1
|
Cuenca-Martínez F, Herranz-Gómez A, Varangot-Reille C, Bajcar EA, Adamczyk WM, Suso-Martí L, Bąbel P. Pain memory in children: a systematic review and meta-analysis with a meta-regression. Pain 2024; 165:1450-1463. [PMID: 38314811 DOI: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000003170] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/05/2023] [Accepted: 12/04/2023] [Indexed: 02/07/2024]
Abstract
ABSTRACT The aim of this systematic review and meta-analysis was to analyze the accuracy of memory of pain and the variables that may influence it in children with acute, experimental, and chronic pain. We conducted a search in electronic databases from inception to February 11, 2022. Twelve observational studies and 3 randomized controlled studies were included in the study. The main outcome measure was the accuracy of the memory of the pain intensity (experienced/recalled). To compare the outcomes reported by the studies, we calculated the standardized mean difference (SMD) over time for the continuous variables. The overall meta-analysis showed a small effect size in favor of an overestimation of experienced pain intensity (SMD = 0.28). Subanalyzing per pain context, there was a small effect size in favor of overestimation in the clinical context (SMD = 0.33), but there was no evidence of any change in the accuracy of memory of pain in the experimental context (SMD = 0.07). The mean age of the participants and the proportion of girls significantly predicted the accuracy of the memory of pain. The period since the experienced pain measurement, the intensity of expected and recalled fear, trait anxiety, and anxiety sensitivity did not significantly predict the accuracy of the memory of pain. Children showed an overestimation in pain memory between the experienced and recalled intensity of acute pain, especially in a clinical context. Furthermore, only gender and age were predictors of the accuracy of pain memory. These results highlight the relevance of pain memory to medical practice and future research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Aida Herranz-Gómez
- Department of Physiotherapy, Faculty of Health Sciences, European University of Valencia, Valencia, Spain
| | | | - Elżbieta A Bajcar
- Jagiellonian University, Institute of Psychology, Pain Research Group, Kraków, Poland
| | - Wacław M Adamczyk
- Division of Behavioral Medicine and Clinical Psychology, Cincinnati Children's Hospital Medical Center, Cincinnati, OH, United States
| | - Luis Suso-Martí
- Department of Physiotherapy, University of Valencia, Valencia, Spain
| | - Przemysław Bąbel
- Jagiellonian University, Institute of Psychology, Pain Research Group, Kraków, Poland
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Kang S, Van Ryckeghem DML, Vlaeyen JWS, De Paepe AL, Crombez G. In search of conditioned pain: an experimental analysis. Pain 2023; 164:2596-2605. [PMID: 37288937 DOI: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000002964] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/07/2023] [Accepted: 04/26/2023] [Indexed: 06/09/2023]
Abstract
ABSTRACT There is an ongoing debate about whether pain can be classically conditioned, but surprisingly, evidence is scarce. Here, we report 3 experiments investigating this idea. In a virtual reality task, healthy participants were approached and touched near or on their hand with a coloured pen (blue or yellow). During acquisition, participants learned that one of the colours of the pen (CS+) was predictive of a painful electrocutaneous stimulus (ECS) whereas the other coloured pen (CS-) was not. During the test phase, more frequent reports of experiencing an US when none was delivered ("false alarm") for the CS+ vs CS- qualified as evidence of conditioned pain. Notable differences between experiments were that the US was delivered when the pen touched a spot between the thumb and index finger (experiment 1; n = 23), when it virtually touched the hand (experiment 2; n = 28) and when participants were informed that the pen caused pain rather than simply predicting something (experiment 3; n = 21). The conditioning procedure proved successful in all 3 experiments: Self-reported fear, attention, pain, fear, and US expectancy were higher ( P < 0.0005) for the CS+ than the CS-. There was no evidence for conditioned pain in experiment 1, but there was some evidence in experiments 2 and 3. Our findings indicate that conditioned pain may exist, albeit most likely in rare cases or under specific situations. More research is needed to understand the specific conditions under which conditioned pain exists and the underlying processes (eg, response bias).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sahaj Kang
- Department of Experimental Clinical and Health Psychology, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium
- Research Group Health Psychology, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
| | - Dimitri M L Van Ryckeghem
- Department of Experimental Clinical and Health Psychology, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium
- Experimental Health Psychology, Maastricht University, Maastricht, the Netherlands
- Department of Behavioural and Cognitive Sciences, University of Luxembourg, Esch-sur-Alzette, Luxembourg
| | - Johan W S Vlaeyen
- Research Group Health Psychology, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
- Experimental Health Psychology, Maastricht University, Maastricht, the Netherlands
| | - Annick L De Paepe
- Department of Experimental Clinical and Health Psychology, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium
| | - Geert Crombez
- Department of Experimental Clinical and Health Psychology, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Rheel E, Ickmans K, Wauters A, Van Ryckeghem DML, Barbé K, Malfliet A, Vervoort T. The Effect of a Pain Educational Video Upon Child Pain-Related Memory and the Moderating Role of Parental Pain- and Non-Pain-Attending Verbalizations: An Experimental Lab-Based Study. J Pediatr Psychol 2022; 47:1057-1070. [PMID: 35640009 DOI: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsac044] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/27/2022] [Revised: 03/28/2022] [Accepted: 04/26/2022] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
OBJECTIVES Early memories of pain contribute to fear and may underlie the maintenance and development of chronic pain into adulthood. Accordingly, understanding determinants that may impact children's pain memory development is key. This study examined (a) the effect of a brief engaging pain educational video in healthy children before undergoing an experimental pain task upon children's recalled pain intensity and pain-related fear and (b) the moderating role of parental pain- and non-pain-attending verbalizations before and after the pain task. METHODS Seventy-seven children (8-15 years old) participated in an experimental heat pain task, including actual heat pain stimuli delivered through a thermode on their forearm. Children were randomized to the experimental group (i.e., watching a pain educational video) or the control group (i.e., no video). Children's recalled pain intensity and pain-related fear were elicited 2 weeks later. RESULTS Findings showed that recalled pain intensity (but not recalled pain-related fear) of children who watched the pain educational video was significantly lower compared to the control group (p = .028). Further, parental pain-attending verbalizations before the pain task moderated the impact of the video upon children's recalled pain intensity (p = .038). Specifically, children in the control group, but not the experimental group, whose parents used less pain-attending verbalizations recalled higher pain intensity, whereas children whose parents used more pain-attending verbalizations recalled lower pain intensity. CONCLUSIONS As children's pain memories have important implications for pain assessment, treatment, and health across the lifespan, these findings might have important implications for the prevention of development or maintenance of maladaptive pain-related outcomes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Emma Rheel
- Pain in Motion Research Group (PAIN), Department of Physiotherapy, Human Physiology and Anatomy, Faculty of Physical Education & Physiotherapy, Vrije Universiteit Brussel, Brussels, Belgium.,Department of Experimental-Clinical and Health Psychology, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium
| | - Kelly Ickmans
- Pain in Motion Research Group (PAIN), Department of Physiotherapy, Human Physiology and Anatomy, Faculty of Physical Education & Physiotherapy, Vrije Universiteit Brussel, Brussels, Belgium.,Department of Physical Medicine and Physiotherapy, Universitair Ziekenhuis Brussel, Brussels, Belgium.,Movement & Nutrition for Health & Performance research group (MOVE), Department of Movement and Sport Sciences, Faculty of Physical Education and Physiotherapy, Vrije Universiteit Brussel, Brussels, Belgium
| | - Aline Wauters
- Department of Experimental-Clinical and Health Psychology, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium
| | - Dimitri M L Van Ryckeghem
- Department of Experimental-Clinical and Health Psychology, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium.,Section Experimental Health Psychology, Clinical Psychological Science, Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands.,Institute for Health and Behavior, INSIDE, University of Luxembourg, Luxembourg City, Luxembourg
| | - Kurt Barbé
- Interfaculty Center for Date-processing and Statistics (ICDS), Vrije Universiteit Brussel, Brussels, Belgium
| | - Anneleen Malfliet
- Pain in Motion Research Group (PAIN), Department of Physiotherapy, Human Physiology and Anatomy, Faculty of Physical Education & Physiotherapy, Vrije Universiteit Brussel, Brussels, Belgium.,Department of Physical Medicine and Physiotherapy, Universitair Ziekenhuis Brussel, Brussels, Belgium.,Research Foundation-Flanders (FWO), Brussels, Belgium
| | - Tine Vervoort
- Department of Experimental-Clinical and Health Psychology, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Neuroscientific evidence for pain being a classically conditioned response to trauma- and pain-related cues in humans. Pain 2022; 163:2118-2137. [PMID: 35239544 DOI: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000002621] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/26/2021] [Accepted: 12/07/2021] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
Abstract
ABSTRACT Psychological trauma is typically accompanied by physical pain, and posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) often co-occurs with chronic pain. Clinical reports suggest that pain after trauma may be part of a re-experiencing symptomatology. Classical conditioning can underlie visual re-experiencing since intrusions can occur as conditioned responses (CRs) to trauma-related cues. If individuals also experience pain to cues previously paired with, but not anymore inflicting nociceptive stimulation (CSs), conditioning could also explain re-experiencing of pain. Sixty-five participants underwent classical conditioning, where painful electrocutaneous stimulation and aversive film-clips served as unconditioned stimuli (USs) in a 2(pain/no pain)×2(aversive/neutral film) design. CSs were neutral pictures depicting contextual details from the films. One day later, participants were re-exposed to CSs during a memory-triggering-task (MTT). We assessed pain-CRs by self-report and an fMRI-based marker of nociceptive pain, the neurologic pain signature (NPS); and recorded spontaneous daily-life pain-intrusions with an e-diary. During conditioning, pain-signaling CSs elicited more self-reported-pain and NPS-responses than no-pain-signaling CSs. Possibly because the aversive-film masked differences in participants' responses to pain-signaling vs. no-pain-signaling CSs, pain-CRs during acquisition only emerged within the neutral-film condition. When participants were re-exposed to CSs during MTT, self-reported-pain-CRs during the neutral-film condition and, though more uncertain, NPS-CRs during the aversive-film condition persisted. Importantly, participants with stronger pain-CRs showed a greater probability and severity of experiencing spontaneous pain intrusions during daily-life. Our data support that pain can emerge as a CR with emotional and sensory components. Classical conditioning presents a possible mechanism explaining pain-intrusions, and more broadly, pain experienced without nociceptive input.
Collapse
|
5
|
Doshi TL, Dworkin RH, Polomano RC, Carr DB, Edwards RR, Finnerup NB, Freeman RL, Paice JA, Weisman SJ, Raja SN. AAAPT Diagnostic Criteria for Acute Neuropathic Pain. PAIN MEDICINE 2021; 22:616-636. [PMID: 33575803 DOI: 10.1093/pm/pnaa407] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/31/2022]
Abstract
OBJECTIVE Acute neuropathic pain is a significant diagnostic challenge, and it is closely related to our understanding of both acute pain and neuropathic pain. Diagnostic criteria for acute neuropathic pain should reflect our mechanistic understanding and provide a framework for research on and treatment of these complex pain conditions. METHODS The Analgesic, Anesthetic, and Addiction Clinical Trial Translations, Innovations, Opportunities, and Networks (ACTTION) public-private partnership with the U.S. Food and Drug Administration (FDA), the American Pain Society (APS), and the American Academy of Pain Medicine (AAPM) collaborated to develop the ACTTION-APS-AAPM Pain Taxonomy (AAAPT) for acute pain. A working group of experts in research and clinical management of neuropathic pain was convened. Group members used literature review and expert opinion to develop diagnostic criteria for acute neuropathic pain, as well as three specific examples of acute neuropathic pain conditions, using the five dimensions of the AAAPT classification of acute pain. RESULTS AAAPT diagnostic criteria for acute neuropathic pain are presented. Application of these criteria to three specific conditions (pain related to herpes zoster, chemotherapy, and limb amputation) illustrates the spectrum of acute neuropathic pain and highlights unique features of each condition. CONCLUSIONS The proposed AAAPT diagnostic criteria for acute neuropathic pain can be applied to various acute neuropathic pain conditions. Both the general and condition-specific criteria may guide future research, assessment, and management of acute neuropathic pain.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tina L Doshi
- Department of Anesthesiology and Critical Care Medicine, Division of Pain Medicine, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, Maryland, USA
| | - Robert H Dworkin
- Department of Anesthesiology, University of Rochester School of Medicine and Dentistry, Rochester, New York, and Department of Neurology, Center for Human Experimental Therapeutics, University of Rochester School of Medicine and Dentistry, Rochester, New York, USA
| | - Rosemary C Polomano
- Division of Biobehavioral Health Sciences, University of Pennsylvania-School of Nursing, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA
| | - Daniel B Carr
- Public Health and Community Medicine Program, Tufts University School of Medicine, Boston, Massachusetts, USA
| | - Robert R Edwards
- Department of Anesthesiology, Perioperative, and Pain Medicine, Brigham and Women's Hospital, Boston, Massachusetts, USA
| | - Nanna B Finnerup
- Danish Pain Research Center, Department of Clinical Medicine, Aarhus University, and Department of Neurology, Aarhus University Hospital, Aarhus, Denmark
| | - Roy L Freeman
- Center for Autonomic and Peripheral Nerve Disorders, Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center, Boston, Massachusetts, USA
| | - Judith A Paice
- Cancer Pain Program, Northwestern University, Feinberg School of Medicine, Chicago, Illinois, USA
| | - Steven J Weisman
- Jane B. Pettit Pain and Headache Center, Children's Wisconsin, Departments of Anesthesiology and Pediatrics, Medical College of Wisconsin, Milwaukee, Wisconsin, USA
| | - Srinivasa N Raja
- Department of Anesthesiology and Critical Care Medicine, Division of Pain Medicine, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, Maryland, USA
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Meulders A. Fear in the context of pain: Lessons learned from 100 years of fear conditioning research. Behav Res Ther 2020; 131:103635. [DOI: 10.1016/j.brat.2020.103635] [Citation(s) in RCA: 50] [Impact Index Per Article: 12.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/13/2019] [Revised: 04/20/2020] [Accepted: 04/24/2020] [Indexed: 02/06/2023]
|
7
|
Traxler J, Madden VJ, Moseley GL, Vlaeyen JWS. Modulating pain thresholds through classical conditioning. PeerJ 2019; 7:e6486. [PMID: 30867984 PMCID: PMC6410694 DOI: 10.7717/peerj.6486] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/28/2018] [Accepted: 01/17/2019] [Indexed: 12/04/2022] Open
Abstract
Background Classical conditioning has frequently been shown to be capable of evoking fear of pain and avoidance behavior in the context of chronic pain. However, whether pain itself can be conditioned has rarely been investigated and remains a matter of debate. Therefore, the present study investigated whether pain threshold ratings can be modified by the presence of conditioned non-nociceptive sensory stimuli in healthy participant. Methods In 51 healthy volunteers, pain threshold to electrocutaneous stimuli was determined prior to participation in a simultaneous conditioning paradigm. Participants underwent an acquisition phase in which one non-painful vibrotactile stimulus (CS+) was repeatedly paired with a painful electrocutaneous stimulus, whereas a second vibrotactile stimulus of the same quality and intensity (CS−) was paired with a non-painful electrocutaneous stimulus. Stimulation was provided on the lower back with close proximity between the conditioned stimulus and the unconditioned stimulus. In the test phase, electrocutaneous stimuli at the individually-set threshold intensity were simultaneously delivered together with either a CS+ or CS−. Pain intensity ratings were obtained after each trial; expectancy ratings were obtained after each block. The primary outcome was the percentage of test stimuli that were rated as painful. Results Test stimuli were more likely to be rated as painful when they were paired with the CS+ than when they were paired with the CS−. This effect was not influenced by contingency awareness, nor by expectancies or mood states. Discussion The findings support the notion that the judgement of an event being painful or non-painful can be influenced by classical conditioning and corroborate the possible role of associative learning in the development and maintenance of chronic pain.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Juliane Traxler
- Research Centre for Health Psychology, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium.,Experimental Health Psychology, Maastricht University, Maastricht, Netherlands
| | - Victoria J Madden
- Research Centre for Health Psychology, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium.,Department of Psychiatry and Mental Health, Department of Anaesthesia and Perioperative Medicine, University of Cape Town, Cape Town, South Africa
| | - G Lorimer Moseley
- Body in Mind Research Group, University of South Australia, Adelaide, SA, Australia
| | - Johan W S Vlaeyen
- Research Centre for Health Psychology, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium.,Experimental Health Psychology, Maastricht University, Maastricht, Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Reddan MC, Wager TD. Brain systems at the intersection of chronic pain and self-regulation. Neurosci Lett 2018; 702:24-33. [PMID: 30503923 DOI: 10.1016/j.neulet.2018.11.047] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/19/2022]
Abstract
Chronic pain is a multidimensional experience with cognitive, affective, and somatosensory components that can be modified by expectations and learning. Individual differences in cognitive and affective processing, as well as contextual aspects of the pain experience, render chronic pain an inherently personal experience. Such individual differences are supported by the heterogeneity of brain representations within and across chronic pain pathologies. In this review, we discuss the complexity of brain representations of pain, and, with respect to this complexity, identify common elements of network-level disruptions in chronic pain. Specifically, we identify prefrontal-limbic circuitry and the default mode network as key elements of functional disruption. We then discuss how these disrupted circuits can be targeted through self-regulation and related cognitive strategies to alleviate chronic pain. We conclude with a proposal for how to develop personalized multivariate models of pain representation in the brain and target them with real-time neurofeedback, so that patients can explore and practice self-regulatory techniques with maximal efficiency.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Tor D Wager
- University of Colorado, Boulder, United States.
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Abstract
OBJECTIVE The goal of this study was to examine pain responses in pediatric patients with cancer. METHOD Children (ages 6 to 18) undergoing treatment for cancer (N=68) completed the cold pressor task. RESULTS Average pain tolerance was 118.22 seconds (SD=101.18) and 40% of the children kept their hand in the water the entire 4-minute ceiling. On a 0 to 10 numeric rating scale, children reported a pain severity of 5.07 (SD=3.47) at their first report of pain, a pain severity of 5.94 (SD=3.54) at their maximum report of pain, and a pain severity of 5.33 (SD=3.72) at the time they reached pain tolerance. Children receiving chemotherapy agents (N=56) with possible neuropathic effects exhibited higher pain tolerance compared with children not receiving such treatments (N=10), β=0.84, SE=0.38, Wald χ1=4.88, P=0.027, hazard ratio=2.33, 95% confidence interval (1.10-4.92). CONCLUSIONS This study provides data on experimental pain responses in a sample of children undergoing cancer treatment and suggests that pain experience may be moderated by cancer treatment type.
Collapse
|
10
|
Abstract
The interaction of cardiovascular dynamics and pain perception is an important component of intrinsic pain regulation. In healthy subjects acute pain stimuli cause increased sympathetic arousal and increased mean arterial pressure. Arterial baroreceptors sense phasic blood pressure changes and relay the information to the lower brainstem via the dorsomedial nucleus tractus solitarius (dmNTS). Projections in the brainstem and also higher cortical areas result in elevation of blood pressure as part of the autonomic nervous system as well as modulation of sleep, anxiety and pain. In healthy subjects there is an inverse relationship between blood pressure and pain sensitivity but this relationship is impaired in chronic pain patients. Persistent stress, pain behavior and classical and operant conditioning mechanisms reduce baroreflex sensitivity (BRS) and dmNTS activity in a subgroup of patients. This leads to a decrease of autonomic regulatory function as well as reduced pain inhibition. Importantly, baroreflex function can be modulated by cognitive and affective processes. This article reviews the role of the baroreflex arc as a possible crucial factor in the development and maintenance of chronic pain. The importance of learning mechanisms is described. Mechanism-based individualized treatment approaches for patients with hypertensive stress reactivity are also critically discussed.
Collapse
|
11
|
Noel M, Chambers CT, Petter M, McGrath PJ, Klein RM, Stewart SH. Pain is not over when the needle ends: a review and preliminary model of acute pain memory development in childhood. Pain Manag 2012; 2:487-97. [DOI: 10.2217/pmt.12.41] [Citation(s) in RCA: 50] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/17/2023] Open
Abstract
SUMMARY Over the past several decades, the field of pediatric pain has made impressive advances in our understanding of the pain experience of the developing child, as well as the devastating impact of inadequately managed pain early in life. It is now well recognized that, from infancy, children are capable of developing implicit memories of pain that can influence their subsequent reactions to pain. The present review provides a synthesis of selected studies that made a significant impact on this field of inquiry, with particular emphasis on recent clinical and laboratory-based experimental research examining children’s explicit autobiographical memories for acute pain. Research has begun to move towards improving the precision with which children at risk for developing negatively estimated pain memories can be identified, given the adverse influence these memories can have on subsequent pain experiences. As such, several fear- and anxiety-related child and parent variables implicated in this process are discussed, and avenues for future research and clinical intervention are identified throughout. Finally, a preliminary empirically and theoretically derived model of acute pain memory development in childhood is presented to parsimoniously summarize the evidence accumulated to date and guide future investigation in this area.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Melanie Noel
- Department of Psychology, Dalhousie University, Life Sciences Centre, PO Box 15000, Halifax, Nova Scotia, B3H 4R2, Canada
| | - Christine T Chambers
- Department of Psychology, Dalhousie University, Life Sciences Centre, PO Box 15000, Halifax, Nova Scotia, B3H 4R2, Canada
- Centre for Pediatric Pain Research, IWK Health Centre, 5850/5980, University Avenue, Halifax, Nova Scotia, B3K 6R8, Canada
- Department of Pediatrics, Dalhousie University and the IWK Health Centre, 5850 University Avenue, PO Box 9700, B3K 6R8, Canada
- Department of Psychiatry, Dalhousie University, 5909 Veterans’ Memorial Lane, 8th floor, Abbie J Lane Memorial Building QEII Health Sciences Centre, Halifax, Nova Scotia, B3H 2E2, Canada
| | - Mark Petter
- Department of Psychology, Dalhousie University, Life Sciences Centre, PO Box 15000, Halifax, Nova Scotia, B3H 4R2, Canada
- Centre for Pediatric Pain Research, IWK Health Centre, 5850/5980, University Avenue, Halifax, Nova Scotia, B3K 6R8, Canada
| | - Patrick J McGrath
- Department of Psychology, Dalhousie University, Life Sciences Centre, PO Box 15000, Halifax, Nova Scotia, B3H 4R2, Canada
- Centre for Pediatric Pain Research, IWK Health Centre, 5850/5980, University Avenue, Halifax, Nova Scotia, B3K 6R8, Canada
- Department of Pediatrics, Dalhousie University and the IWK Health Centre, 5850 University Avenue, PO Box 9700, B3K 6R8, Canada
- Department of Psychiatry, Dalhousie University, 5909 Veterans’ Memorial Lane, 8th floor, Abbie J Lane Memorial Building QEII Health Sciences Centre, Halifax, Nova Scotia, B3H 2E2, Canada
- Research, IWK Health Centre, 5850/5980 University Avenue, PO Box 9700, Halifax, NS B3K 6R8, Canada
| | - Raymond M Klein
- Department of Psychology, Dalhousie University, Life Sciences Centre, PO Box 15000, Halifax, Nova Scotia, B3H 4R2, Canada
| | - Sherry H Stewart
- Department of Psychology, Dalhousie University, Life Sciences Centre, PO Box 15000, Halifax, Nova Scotia, B3H 4R2, Canada
- Department of Psychiatry, Dalhousie University, 5909 Veterans’ Memorial Lane, 8th floor, Abbie J Lane Memorial Building QEII Health Sciences Centre, Halifax, Nova Scotia, B3H 2E2, Canada
- Department of Community Health & Epidemiology, Centre for Clinical Research, 5790 University Avenue, Halifax, NS B3H 1V7, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Noel M, Chambers CT, McGrath PJ, Klein RM, Stewart SH. The influence of children’s pain memories on subsequent pain experience. Pain 2012; 153:1563-1572. [DOI: 10.1016/j.pain.2012.02.020] [Citation(s) in RCA: 103] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/05/2011] [Revised: 02/09/2012] [Accepted: 02/16/2012] [Indexed: 10/28/2022]
|
13
|
Noel M, Chambers CT, McGrath PJ, Klein RM, Stewart SH. The Role of State Anxiety in Children's Memories for Pain. J Pediatr Psychol 2012; 37:567-79. [DOI: 10.1093/jpepsy/jss006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 56] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/09/2023] Open
|
14
|
Van Damme S, Crombez G, Eccleston C, Koster EHW. Hypervigilance to Learned Pain Signals: A Componential Analysis. THE JOURNAL OF PAIN 2006; 7:346-57. [PMID: 16632324 DOI: 10.1016/j.jpain.2005.12.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 57] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/09/2005] [Revised: 12/16/2005] [Accepted: 12/22/2005] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
Abstract
UNLABELLED We report 2 experiments investigating hypervigilance to pain signals in healthy individuals. In order to allow a detailed analysis, we decomposed attention in 3 different processes: 1) initial shifting, 2) engagement, and 3) disengagement. We used a spatial cueing paradigm in which the location of targets is validly or invalidly predicted by spatial cues. In the first experiment, cues were differentially conditioned to create pain signals, allowing the investigation of engagement and disengagement of attention. Because this procedure does not allow an adequate isolation of the shift component, we introduced a new adaptation of the spatial cueing paradigm in the second experiment, in which targets instead of cues were differentially conditioned. We replicated previous findings, showing enhanced engagement to and retarded disengagement from pain signals compared to control signals. In addition, we demonstrated that participants were still hypervigilant to pain signals after extinction. We were not able to demonstrate speeded shifting to pain signals. Instead we found a generally faster detection of pain signals compared to control signals. We conclude that hypervilance to pain signals does not emerge as rapid initial shifting to the pain signal but rather as enhanced processing once it is detected. Theoretical and clinical implications of these findings are discussed. PERSPECTIVE We investigated the fundamental processes associated with hypervigilance to pain. Our findings suggest that treatment approaches using attention techniques should not focus on preventing patients to shift their attention to the pain but rather on diminishing the threat value and learning to disengage from their pain.
Collapse
|
15
|
Carli G, Suman AL, Biasi G, Marcolongo R. Reactivity to superficial and deep stimuli in patients with chronic musculoskeletal pain. Pain 2002; 100:259-269. [PMID: 12467997 DOI: 10.1016/s0304-3959(02)00297-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 114] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
Abstract
In this study, we evaluated pain sensitivity in patients with fibromyalgia or other types of chronic, diffuse musculoskeletal pain to establish whether fibromyalgia represents the end of a continuum of dysfunction in the nociceptive system. One hundred and forty five patients and 22 healthy subjects (HS) completed an epidemiological questionnaire to provide information about fatigue, stiffness, sleep, the intensity of pain (VAS 0-100) and its extent both at onset and at present. Algometry was performed at all American College of Rheumatology (ACR) tender points and at ten control points. Patients were divided into five main groups: fibromyalgia (FS) patients, secondary-concomitant fibromyalgia (SCFS) patients, patients with widespread pain (WP) but not reaching the ACR criterion of 11 tender points, patients with diffuse multiregional pain (MP) not reaching the ACR criteria (widespread pain, tender point counts), and patients with multiregional pain associated with at least 11 tender points (MPTE). von Frey monofilaments were used to assess superficial punctate pressure pain thresholds. Heat and cold pain thresholds were determined with a thermal stimulator. Ischemic pain was assessed by the cold pressure test and the submaximal effort tourniquet test. The scores for stiffness and present pain intensity gradually increased concomitantly with the increase in tender point count and pain extent. The pressure pain thresholds for positive tender and positive control points were significantly lower in the SCFS, FS and MPTE groups than in HS, MP and WP groups, the latter three groups displaying similar values. In all groups, there were no differences in pain thresholds between positive tender and positive control points. The heat pain threshold and the pain threshold in the cold pressure test were lower in the FS and SCFS groups than in HS. The cold pressure tolerance was lower in patients with widespread pain than in HS. In the von Frey test, all patient groups except MP had similar values, which were significantly lower than in HS. Finally, all patient groups displayed lower tourniquet tolerance than HS. In each psychophysical test, patients with widespread pain and patients with multiregional pain showed similar thresholds; however, the thresholds in the MP or MPTE groups differed from those in the FS and SCFS groups. In the FS group, pain thresholds and pain tolerance did not differ according to the presence of ongoing pain at the stimulated site and were not correlated to ongoing pain. The results indicate that dysfunction in the nociceptive system is already present in patients with multiregional pain with a low tender point count; it becomes more and more severe as the positive tender point count and pain extent increase and it is maximal in fibromyalgia patients.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Giancarlo Carli
- Istituto di Fisiologia Umana and Istituto di Reumatologia, Università degli Studi, 53100, Siena, Italy
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
16
|
Abstract
The psychometric distinctiveness of self-reported anxiety and depression in patients with chronic pain was investigated. The item-level responses of 220 patients with heterogeneous pain conditions from the Beck Depression Inventory and State--Trait Anxiety Inventory State--Anxiety scale were submitted to common factor analysis. Three first-order factors were identified: depression, anxiety-absent, and anxiety-present. One second-order factor of negative affect was also identified. Correlations of first-order factor scores with other psychometric measures suggested only minor distinctiveness. The findings indicated that it is possible to distinguish anxiety and depression psychometrically in patients with chronic pain but suggested that negative affect may be the primary underlying construct of the affective experience of these patients.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- D V Nelson
- Department of Anesthesiology, University of Texas--Houston Science Center, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
17
|
Brody S, Angrilli A, Weiss U, Birbaumer N, Mini A, Veit R, Rau H. Somatotosensory evoked potentials during baroreceptor stimulation in chronic low back pain patients and normal controls. Int J Psychophysiol 1997; 25:201-10. [PMID: 9105944 DOI: 10.1016/s0167-8760(96)00740-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 36] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/04/2023]
Abstract
Nineteen chronic low back pain patients (aged 19-63) and 17 controls (aged 20-41) received electrical pain stimuli during manipulation of their carotid baroreceptors. The non-invasive mechanical manipulation of baroreceptors, using the PRES technique (Phase Related External Suction), simulates the end-effects of phasic blood pressure changes. This technique was developed to assess pain responses induced by changes in blood pressure without the typical shortcomings of pharmacological manipulation or lack of a control condition. During maximum baroreceptor activity, there was an unexpected increase in the amplitude of the somatosensory evoked potentials (SEPs) elicited by the electrical pain stimuli condition (N150-P260 peak-to-peak). In most other studies the opposite effect was found, with decreased pain responses during maximum baroreceptor activity. The chronic pain group reported greater pain during highest baroreceptor activation than did the controls. In addition, the chronic pain group showed lower diastolic blood pressure. To determine whether pain and baroreceptor responses observed in the chronic pain group depended on lower blood pressure levels, a second experiment with a non-clinical sample was performed. Results showed that lower tonic blood pressures are associated with greater baroreceptor activity amplifying pain, while higher blood pressure is associated with pain dampening during high baroreceptor activity. Data suggested that the differences in pain responses found in low back pain patients were associated with their lower tonic blood pressure levels. It is proposed that in general, lower blood pressures may be associated with greater pain during baroreceptor activation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- S Brody
- Institute of Medical Psychology and Behavioral Neurobiology, Eberhard-Karls University of Tübingen, Germany
| | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
18
|
|