1
|
Hanke S, Niedeggen M. Event-related potentials of stimuli inhibition and access in cross-modal distractor-induced blindness. PLoS One 2024; 19:e0309425. [PMID: 39441852 PMCID: PMC11498723 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0309425] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/04/2024] [Accepted: 08/13/2024] [Indexed: 10/25/2024] Open
Abstract
Distractor-induced blindness (DIB) describes a reduced access to a cued visual target-if multiple target-like distractors have been presented beforehand. Previous ERP data suggest a cumulative frontal inhibition triggered by distractors, which affects the updating process of the upcoming target. In the present study, we examine whether the modality of the cue-formerly defined in the visual domain-affects the expression of these neural signatures. 27 subjects were tested in a cross-modal DIB task: Distractors and targets were defined by a transient change of stimuli shape in a random-dot kinematogram. The onset of the target was announced by a rise in amplitude of a sinusoidal tone. Behavioral results confirmed that detection of the target relies on the number of preceding distractor episodes. Replicating previous unimodal results, ERP responses to distractors were characterized by a frontal negativity starting at 100 ms, which increases with an increasing number of distractor episodes. However, the processing-and detection-of the target was not characterized by a more-expressed P3 response, but by an occipital negativity. The current data confirm that the neural signatures of target awareness depend on the experimental setup used: In case of the DIB, the cross-modal setting might lead to a reduction of attentional resources in the visual domain.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sophie Hanke
- Division General Psychology and Neuropsychology, Department of Education and Psychology, Freie Universität Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| | - Michael Niedeggen
- Division General Psychology and Neuropsychology, Department of Education and Psychology, Freie Universität Berlin, Berlin, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Kern L, Niedeggen M. ERP signatures of auditory awareness in cross-modal distractor-induced deafness. Conscious Cogn 2021; 96:103241. [PMID: 34823076 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2021.103241] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/07/2021] [Revised: 10/15/2021] [Accepted: 11/05/2021] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
Abstract
Previous research showed that dual-task processes such as the attentional blink are not always transferable from unimodal to cross-modal settings. This study investigated whether such a transfer can be stated for a distractor-induced impairment of target detection established in vision (distractor-induced blindness, DIB) and recently observed in the auditory modality (distractor-induced deafness, DID). A cross-modal DID effect was confirmed: The detection of an auditory target indicated by a visual cue was impaired if multiple auditory distractors preceded the target. Event-related potentials (ERPs) were used to identify psychophysiological correlates of target detection. A frontal negativity about 200 ms succeeded by a sustained, widespread negativity was associated with auditory target awareness. In contrast to unimodal findings, P3 amplitude was not enhanced for hits. The results support the notion that early frontal attentional processes are linked to auditory awareness, whereas the P3 does not seem to be a reliable indicator of target access.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lea Kern
- Freie Universität Berlin, Department of Education and Psychology, Division General Psychology and Neuropsychology, Habelschwerdter Allee 45, 14195 Berlin, Germany.
| | - Michael Niedeggen
- Freie Universität Berlin, Department of Education and Psychology, Division General Psychology and Neuropsychology, Habelschwerdter Allee 45, 14195 Berlin, Germany.
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Nobre AP, Nikolaev AR, Gauer G, van Leeuwen C, Wagemans J. Effects of Temporal Expectations on the Perception of Motion Gestalts. J Cogn Neurosci 2021; 33:853-871. [PMID: 33544060 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_01686] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/04/2022]
Abstract
Gestalt psychology has traditionally ignored the role of attention in perception, leading to the view that autonomous processes create perceptual configurations that are then attended. More recent research, however, has shown that spatial attention influences a form of Gestalt perception: the coherence of random-dot kinematograms (RDKs). Using ERPs, we investigated whether temporal expectations exert analogous attentional effects on the perception of coherence level in RDKs. Participants were presented fixed-length sequences of RDKs and reported the coherence level of a target RDK. The target was indicated immediately after its appearance by a postcue. Target expectancy increased as the sequence progressed until target presentation; afterward, remaining RDKs were perceived without target expectancy. Expectancy influenced the amplitudes of ERP components P1 and N2. Crucially, expectancy interacted with coherence level at N2, but not at P1. Specifically, P1 amplitudes decreased linearly as a function of RDK coherence irrespective of expectancy, whereas N2 exhibited a quadratic dependence on coherence: larger amplitudes for RDKs with intermediate coherence levels, and only when they were expected. These results suggest that expectancy at early processing stages is an unspecific, general readiness for perception. At later stages, expectancy becomes stimulus specific and nonlinearly related to Gestalt coherence.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alexandre P Nobre
- Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul, Porto Alegre, Brazil.,Katholieke Universiteit Leuven
| | | | - Gustavo Gauer
- Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul, Porto Alegre, Brazil
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
4
|
Winther GN, Niedeggen M. Adaptability and specificity of inhibition processes in distractor-induced blindness. Psychophysiology 2017; 54:1882-1898. [DOI: 10.1111/psyp.12974] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/03/2017] [Revised: 06/29/2017] [Accepted: 07/06/2017] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Gesche N. Winther
- Department of Education and Psychology; Freie Universität Berlin; Berlin Germany
| | - Michael Niedeggen
- Department of Education and Psychology; Freie Universität Berlin; Berlin Germany
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Winther GN, Niedeggen M. Distractor-Induced Blindness: A Special Case of Contingent Attentional Capture? Adv Cogn Psychol 2017; 13:52-63. [PMID: 28439320 PMCID: PMC5387411 DOI: 10.5709/acp-0206-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/16/2016] [Accepted: 11/03/2016] [Indexed: 11/23/2022] Open
Abstract
The detection of a salient visual target embedded in a rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) can be severely affected if target-like distractors are presented previously. This phenomenon, known as distractor-induced blindness (DIB), shares the prerequisites of contingent attentional capture (Folk, Remington, & Johnston, 1992). In both, target processing is transiently impaired by the presentation of distractors defined by similar features. In the present study, we investigated whether the speeded response to a target in the DIB paradigm can be described in terms of a contingent attentional capture process. In the first experiments, multiple distractors were embedded in the RSVP stream. Distractors either shared the target's visual features (Experiment 1A) or differed from them (Experiment 1B). Congruent with hypotheses drawn from contingent attentional capture theory, response times (RTs) were exclusively impaired in conditions with target-like distractors. However, RTs were not impaired if only one single target-like distractor was presented (Experiment 2). If attentional capture directly contributed to DIB, the single distractor should be sufficient to impair target processing. In conclusion, DIB is not due to contingent attentional capture, but may rely on a central suppression process triggered by multiple distractors.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gesche N Winther
- Department of Education and Psychology, Freie Universität Berlin, Germany
| | - Michael Niedeggen
- Department of Education and Psychology, Freie Universität Berlin, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Use of NeuroEyeCoach™ to Improve Eye Movement Efficacy in Patients with Homonymous Visual Field Loss. BIOMED RESEARCH INTERNATIONAL 2016; 2016:5186461. [PMID: 27703974 PMCID: PMC5040783 DOI: 10.1155/2016/5186461] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/14/2016] [Revised: 07/21/2016] [Accepted: 08/03/2016] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
Abstract
Visual field deficits are common in patients with damaged retinogeniculostriate pathways. The patient's eye movements are often affected leading to inefficient visual search. Systematic eye movement training also called compensatory therapy is needed to allow patients to develop effective coping strategies. There is a lack of evidence-based, clinical gold-standard registered medical device accessible to patients at home or in clinical settings and NeuroEyeCoach (NEC) is developed to address this need. In three experiments, we report on performance of patients on NEC compared to the data obtained previously on the earlier versions of the search task (n = 32); we assessed whether the self-administered computerised tasks can be used to monitor the progress (n = 24) and compared the findings in a subgroup of patients to a healthy control group. Performance on cancellation tasks, simple visual search, and self-reported responses on activities of daily living was compared, before and after training. Patients performed similarly well on NEC as on previous versions of the therapy; the inbuilt functionality for pre- and postevaluation functions was sensitive to allowing assessment of improvements; and improvements in patients were significantly greater than those in a group of healthy adults. In conclusion, NeuroEyeCoach can be used as an effective rehabilitation tool to develop compensatory strategies in patients with visual field deficits after brain injury.
Collapse
|
7
|
The state of a central inhibition system predicts access to visual targets: An ERP study on distractor-induced blindness (DIB). Conscious Cogn 2015; 35:308-18. [DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2015.02.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/04/2014] [Revised: 01/29/2015] [Accepted: 02/03/2015] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
|
8
|
Niedeggen M, Michael L, Hesselmann G. Closing the Gates to Consciousness: Distractors Activate a Central Inhibition Process. J Cogn Neurosci 2012; 24:1294-304. [DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_00177] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/04/2022]
Abstract
Abstract
The paradigm of distractor-induced blindness has previously been used to track the transition from unconscious to conscious visual processing. In a variation of this paradigm used in this study, participants (n = 13) had to detect an orientation change of tilted bars (target) embedded in a dynamic random pattern; the onset of the target was signaled by the presentation of a color cue. Occasional orientation changes preceding the cue served as distractors and severely impaired the target's detection. ERPs showed that a frontal negativity was cumulatively activated by the distractors, and early sensory components were not affected. In a control condition, the target was defined by a coherent motion of the bars. Orientation changes preceding the motion target did not affect its detection, and the frontal suppression process was not observed. However, we obtained a significant reduction of the sensory components. The data support the notion that distractors that share the target's features trigger a cumulative inhibition process preventing the conscious representation of the inhibited features. Explorative source modeling suggests that this process originates in the pFC. A top–down modulation of sensory processing could not be observed.
Collapse
|
9
|
The influence of distracter and target features on distracter induced blindness. Adv Cogn Psychol 2012; 8:62-9. [PMID: 22419967 PMCID: PMC3303155 DOI: 10.2478/v10053-008-0103-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/25/2011] [Accepted: 01/11/2012] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
Abstract
The inhibitory effect of the processing of target-like distracters has already been shown to affect the conscious detection of simple motion and simple orientation stimuli in a random dot kinematogram. In two experiments we examined the effects of single-feature motion distracters, single-feature orientation distracters, and combined-feature distracters containing both motion and orientation information. The target was specified as a coherent motion episode (Experiment 1) or as a combined-feature episode where the coherent motion was accompanied by an abrupt change in line orientation (Experiment 2). Results showed that (a) the respective feature-specific inhibitory processes operate separately even when the distracter features are presented simultaneously and (b) both inhibitory processes contribute to the blindness effect when the conjunction of two features is defined as the target. Again, this inhibitory-process is feature-specific: Only features that are defined in the task are represented in the inhibitory task set. In case of combined- feature task-sets, these representations remain separate, so that combined-feature distracters as well as single-feature distracters are able to induce blindness effects.
Collapse
|
10
|
Neuro-cognitive mechanisms of conscious and unconscious visual perception: From a plethora of phenomena to general principles. Adv Cogn Psychol 2011; 7:55-67. [PMID: 22253669 PMCID: PMC3259028 DOI: 10.2478/v10053-008-0090-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/17/2011] [Accepted: 08/05/2011] [Indexed: 11/20/2022] Open
Abstract
Psychological and neuroscience approaches have promoted much progress in elucidating the cognitive and neural mechanisms that underlie phenomenal visual awareness during the last decades. In this article, we provide an overview of the latest research investigating important phenomena in conscious and unconscious vision. We identify general principles to characterize conscious and unconscious visual perception, which may serve as important building blocks for a unified model to explain the plethora of findings. We argue that in particular the integration of principles from both conscious and unconscious vision is advantageous and provides critical constraints for developing adequate theoretical models. Based on the principles identified in our review, we outline essential components of a unified model of conscious and unconscious visual perception. We propose that awareness refers to consolidated visual representations, which are accessible to the entire brain and therefore globally available. However, visual awareness not only depends on consolidation within the visual system, but is additionally the result of a post-sensory gating process, which is mediated by higher-level cognitive control mechanisms. We further propose that amplification of visual representations by attentional sensitization is not exclusive to the domain of conscious perception, but also applies to visual stimuli, which remain unconscious. Conscious and unconscious processing modes are highly interdependent with influences in both directions. We therefore argue that exactly this interdependence renders a unified model of conscious and unconscious visual perception valuable. Computational modeling jointly with focused experimental research could lead to a better understanding of the plethora of empirical phenomena in consciousness research.
Collapse
|
11
|
Michael L, Hesselmann G, Kiefer M, Niedeggen M. Distractor-induced blindness for orientation changes and coherent motion. Vision Res 2011; 51:1781-7. [DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2011.06.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/01/2011] [Revised: 05/26/2011] [Accepted: 06/08/2011] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
|
12
|
Zhang J, Kong W, Yang Z. Identification of a novel dynamic red blindness in human by event-related brain potentials. JOURNAL OF HUAZHONG UNIVERSITY OF SCIENCE AND TECHNOLOGY. MEDICAL SCIENCES = HUA ZHONG KE JI DA XUE XUE BAO. YI XUE YING DE WEN BAN = HUAZHONG KEJI DAXUE XUEBAO. YIXUE YINGDEWEN BAN 2010; 30:786-791. [PMID: 21181373 DOI: 10.1007/s11596-010-0659-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/24/2010] [Indexed: 05/30/2023]
Abstract
Dynamic color is an important carrier that takes information in some special occupations. However, up to the present, there are no available and objective tests to evaluate dynamic color processing. To investigate the characteristics of dynamic color processing, we adopted two patterns of visual stimulus called "onset-offset" which reflected static color stimuli and "sustained moving" without abrupt mode which reflected dynamic color stimuli to evoke event-related brain potentials (ERPs) in primary color amblyopia patients (abnormal group) and subjects with normal color recognition ability (normal group). ERPs were recorded by Neuroscan system. The results showed that in the normal group, ERPs in response to the dynamic red stimulus showed frontal positive amplitudes with a latency of about 180 ms, a negative peak at about 240 ms and a peak latency of the late positive potential (LPP) in a time window between 290 and 580 ms. In the abnormal group, ERPs in response to the dynamic red stimulus were fully lost and characterized by vanished amplitudes between 0 and 800 ms. No significant difference was noted in ERPs in response to the dynamic green and blue stimulus between the two groups (P>0.05). ERPs of the two groups in response to the static red, green and blue stimulus were not much different, showing a transient negative peak at about 170 ms and a peak latency of LPP in a time window between 350 and 650 ms. Our results first revealed that some subjects who were not identified as color blindness under static color recognition could not completely apperceive a sort of dynamic red stimulus by ERPs, which was called "dynamic red blindness". Furthermore, these results also indicated that low-frequency ERPs induced by "sustained moving" may be a good and new method to test dynamic color perception competence.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jiahua Zhang
- Department of Otorhinolaryngology, Union Hospital, Tongji Medical College, Huazhong University of Science and Technology, Wuhan 430022, China.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
13
|
Xuan D, Wang S, Yang Y, Meng P, Xu F, Yang W, Sheng W, Yang Y. Age difference in numeral recognition and calculation: an event-related potential study. Child Neuropsychol 2007; 13:1-17. [PMID: 17364561 DOI: 10.1080/09297040600760465] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
Abstract
In this study, we investigated the age difference in numeral recognition and calculation in one group of school-aged children (n = 38) and one of undergraduate students (n = 26) using the event-related potential (ERP) methods. Consistent with previous reports, the age difference was significant in behavioral results. Both numeral recognition and calculation elicited a negativity peaking at about 170-280 ms (N2) and a positivity peaking at 200-470 ms (pSW) in raw ERPs, and a difference potential (dN3) between 360 and 450 ms. The difference between the two age groups indicated that more attention resources were devoted to arithmetical tasks in school-aged children, and that school-aged children and undergraduate students appear to use different strategies to solve arithmetical problems. The analysis of frontal negativity suggested that numeral recognition and mental calculation impose greater load on working memory and executive function in schoolchildren than in undergraduate students. The topography data determined that the parietal regions were responsible for arithmetical function in humans, and there was an age-related difference in the area of cerebral activation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dong Xuan
- Department of Neuroscience, The Third Affiliated Hospital, Soochow University, China
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
14
|
Heinrich SP. A primer on motion visual evoked potentials. Doc Ophthalmol 2007; 114:83-105. [PMID: 17431818 DOI: 10.1007/s10633-006-9043-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 54] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/18/2006] [Revised: 11/28/2006] [Indexed: 12/18/2022]
Abstract
Motion visual evoked potentials (motion VEPs) have been used since the late 1960s to investigate the properties of human visual motion processing, and continue to be a popular tool with a possible future in clinical diagnosis. This review first provides a synopsis of the characteristics of motion VEPs and then summarizes important methodological aspects. A subsequent overview illustrates how motion VEPs have been applied to study basic functions of human motion processing and shows perspectives for their use as a diagnostic tool.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sven P Heinrich
- Sektion Funktionelle Sehforschung, Universitäts-Augenklinik, Freiburg, Germany.
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Dong X, Wang S, Yang Y, Ren Y, Meng P, Yang Y. Cognitive Development of Semantic Process and Mental Arithmetic in Childhood: An Event-Related Potential. DATA SCIENCE JOURNAL 2007. [DOI: 10.2481/dsj.6.s535] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/20/2022] Open
|
16
|
Tsushima Y, Sasaki Y, Watanabe T. Greater disruption due to failure of inhibitory control on an ambiguous distractor. Science 2006; 314:1786-8. [PMID: 17170308 DOI: 10.1126/science.1133197] [Citation(s) in RCA: 118] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/02/2022]
Abstract
Considerable evidence indicates that a stimulus that is subthreshold, and thus consciously invisible, influences brain activity and behavioral performance. However, it is not clear how subthreshold stimuli are processed in the brain. We found that a task-irrelevant subthreshold coherent motion led to a stronger disturbance in task performance than did suprathreshold motion. With the subthreshold motion, activity in the visual cortex measured by functional magnetic resonance imaging was higher, but activity in the lateral prefrontal cortex was lower, than with suprathreshold motion. These results suggest that subthreshold irrelevant signals are not subject to effective inhibitory control.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Yoshiaki Tsushima
- Department of Psychology, Boston University, 64 Cummington Street, Boston, MA 02215, USA
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
17
|
Niedeggen M, Hesselmann G, Sahraie A, Milders M. ERPs predict the appearance of visual stimuli in a temporal selection task. Brain Res 2006; 1097:205-15. [PMID: 16730675 DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.04.087] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/21/2005] [Revised: 04/25/2006] [Accepted: 04/26/2006] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
In contrast to the visual spatial domain, the effect of attention on sensory processing and stimulus appearance in temporal selection tasks is still controversial. Using a rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) procedure, we examined whether the stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) between a color cue and a motion target affects the appearance of the latter. Event-related brain potentials (ERPs) recorded simultaneously allowed us to test whether a change in the targets' appearance is associated with a modulation of the sensory ERP components. In the experimental condition 'SOA', the temporal interval between the cue and the target was varied between 0 and 300 ms. In a control condition, the physical appearance of the motion target was varied (level of coherence: 25-100%) while holding the cue-target SOA constant (300 ms). In trials when the participant detected the target motion, his/her task was to report the strength of the perceived motion on a 5-point scale. In both conditions, the mean rating of the target's appearance increased monotonically with increasing SOA and the level of coherence, respectively. The psychophysical ratings were associated with an increase of a negative deflection about 200 ms (N200) related to the sensory processing of visual motion. The physical variation of motion coherence and the variation of the cue-target SOA affected the N200 response in similar fashion. These results indicate that sensory processing is also modulated by attentional resources in temporal selection tasks which - in turn - affect the appearance of the relevant target stimulus.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michael Niedeggen
- Institute of Experimental Psychology II, Heinrich-Heine-University, D-40225 Düsseldorf, Germany.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
18
|
Kreegipuu K, Allik J. Detection of motion onset and offset: reaction time and visual evoked potential analysis. PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2006; 71:703-8. [PMID: 16636840 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-006-0059-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/02/2005] [Accepted: 12/21/2005] [Indexed: 12/19/2022]
Abstract
Manual reaction time (RT) and visual evoked potentials (VEP) were measured in motion onset and offset detection tasks. A considerable homology was observed between the temporal structure of RTs and VEP intervals, provided that the change in motion was detected as soon as the VEP signal has reached critical threshold amplitude. Both manual reactions and VEP rise in latency as the velocity of the onset or offset motion decreases and were well approximated by the same negative power function with the exponent close to -2/3. This indicates that motion processing is normalised by subtracting the initial motion vector from ongoing motion. A comparison of the motion onset VEP signals in two different conditions, in one of which the observer was instructed to abstain from the reaction and in the other to indicate as fast as possible the beginning of the motion, contained accurate information about the manual response.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kairi Kreegipuu
- University of Tartu and the ECBHS, Tiigi 78, Tartu, 50410, Estonia.
| | | |
Collapse
|
19
|
Hesselmann G, Niedeggen M, Sahraie A, Milders M. Specifying the distractor inhibition account of attention-induced motion blindness. Vision Res 2006; 46:1048-56. [PMID: 16309728 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2005.10.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/17/2005] [Revised: 10/11/2005] [Accepted: 10/12/2005] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
Abstract
There is growing evidence that motion perception is modulated by visual selective attention. In the 'attention-induced motion blindness' paradigm the detection of coherent motion in a random dot kinematogram (RDK) is impaired in a rapid serial presentation task [Sahraie, A., Milders, M., & Niedeggen, M. (2001). Attention induced motion blindness. Vision Research, 41, 1613-1617]. The effect depends on irrelevant motion episodes (distractors) prior to the target. In this study, we show that both the number and timing of distractors affect detection performance, allowing for implications on the build-up and release of inhibition. Furthermore, we rule out the possibility that subjects falsely classify targets as distractors due to uncertainty of temporal order.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Guido Hesselmann
- Institute of Experimental Psychology II, Heinrich-Heine-Universität, Düesseldorf, Germany.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
20
|
Rodríguez V, Valdés-Sosa M. Sensory suppression during shifts of attention between surfaces in transparent motion. Brain Res 2006; 1072:110-8. [PMID: 16438942 DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.10.071] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/14/2004] [Revised: 10/18/2005] [Accepted: 10/20/2005] [Indexed: 10/25/2022]
Abstract
During transparent motion, attention to changes in the direction of one illusory surface will impede recognition of a similar event affecting the other surface if both are close together in time. This is a form of object-based attentional blink (AB). Here, we show that this AB is related to a smaller N200 response to the change in direction and that the response is even smaller for trials on which the subject makes mistakes compared to those with correct responses consistent with signal detection theory models. The variation of N200 associated with the AB can be modeled by an attenuation of current sources estimated in visual extrastriate cortex. These results suggest that the AB in the transparent motion paradigm is due to the suppression of sensory signals in early visual areas.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Valia Rodríguez
- Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Cuban Neuroscience Center, Ave. 25 # 15202, esq.158, Cubanacán, Playa, CP 11600 C. Habana, Cuba.
| | | |
Collapse
|
21
|
Niedeggen M, Hesselmann G, Sahraie A, Milders M, Blakemore C. Probing the Prerequisites for Motion Blindness. J Cogn Neurosci 2004; 16:584-97. [PMID: 15165349 DOI: 10.1162/089892904323057317] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/04/2022]
Abstract
Abstract
Neurobiological studies of visual awareness usually focus on the neural events elicited by perceived or nonperceived stimuli but neglect the preexisting conditions that allow (or prevent) conscious perception. We have examined the conditions that lead to temporary motion blindness in a rapid serial visual presentation paradigm, in which subjects have to detect coherent motion in the peripheral stream after a cue (a red fixation point) in the central stream. The failure of awareness depends critically on the occurrence of similar coherent motion events (probes) before the cue. Event-related brain potentials (ERPs) were recorded to track the processing of motion distractors, which determine the prerequisites for this transient deficit. Analysis of motion-evoked responses revealed that there is no progressive reduction in sensitivity in early visual processing. There is, however, a progressive increase in amplitude of a negative wave over the frontal cortex at approximately 250 msec after motion onset and a corresponding reduction of a centro-parietal positivity at approximately 350 msec with an increasing number of distractors. We propose that these nonsensory ERP components reflect a postperceptual frontal gating mechanism that controls the access of visual stimuli to higher order evaluation and conscious detection.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michael Niedeggen
- Institute for Experimental Psychology II, Heinrich-Heine-University, Duesseldorf, Germany.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
22
|
Abstract
A pattern of light striking the retina of an alert observer is normally readily perceived. While a handful of conditions exist in which even salient visual stimuli can be rendered invisible, the mechanisms underlying such suppression remain poorly understood. Here, we describe experiments using a novel stimulation sequence that gives rise to the sudden and reliable subjective disappearance of a wide range of visual patterns. We found that a parafoveal target immediately vanished from perception following the abrupt onset of a surrounding texture. The probability of disappearance was influenced by the ocular configuration of the target and surround, as well as their spatial separation. In addition, suppression was critically dependent upon several hundred milliseconds of stimulus-specific adaptation. These findings demonstrate that the all-or-none disappearance of a salient visual target, which is reminiscent of a high-level selection process, is inextricably linked to topographic stimulus representations, presumably in the early visual cortex.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Melanie Wilke
- Max Planck Institut für biologische Kybernetik, Spemannstrasse 38, 72076 Tübingen, Germany
| | | | | |
Collapse
|