syndrome, but some of its underlying mechanisms might be understood as an association of disorders of spatial attention. A bias of automatic orienting towards right-sided objects seems typical of left USN. Afterwards, patients find it difficult to disengage their attention in order to explore the rest of the visual scene. Neglected objects are sometimes processed in an "implicit" way. PERSPECTIVES The development of behavioural paradigms and of neuroimaging techniques and their application to the study of USN has advanced our understanding of the functional mechanisms of attention and spatial awareness, as well as of their neural bases. A number of new procedures for rehabilitation have recently been proposed. CONCLUSION The present review describes the clinical presentation of USN, its anatomical basis and some of possible accounts of different aspects of neglect behavior. Results of computer simulations and of rehabilitation techniques are also presented with implications for the functioning of normal neurocognitive systems.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- M Urbanski
- INSERM U610, Pavillon Claude Bernard, Hôpital de la Salpêtrière, Paris, France.
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
38
|
Olk B, Harvey M. Characterizing exploration behavior in spatial neglect: omissions and repetitive search. Brain Res 2006; 1118:106-15. [PMID: 16979143 DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.08.011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/16/2005] [Revised: 06/02/2006] [Accepted: 08/04/2006] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
In search tasks, patients with spatial neglect typically fail to respond to stimuli on the contralesional side. Such behavior has been associated with hyperattention to the ipsilesional side and a deficit in disengaging from attended stimuli. The present study investigated whether such explanations can also account for a further kind of behavior frequently shown by neglect patients: repetitive returns to previously indicated stimuli, particularly on the ipsilesional side. A group of neglect patients was tested along with a group of healthy participants and a patient control group without neglect. Participants performed an exploration task in which they searched for targets defined by their shape or for all stimuli either with the aid of vision or blindfolded. The results showed differential effects of reducing the salience of visual stimuli by blindfolding. For a subgroup of patients, detection rate improved, while for others the percentage of omissions increased. However, contrary to the control groups, blindfolding had no effect on repetitive search in the neglect group, inconsistent with hyperattention, a disengage or impaired working memory deficits. The rate of repetitive returns to previously indicated locations did not seem to be associated with the percentage of omitted stimuli, suggesting that repetitive returns may be best explained by a disruption of systematic search and lack of volitional control in spatial neglect. The results further underline the importance of considering repetitive search behavior in addition to omissions in standard neglect assessments.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Bettina Olk
- School of Humanities and Social Sciences, International University Bremen, Bremen, Germany.
| | | |
Collapse
|
39
|
Bartolomeo P, Bachoud-Lévi AC, Azouvi P, Chokron S. Time to imagine space: a chronometric exploration of representational neglect. Neuropsychologia 2005; 43:1249-57. [PMID: 15949509 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.12.013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 48] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/17/2004] [Revised: 12/08/2004] [Accepted: 12/14/2004] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
When describing known places from memory, patients with left spatial neglect may mention more right- than left-sided items, thus showing representational, or imaginal, neglect. This suggests that these patients cannot either build or explore left locations in visual mental imagery. However, in place description there is no guarantee that patients are really employing visual mental imagery abilities, rather than verbal-propositional knowledge. Thus, patients providing symmetrical descriptions might be using other strategies than visual mental imagery. To address this issue, we devised a new test which strongly encourages the use of visual mental imagery. Twelve participants without brain damage and 12 right brain-damaged patients, of whom 7 had visual neglect, were invited to conjure up a visual mental image of the map of France. They subsequently had to state by pressing a left- or a right-sided key whether auditorily presented towns or regions were situated to the left or right of Paris on the imagined map. This provided measures of response time and accuracy for imagined locations. A further task, devised to assess response bias, used the words "left" or "right" as stimuli and the same keypress responses. Controls and non-neglect patients performed symmetrically. Neglect patients were slower for left than for right imagined locations. On single-case analysis, two patients with visual neglect had a greater response time asymmetry on the geographical task than predicted by the response bias task, but with symmetrical accuracy. The dissociation between response times and accuracy suggests that, in these patients, the left side of the mental map of space was not lost, but only "explored" less efficiently.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Paolo Bartolomeo
- INSERM U610, and Fédération de Neurologie, Hôpital de la Salpêtrière (AP-HP), Pavillon Claude Bernard, Hôpital de la Salpêtrière, 47 bd de l'Hôpital, F-75013 Paris, France.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
40
|
Pisella L, Mattingley JB. The contribution of spatial remapping impairments to unilateral visual neglect. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2004; 28:181-200. [PMID: 15172763 DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.03.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 120] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/23/2003] [Revised: 03/22/2004] [Accepted: 03/23/2004] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
Left visual neglect following right hemisphere damage is a heterogeneous phenomenon, in which several underlying impairments have been identified. Despite recent advances in understanding the neural and cognitive bases of these impairments, current theories of neglect, particularly those that emphasise attentional deficits, do not explain a number of phenomena, including: 'Ipsilesional' neglect after left orienting. Positive or 'productive' manifestations. Spatial transposition errors. Mislocalisations. Revisiting behaviour during visual search. Lack of awareness for objects toward the contralesional side of space. We propose that these manifestations of neglect can be accounted for by an additional underlying disorder of spatial remapping due to parietal dysfunction. In primary visual areas, retinotopic maps are renewed and thus overwritten at each new ocular fixation. Remapping processes operating in higher-level oculocentric visual maps of the parietal cortex ensure visual integration of these successive retinal images over time and space, by creating a constantly updated representation of stimulus locations in terms of distance and direction from the fovea. They consist in the storage, refreshment and re-localization of the different components of the visual scene that are successively attended during its exploration, and provide spatial constancy of visual perception and a spatial buffer for working memory [Cereb Cortex 5 (1995) 470; Visual Cogn 7 (2000) 17]. We begin this article by reviewing theoretical and experimental arguments that have highlighted the importance of parietal remapping processes in maintaining an accurate representation of space across saccadic shifts. We then focus on findings from the double-step saccade task, [Ann Neurol 38 (1995) 739] as a basis for our model of the role of remapping impairments in many of the symptoms of neglect. From these results, remapping impairments would be demonstrated when a saccade has to be guided across the midline after having fixated an object in either the left or right visual field for patients with either left- or right-side parietal lesions. In addition, patients with right-side lesions will have remapping impairments within the left visual field following a saccade to a left-side target (see Fig. 5). In a large part of the article, we seek to build our hypothesis based on this basic model and more speculative assumptions supported with extensive evidence from the literature.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Laure Pisella
- INSERM U534, Espace et action, 16 avenue Lépine, 69500 Bron, France.
| | | |
Collapse
|
41
|
Bartolomeo P, Urbanski M, Chokron S, Chainay H, Moroni C, Siéroff E, Belin C, Halligan P. Neglected attention in apparent spatial compression. Neuropsychologia 2004; 42:49-61. [PMID: 14615075 DOI: 10.1016/s0028-3932(03)00146-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 33] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
Halligan and Marshall [Cortex 27 (1991) 623] devised a new test to evaluate the hypothesis that in visual neglect, left space is systematically compressed rightwards. In the critical condition of the original study, rows of horizontally arranged numbers with a target arrow pointing to one of them from the opposite margin of the display were presented. When asked to verbally identify the number indicated by the arrow, a right brain-damaged patient with left neglect and hemianopia often indicated a number to the right of the target. The more the target was located on the left, the greater the response shift rightward, as if rightward compression were linearly proportional to the co-ordinates of Euclidian space. However, a possible alternative account could be that the patient's attention was attracted by the numbers located to the right of the target digit, thus biasing her responses toward numbers on the right. To explore this hypothesis, we asked normal participants and patients with right hemisphere lesions, with and without neglect or hemianopia, to mark on the margin of a sheet the approximate location indicated by an arrow situated on the opposite margin. In three different conditions, the arrow indicated either one of several numbers or lines in a row, or a blank location on the sheet margin. Only patients with left neglect, and especially those with associated hemianopia, deviated rightward, and then crucially only on those conditions where visible targets were present, consistent with the attentional bias account.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Paolo Bartolomeo
- French Institute of Biomedical Research, INSERM EMI 007, Centre Paul Broca, 2ter rue d'Alésia, F-75014 Paris, France.
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|