1
|
Chen CC, Berteletti I, Hyde DC. Neural evidence of core foundations and conceptual change in preschool numeracy. Dev Sci 2024; 27:e13556. [PMID: 39105368 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13556] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/25/2023] [Revised: 07/15/2024] [Accepted: 07/17/2024] [Indexed: 08/07/2024]
Abstract
Symbolic numeracy first emerges as children learn the meanings of number words and how to use them to precisely count sets of objects. This development starts before children enter school and forms a foundation for lifelong mathematics achievement. Despite its importance, exactly how children acquire this basic knowledge is unclear. Here we test competing theories of early number learning by measuring event-related brain potentials during a novel number word-quantity comparison task in 3-4-year-old preschool children (N = 128). We find several qualitative differences in neural processing of number by conceptual stage of development. Specifically, we find differences in early attention-related parietal electrophysiology (N1), suggesting that less conceptually advanced children process arrays as individual objects and more advanced children distribute attention over the entire set. Subsequently, we find that only more conceptually advanced children show later-going frontal (N2) sensitivity to the numerical-distance relationship between the number word and visual quantity. The nature of this response suggested that exact rather than approximate numerical meanings were being associated with number words over frontal sites. No evidence of numerical distance effects was observed over posterior scalp sites. Together these results suggest that children may engage parallel individuation of objects to learn the meanings of the first few number words, but, ultimately, create new exact cardinal value representations for number words that cannot be defined in terms of core, nonverbal number systems. More broadly, these results document an interaction between attentional and general cognitive mechanisms in cognitive development. RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTS: Conceptual development in numeracy is associated with a shift in attention from objects to sets. Children acquire meanings of the first few number words through associations with parallel attentional individuation of objects. Understanding of cardinality is associated with attentional processing of sets rather than individuals. Brain signatures suggest children attribute exact rather than approximate numerical meanings to the first few number words. Number-quantity relationship processing for the first few number words is evident in frontal but not parietal scalp electrophysiology of young children.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chi-Chuan Chen
- Department of Psychology, University of Illinois Urbana-Champaign, Champaign, Illinois, USA
| | - Ilaria Berteletti
- Educational Neuroscience Program, Gallaudet University, Washington, DC, USA
| | - Daniel C Hyde
- Department of Psychology, University of Illinois Urbana-Champaign, Champaign, Illinois, USA
- Neuroscience Program, University of Illinois Urbana-Champaign, Urbana, Illinois, USA
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Piantadosi ST. The algorithmic origins of counting. Child Dev 2023; 94:1472-1490. [PMID: 37984061 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.14031] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/20/2022] [Revised: 09/16/2023] [Accepted: 09/19/2023] [Indexed: 11/22/2023]
Abstract
The study of how children learn numbers has yielded one of the most productive research programs in cognitive development, spanning empirical and computational methods, as well as nativist and empiricist philosophies. This paper provides a tutorial on how to think computationally about learning models in a domain like number, where learners take finite data and go far beyond what they directly observe or perceive. To illustrate, this paper then outlines a model which acquires a counting procedure using observations of sets and words, extending the proposal of Piantadosi et al. (2012). This new version of the model responds to several critiques of the original work and outlines an approach which is likely appropriate for acquiring further aspects of mathematics.
Collapse
|
3
|
Gerbrand A, Gredebäck G, Lindskog M. Recognition of small numbers in subset knowers Cardinal knowledge in early childhood. ROYAL SOCIETY OPEN SCIENCE 2023; 10:230474. [PMID: 37885983 PMCID: PMC10598441 DOI: 10.1098/rsos.230474] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/14/2023] [Accepted: 10/06/2023] [Indexed: 10/28/2023]
Abstract
Previous research suggests that subset-knowers have an approximate understanding of small numbers. However, it is still unclear exactly what subset-knowers understand about small numbers. To investigate this further, we tested 133 participants, ages 2.6-4 years, on a newly developed eye-tracking task targeting cardinal recognition. Participants were presented with two sets differing in cardinality (1-4 items) and asked to find a specific cardinality. Our main finding showed that on a group level, subset-knowers could identify all presented targets at rates above chance, further supporting that subset-knowers understand several of the basic principles of small numbers. Exploratory analyses tentatively suggest that 1-knowers could identify the targets 1 and 2, but struggled when the target was 3 and 4, whereas 2-knowers and above could identify all targets at rates above chance. This might tentatively suggest that subset-knowers have an approximate understanding of numbers that is just (i.e. +1) above their current knower level. We discuss the implications of these results at length.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anton Gerbrand
- Uppsala Child and Babylab, Uppsala Universitet, Department of psychology, Sweden
| | - Gustaf Gredebäck
- Uppsala Child and Babylab, Uppsala Universitet, Department of psychology, Sweden
| | - Marcus Lindskog
- Uppsala Child and Babylab, Uppsala Universitet, Department of psychology, Sweden
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Lee SH, Kim D, Opfer JE, Pitt MA, Myung JI. A number-line task with a Bayesian active learning algorithm provides insights into the development of non-symbolic number estimation. Psychon Bull Rev 2022; 29:971-984. [PMID: 34918270 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-021-02041-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 11/11/2021] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Abstract
To characterize numerical representations, the number-line task asks participants to estimate the location of a given number on a line flanked with zero and an upper-bound number. An open question is whether estimates for symbolic numbers (e.g., Arabic numerals) and non-symbolic numbers (e.g., number of dots) rely on common processes with a common developmental pathway. To address this question, we explored whether well-established findings in symbolic number-line estimation generalize to non-symbolic number-line estimation. For exhaustive investigations without sacrificing data quality, we applied a novel Bayesian active learning algorithm, dubbed Gaussian process active learning (GPAL), that adaptively optimizes experimental designs. The results showed that the non-symbolic number estimation in participants of diverse ages (5-73 years old, n = 238) exhibited three characteristic features of symbolic number estimation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sang Ho Lee
- Department of Psychology, The Ohio State University, 212 Psychology Building, 1835 Neil Avenue, Columbus, OH, 43210, USA.
| | - Dan Kim
- Department of Psychology, The Ohio State University, 212 Psychology Building, 1835 Neil Avenue, Columbus, OH, 43210, USA
| | - John E Opfer
- Department of Psychology, The Ohio State University, 212 Psychology Building, 1835 Neil Avenue, Columbus, OH, 43210, USA
| | - Mark A Pitt
- Department of Psychology, The Ohio State University, 212 Psychology Building, 1835 Neil Avenue, Columbus, OH, 43210, USA
| | - Jay I Myung
- Department of Psychology, The Ohio State University, 212 Psychology Building, 1835 Neil Avenue, Columbus, OH, 43210, USA
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Schröder E, Gredebäck G, Forssman L, Lindskog M. Predicting children's emerging understanding of numbers. Dev Sci 2021; 25:e13207. [PMID: 34870876 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13207] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/10/2020] [Revised: 10/26/2021] [Accepted: 11/08/2021] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
How do children construct a concept of natural numbers? Past research addressing this question has mainly focused on understanding how children come to acquire the cardinality principle. However, at that point children already understand the first number words and have a rudimentary natural number concept in place. The question therefore remains; what gets children's number learning off the ground? We therefore, based on previous empirical and theoretical work, tested which factors predict the first stages of children's natural number understanding. We assessed if children's expressive vocabulary, visuospatial working memory, and ANS (Approximate number system) acuity at 18 months of age could predict their natural number knowledge at 2.5 years of age. We found that early expressive vocabulary and visuospatial working memory were important for later number knowledge. The results of the current study add to a growing body of literature showing the importance of language in children's learning about numbers.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Elin Schröder
- Department of Psychology, Uppsala University, Uppsala, Sweden
| | | | - Linda Forssman
- Department of Psychology, Uppsala University, Uppsala, Sweden
| | - Marcus Lindskog
- Department of Psychology, Uppsala University, Uppsala, Sweden
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Hyde DC, Mou Y, Berteletti I, Spelke ES, Dehaene S, Piazza M. Testing the role of symbols in preschool numeracy: An experimental computer-based intervention study. PLoS One 2021; 16:e0259775. [PMID: 34780526 PMCID: PMC8592431 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0259775] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/29/2021] [Accepted: 10/27/2021] [Indexed: 01/29/2023] Open
Abstract
Numeracy is of critical importance for scholastic success and modern-day living, but the precise mechanisms that drive its development are poorly understood. Here we used novel experimental training methods to begin to investigate the role of symbols in the development of numeracy in preschool-aged children. We assigned pre-school children in the U.S. and Italy (N = 215; Mean age = 49.15 months) to play one of five versions of a computer-based numerical comparison game for two weeks. The different versions of the game were equated on basic features of gameplay and demands but systematically varied in numerical content. Critically, some versions included non-symbolic numerical comparisons only, while others combined non-symbolic numerical comparison with symbolic aids of various types. Before and after training we assessed four components of early numeracy: counting proficiency, non-symbolic numerical comparison, one-to-one correspondence, and arithmetic set transformation. We found that overall children showed improvement in most of these components after completing these short trainings. However, children trained on numerical comparisons with symbolic aids made larger gains on assessments of one-to-one correspondence and arithmetic transformation compared to children whose training involved non-symbolic numerical comparison only. Further exploratory analyses suggested that, although there were no major differences between children trained with verbal symbols (e.g., verbal counting) and non-verbal visuo-spatial symbols (i.e., abacus counting), the gains in one-to-one correspondence may have been driven by abacus training, while the gains in non-verbal arithmetic transformations may have been driven by verbal training. These results provide initial evidence that the introduction of symbols may contribute to the emergence of numeracy by enhancing the capacity for thinking about exact equality and the numerical effects of set transformations. More broadly, this study provides an empirical basis to motivate further focused study of the processes by which children’s mastery of symbols influences children’s developing mastery of numeracy.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Daniel C. Hyde
- Department of Psychology, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Champaign, IL, United States of America
- Neuroscience Program, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Champaign, IL, United States of America
- * E-mail:
| | - Yi Mou
- Department of Psychology, Guangdong Provincial Key Laboratory of Social Cognitive Neuroscience and Mental Health, Sun Yat-sen University, Guangzhou, China
| | - Ilaria Berteletti
- Educational Neuroscience Program, Gallaudet University, Washington, D.C, United States of America
| | - Elizabeth S. Spelke
- Department of Psychology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, United States of America
| | - Stanislas Dehaene
- Cognitive Neuroimaging Unit, CEA DRF/I2BM, INSERM, NeuroSpin Center, Université Paris-Sud, Université Paris-Saclay, Gif/Yvette, France
- Collège de France, Paris, France
| | - Manuela Piazza
- Center for Mind/Brain Sciences, University of Trento, Rovereto, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Wagemann J, Raggatz J. First-person dimensions of mental agency in visual counting of moving objects. Cogn Process 2021; 22:453-473. [PMID: 33818664 PMCID: PMC8324628 DOI: 10.1007/s10339-021-01020-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/12/2020] [Accepted: 02/16/2021] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
Counting objects, especially moving ones, is an important capacity that has been intensively explored in experimental psychology and related disciplines. The common approach is to trace the three counting principles (estimating, subitizing, serial counting) back to functional constructs like the Approximate Number System and the Object Tracking System. While usually attempts are made to explain these competing models by computational processes at the neural level, their first-person dimensions have been hardly investigated so far. However, explanatory gaps in both psychological and philosophical terms may suggest a methodologically complementary approach that systematically incorporates introspective data. For example, the mental-action debate raises the question of whether mental activity plays only a marginal role in otherwise automatic cognitive processes or if it can be developed in such a way that it can count as genuine mental action. To address this question not only theoretically, we conducted an exploratory study with a moving-dots task and analyze the self-report data qualitatively and quantitatively on different levels. Building on this, a multi-layered, consciousness-immanent model of counting is presented, which integrates the various counting principles and concretizes mental agency as developing from pre-reflective to increasingly conscious mental activity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Johannes Wagemann
- Institute for Waldorf Education, Inclusion and Interculturalism, Alanus University, Campus Mannheim, Am Exerzierplatz 21, 68167 Mannheim, Germany
| | - Jonas Raggatz
- Institute for Waldorf Education, Inclusion and Interculturalism, Alanus University, Campus Mannheim, Am Exerzierplatz 21, 68167 Mannheim, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Piantadosi ST. The computational origin of representation. Minds Mach (Dordr) 2021; 31:1-58. [PMID: 34305318 PMCID: PMC8300595 DOI: 10.1007/s11023-020-09540-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/12/2019] [Accepted: 08/29/2020] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Abstract
Each of our theories of mental representation provides some insight into how the mind works. However, these insights often seem incompatible, as the debates between symbolic, dynamical, emergentist, sub-symbolic, and grounded approaches to cognition attest. Mental representations-whatever they are-must share many features with each of our theories of representation, and yet there are few hypotheses about how a synthesis could be possible. Here, I develop a theory of the underpinnings of symbolic cognition that shows how sub-symbolic dynamics may give rise to higher-level cognitive representations of structures, systems of knowledge, and algorithmic processes. This theory implements a version of conceptual role semantics by positing an internal universal representation language in which learners may create mental models to capture dynamics they observe in the world. The theory formalizes one account of how truly novel conceptual content may arise, allowing us to explain how even elementary logical and computational operations may be learned from a more primitive basis. I provide an implementation that learns to represent a variety of structures, including logic, number, kinship trees, regular languages, context-free languages, domains of theories like magnetism, dominance hierarchies, list structures, quantification, and computational primitives like repetition, reversal, and recursion. This account is based on simple discrete dynamical processes that could be implemented in a variety of different physical or biological systems. In particular, I describe how the required dynamics can be directly implemented in a connectionist framework. The resulting theory provides an "assembly language" for cognition, where high-level theories of symbolic computation can be implemented in simple dynamics that themselves could be encoded in biologically plausible systems.
Collapse
|
9
|
Is thirty-two three tens and two ones? The embedded structure of cardinal numbers. Cognition 2020; 203:104331. [PMID: 32590201 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104331] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/20/2018] [Revised: 04/15/2020] [Accepted: 05/18/2020] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Abstract
The acquisition and representation of natural numbers have been a central topic in cognitive science. However, a key question in this topic about how humans acquire the capacity to understand that numbers make 'infinite use of finite means' (or that numbers are generative) has been left unanswered. While previous theories rely on the idea of the successor principle, we propose an alternative hypothesis that children's understanding of the syntactic rules for building complex numerals-or numerical syntax-is a crucial foundation for the acquisition of number concepts. In two independent studies, we assessed children's understanding of numerical syntax by probing their knowledge about the embedded structure of cardinal numbers using a novel task called Give-a-number Base-10 (Give-N10). In Give-N10, children were asked to give a large number of items (e.g., 32 items) from a pool that is organized in sets of ten items. Children's knowledge about the embedded structure of numbers (e.g., knowing that thirty-two items are composed of three tens and two ones) was assessed from their ability to use those sets. Study 1 tested English-speaking 4- to 10-year-olds and revealed that children's understanding of the embedded structure of numbers emerges relatively late in development (several months into kindergarten), beyond when they are capable of making a semantic induction over a local sequence of numbers. Moreover, performance in Give-N10 was predicted by other task measures that assessed children's knowledge about the syntactic rules that govern numerals (such as counting fluency), demonstrating the validity of the measure. In Study 2, this association was tested again in monolingual Korean kindergarteners (5-6 years), as we aimed to test the same effect in a language with a highly regular numeral system. It replicated the association between Give-N10 performance and counting fluency, and it also demonstrated that Korean-speaking children understand the embedded structure of cardinal numbers earlier in the acquisition path than English-speaking peers, suggesting that regularity in numerical syntax facilitates the acquisition of generative properties of numbers. Based on these observations and our theoretical analysis of the literature, we propose that the syntax for building complex numerals, not the successor principle, represents a structural platform for numerical thinking in young children.
Collapse
|
10
|
Vanderwegen J, Van Nuffelen G, Elen R, De Bodt M. The Influence of Age, Sex, Visual Feedback, Bulb Position, and the Order of Testing on Maximum Anterior and Posterior Tongue Strength in Healthy Belgian Children. Dysphagia 2019; 34:834-851. [DOI: 10.1007/s00455-019-09976-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/25/2018] [Accepted: 01/02/2019] [Indexed: 02/06/2023]
|
11
|
Asmuth J, Morson EM, Rips LJ. Children's Understanding of the Natural Numbers' Structure. Cogn Sci 2018; 42:1945-1973. [PMID: 29974507 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.12615] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/16/2016] [Revised: 12/12/2017] [Accepted: 02/19/2018] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Abstract
When young children attempt to locate numbers along a number line, they show logarithmic (or other compressive) placement. For example, the distance between "5" and "10" is larger than the distance between "75" and "80." This has often been explained by assuming that children have a logarithmically scaled mental representation of number (e.g., Berteletti, Lucangeli, Piazza, Dehaene, & Zorzi, ; Siegler & Opfer, ). However, several investigators have questioned this argument (e.g., Barth & Paladino, ; Cantlon, Cordes, Libertus, & Brannon, ; Cohen & Blanc-Goldhammer, ). We show here that children prefer linear number lines over logarithmic lines when they do not have to deal with the meanings of individual numerals (i.e., number symbols, such as "5" or "80"). In Experiments 1 and 2, when 5- and 6-year-olds choose between number lines in a forced-choice task, they prefer linear to logarithmic and exponential displays. However, this preference does not persist when Experiment 3 presents the same lines without reference to numbers, and children simply choose which line they like best. In Experiments 4 and 5, children position beads on a number line to indicate how the integers 1-100 are arranged. The bead placement of 4- and 5-year-olds is better fit by a linear than by a logarithmic model. We argue that previous results from the number-line task may depend on strategies specific to the task.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Emily M Morson
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Indiana University
| | - Lance J Rips
- Department of Psychology, Northwestern University
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Abstract
Recent research has begun to investigate the impact of different formats for rational numbers on the processes by which people make relational judgments about quantitative relations. DeWolf, Bassok, and Holyoak (Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144(1), 127-150, 2015) found that accuracy on a relation identification task was highest when fractions were presented with countable sets, whereas accuracy was relatively low for all conditions where decimals were presented. However, it is unclear what processing strategies underlie these disparities in accuracy. We report an experiment that used eye-tracking methods to externalize the strategies that are evoked by different types of rational numbers for different types of quantities (discrete vs. continuous). Results showed that eye-movement behavior during the task was jointly determined by image and number format. Discrete images elicited a counting strategy for both fractions and decimals, but this strategy led to higher accuracy only for fractions. Continuous images encouraged magnitude estimation and comparison, but to a greater degree for decimals than fractions. This strategy led to decreased accuracy for both number formats. By analyzing participants' eye movements when they viewed a relational context and made decisions, we were able to obtain an externalized representation of the strategic choices evoked by different ontological types of entities and different types of rational numbers. Our findings using eye-tracking measures enable us to go beyond previous studies based on accuracy data alone, demonstrating that quantitative properties of images and the different formats for rational numbers jointly influence strategies that generate eye-movement behavior.
Collapse
|
13
|
Graziano M. Possible Explanations. DUAL-PROCESS THEORIES OF NUMERICAL COGNITION 2018:89-121. [DOI: 10.1007/978-3-319-96797-4_5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 09/02/2023]
|
14
|
Barner D. Language, procedures, and the non-perceptual origin of number word meanings. JOURNAL OF CHILD LANGUAGE 2017; 44:553-590. [PMID: 28376934 DOI: 10.1017/s0305000917000058] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/07/2023]
Abstract
Perceptual representations of objects and approximate magnitudes are often invoked as building blocks that children combine to acquire the positive integers. Systems of numerical perception are either assumed to contain the logical foundations of arithmetic innately, or to supply the basis for their induction. I propose an alternative to this framework, and argue that the integers are not learned from perceptual systems, but arise to explain perception. Using cross-linguistic and developmental data, I show that small (~1-4) and large (~5+) numbers arise both historically and in individual children via distinct mechanisms, constituting independent learning problems, neither of which begins with perceptual building blocks. Children first learn small numbers using the same logic that supports other linguistic number marking (e.g. singular/plural). Years later, they infer the logic of counting from the relations between large number words and their roles in blind counting procedures, only incidentally associating number words with approximate magnitudes.
Collapse
|
15
|
Overmann KA. Number Concepts Are Constructed through Material Engagement: A Reply to Sutliff, Read, and Everett. CURRENT ANTHROPOLOGY 2016. [DOI: 10.1086/686529] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
|
16
|
Ebersbach M. Development of Children's Estimation Skills: The Ambiguous Role of Their Familiarity With Numerals. CHILD DEVELOPMENT PERSPECTIVES 2016. [DOI: 10.1111/cdep.12172] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
|
17
|
Schneider M, Beeres K, Coban L, Merz S, Susan Schmidt S, Stricker J, De Smedt B. Associations of non-symbolic and symbolic numerical magnitude processing with mathematical competence: a meta-analysis. Dev Sci 2016; 20. [PMID: 26768176 DOI: 10.1111/desc.12372] [Citation(s) in RCA: 308] [Impact Index Per Article: 38.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/01/2014] [Accepted: 08/29/2015] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Abstract
Many studies have investigated the association between numerical magnitude processing skills, as assessed by the numerical magnitude comparison task, and broader mathematical competence, e.g. counting, arithmetic, or algebra. Most correlations were positive but varied considerably in their strengths. It remains unclear whether and to what extent the strength of these associations differs systematically between non-symbolic and symbolic magnitude comparison tasks and whether age, magnitude comparison measures or mathematical competence measures are additional moderators. We investigated these questions by means of a meta-analysis. The literature search yielded 45 articles reporting 284 effect sizes found with 17,201 participants. Effect sizes were combined by means of a two-level random-effects regression model. The effect size was significantly higher for the symbolic (r = .302, 95% CI [.243, .361]) than for the non-symbolic (r = .241, 95% CI [.198, .284]) magnitude comparison task and decreased very slightly with age. The correlation was higher for solution rates and Weber fractions than for alternative measures of comparison proficiency. It was higher for mathematical competencies that rely more heavily on the processing of magnitudes (i.e. mental arithmetic and early mathematical abilities) than for others. The results support the view that magnitude processing is reliably associated with mathematical competence over the lifespan in a wide range of tasks, measures and mathematical subdomains. The association is stronger for symbolic than for non-symbolic numerical magnitude processing. So symbolic magnitude processing might be a more eligible candidate to be targeted by diagnostic screening instruments and interventions for school-aged children and for adults.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Leyla Coban
- Department of Psychology, University of Trier, Germany
| | - Simon Merz
- Department of Psychology, University of Trier, Germany
| | | | | | - Bert De Smedt
- Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, Parenting and Special Education Research Group, KU Leuven, University of Leuven, Belgium
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Matthews PG, Lewis MR, Hubbard EM. Individual Differences in Nonsymbolic Ratio Processing Predict Symbolic Math Performance. Psychol Sci 2015; 27:191-202. [DOI: 10.1177/0956797615617799] [Citation(s) in RCA: 66] [Impact Index Per Article: 7.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/19/2015] [Accepted: 10/21/2015] [Indexed: 02/01/2023] Open
Abstract
What basic capacities lay the foundation for advanced numerical cognition? Are there basic nonsymbolic abilities that support the understanding of advanced numerical concepts, such as fractions? To date, most theories have posited that previously identified core numerical systems, such as the approximate number system (ANS), are ill-suited for learning fraction concepts. However, recent research in developmental psychology and neuroscience has revealed a ratio-processing system (RPS) that is sensitive to magnitudes of nonsymbolic ratios and may be ideally suited for supporting fraction concepts. We provide evidence for this hypothesis by showing that individual differences in RPS acuity predict performance on four measures of mathematical competence, including a university entrance exam in algebra. We suggest that the nonsymbolic RPS may support symbolic fraction understanding much as the ANS supports whole-number concepts. Thus, even abstract mathematical concepts, such as fractions, may be grounded not only in higher-order logic and language, but also in basic nonsymbolic processing abilities.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Mark Rose Lewis
- Department of Educational Psychology, University of Wisconsin–Madison
| | - Edward M. Hubbard
- Department of Educational Psychology, University of Wisconsin–Madison
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Lee HS, DeWolf M, Bassok M, Holyoak KJ. Conceptual and procedural distinctions between fractions and decimals: A cross-national comparison. Cognition 2015; 147:57-69. [PMID: 26615331 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.11.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/08/2015] [Revised: 10/28/2015] [Accepted: 11/11/2015] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
Abstract
Previous work has shown that adults in the United States process fractions and decimals in distinctly different ways, both in tasks requiring magnitude judgments and in tasks requiring mathematical reasoning. In particular, fractions and decimals are preferentially used to model discrete and continuous entities, respectively. The current study tested whether similar alignments between the format of rational numbers and quantitative ontology hold for Korean college students, who differ from American students in educational background, overall mathematical proficiency, language, and measurement conventions. A textbook analysis and the results of five experiments revealed that the alignments found in the United States were replicated in South Korea. The present study provides strong evidence for the existence of a natural alignment between entity type and the format of rational numbers. This alignment, and other processing differences between fractions and decimals, cannot be attributed to the specifics of education, language, and measurement units, which differ greatly between the United States and South Korea.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hee Seung Lee
- Department of Education, Yonsei University, Republic of Korea.
| | - Melissa DeWolf
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles, United States
| | - Miriam Bassok
- Department of Psychology, University of Washington, United States
| | - Keith J Holyoak
- Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles, United States
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Overmann KA. Numerosity Structures the Expression of Quantity in Lexical Numbers and Grammatical Number. CURRENT ANTHROPOLOGY 2015. [DOI: 10.1086/683092] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
|
21
|
|
22
|
Ebersbach M. Evidence for a Spatial–Numerical Association in Kindergartners Using a Number Line Task. JOURNAL OF COGNITION AND DEVELOPMENT 2015. [DOI: 10.1080/15248372.2013.805134] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
|
23
|
Moore RD, Drollette ES, Scudder MR, Bharij A, Hillman CH. The influence of cardiorespiratory fitness on strategic, behavioral, and electrophysiological indices of arithmetic cognition in preadolescent children. Front Hum Neurosci 2014; 8:258. [PMID: 24829556 PMCID: PMC4017138 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00258] [Citation(s) in RCA: 31] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/06/2013] [Accepted: 04/07/2014] [Indexed: 12/19/2022] Open
Abstract
The current study investigated the influence of cardiorespiratory fitness on arithmetic cognition in forty 9-10 year old children. Measures included a standardized mathematics achievement test to assess conceptual and computational knowledge, self-reported strategy selection, and an experimental arithmetic verification task (including small and large addition problems), which afforded the measurement of event-related brain potentials (ERPs). No differences in math achievement were observed as a function of fitness level, but all children performed better on math concepts relative to math computation. Higher fit children reported using retrieval more often to solve large arithmetic problems, relative to lower fit children. During the arithmetic verification task, higher fit children exhibited superior performance for large problems, as evidenced by greater d' scores, while all children exhibited decreased accuracy and longer reaction time for large relative to small problems, and incorrect relative to correct solutions. On the electrophysiological level, modulations of early (P1, N170) and late ERP components (P3, N400) were observed as a function of problem size and solution correctness. Higher fit children exhibited selective modulations for N170, P3, and N400 amplitude relative to lower fit children, suggesting that fitness influences symbolic encoding, attentional resource allocation and semantic processing during arithmetic tasks. The current study contributes to the fitness-cognition literature by demonstrating that the benefits of cardiorespiratory fitness extend to arithmetic cognition, which has important implications for the educational environment and the context of learning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- R Davis Moore
- Neurocognitive Kinesiology Laboratory, Kinesiology, University of Illinois Urbana, IL, USA
| | - Eric S Drollette
- Neurocognitive Kinesiology Laboratory, Kinesiology, University of Illinois Urbana, IL, USA
| | - Mark R Scudder
- Neurocognitive Kinesiology Laboratory, Kinesiology, University of Illinois Urbana, IL, USA
| | - Aashiv Bharij
- Neurocognitive Kinesiology Laboratory, Kinesiology, University of Illinois Urbana, IL, USA
| | - Charles H Hillman
- Neurocognitive Kinesiology Laboratory, Kinesiology, University of Illinois Urbana, IL, USA
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Piantadosi ST, Jara-Ettinger J, Gibson E. Children's learning of number words in an indigenous farming-foraging group. Dev Sci 2014; 17:553-63. [PMID: 24766463 DOI: 10.1111/desc.12078] [Citation(s) in RCA: 33] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/11/2012] [Accepted: 02/26/2013] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Abstract
We show that children in the Tsimane', a farming-foraging group in the Bolivian rain-forest, learn number words along a similar developmental trajectory to children from industrialized countries. Tsimane' children successively acquire the first three or four number words before fully learning how counting works. However, their learning is substantially delayed relative to children from the United States, Russia, and Japan. The presence of a similar developmental trajectory likely indicates that the incremental stages of numerical knowledge - but not their timing - reflect a fundamental property of number concept acquisition which is relatively independent of language, culture, age, and early education.
Collapse
|
25
|
Izard V, Streri A, Spelke ES. Toward exact number: young children use one-to-one correspondence to measure set identity but not numerical equality. Cogn Psychol 2014; 72:27-53. [PMID: 24680885 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2014.01.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 37] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/17/2012] [Revised: 01/23/2014] [Accepted: 01/30/2014] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Abstract
Exact integer concepts are fundamental to a wide array of human activities, but their origins are obscure. Some have proposed that children are endowed with a system of natural number concepts, whereas others have argued that children construct these concepts by mastering verbal counting or other numeric symbols. This debate remains unresolved, because it is difficult to test children's mastery of the logic of integer concepts without using symbols to enumerate large sets, and the symbols themselves could be a source of difficulty for children. Here, we introduce a new method, focusing on large quantities and avoiding the use of words or other symbols for numbers, to study children's understanding of an essential property underlying integer concepts: the relation of exact numerical equality. Children aged 32-36 months, who possessed no symbols for exact numbers beyond 4, were given one-to-one correspondence cues to help them track a set of puppets, and their enumeration of the set was assessed by a non-verbal manual search task. Children used one-to-one correspondence relations to reconstruct exact quantities in sets of 5 or 6 objects, as long as the elements forming the sets remained the same individuals. In contrast, they failed to track exact quantities when one element was added, removed, or substituted for another. These results suggest an alternative to both nativist and symbol-based constructivist theories of the development of natural number concepts: Before learning symbols for exact numbers, children have a partial understanding of the properties of exact numbers.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Véronique Izard
- Laboratoire Psychologie de la Perception, Université Paris Descartes, Sorbonne Paris Cité, 45 rue des Saints-Pères, 75006 Paris, France; CNRS UMR 8158, 45 rue des Saints-Pères, 75006 Paris, France; Department of Psychology, Harvard University, 33 Kirkland St., Cambridge, MA 02138, USA.
| | - Arlette Streri
- Laboratoire Psychologie de la Perception, Université Paris Descartes, Sorbonne Paris Cité, 45 rue des Saints-Pères, 75006 Paris, France; CNRS UMR 8158, 45 rue des Saints-Pères, 75006 Paris, France
| | - Elizabeth S Spelke
- Department of Psychology, Harvard University, 33 Kirkland St., Cambridge, MA 02138, USA
| |
Collapse
|
26
|
Abstract
Number systems-such as the natural numbers, integers, rationals, reals, or complex numbers-play a foundational role in mathematics, but these systems can present difficulties for students. In the studies reported here, we probed the boundaries of people's concept of a number system by asking them whether "number lines" of varying shapes qualify as possible number systems. In Experiment 1, participants rated each of a set of number lines as a possible number system, where the number lines differed in their structures (a single straight line, a step-shaped line, a double line, or two branching structures) and in their boundedness (unbounded, bounded below, bounded above, bounded above and below, or circular). Participants also rated each of a group of mathematical properties (e.g., associativity) for its importance to number systems. Relational properties, such as associativity, predicted whether participants believed that particular forms were number systems, as did the forms' ability to support arithmetic operations, such as addition. In Experiment 2, we asked participants to produce properties that were important for number systems. Relational, operation, and use-based properties from this set again predicted ratings of whether the number lines were possible number systems. In Experiment 3, we found similar results when the number lines indicated the positions of the individual numbers. The results suggest that people believe that number systems should be well-behaved with respect to basic arithmetic operations, and that they reject systems for which these operations produce ambiguous answers. People care much less about whether the systems have particular numbers (e.g., 0) or sets of numbers (e.g., the positives).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lance J Rips
- Psychology Department, Northwestern University, 2029 Sheridan Road, Evanston, IL, 60208, USA,
| | | |
Collapse
|
27
|
Abstract
How does cross-linguistic variation in linguistic structure affect children's acquisition of early number word meanings? We tested this question by investigating number word learning in two unrelated languages that feature a tripartite singular-dual-plural distinction: Slovenian and Saudi Arabic. We found that learning dual morphology affects children's acquisition of the number word two in both languages, relative to English. Children who knew the meaning of two were surprisingly frequent in the dual languages, relative to English. Furthermore, Slovenian children were faster to learn two than children learning English, despite being less-competent counters. Finally, in both Slovenian and Saudi Arabic, comprehension of the dual was correlated with knowledge of two and higher number words.
Collapse
|
28
|
Rips LJ, Asmuth J, Bloomfield A. Can statistical learning bootstrap the integers? Cognition 2013; 128:320-30. [PMID: 23747652 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.04.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/03/2012] [Revised: 03/22/2013] [Accepted: 04/01/2013] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
This paper examines Piantadosi, Tenenbaum, and Goodman's (2012) model for how children learn the relation between number words ("one" through "ten") and cardinalities (sizes of sets with one through ten elements). This model shows how statistical learning can induce this relation, reorganizing its procedures as it does so in roughly the way children do. We question, however, Piantadosi et al.'s claim that the model performs "Quinian bootstrapping," in the sense of Carey (2009). Unlike bootstrapping, the concept it learns is not discontinuous with the concepts it starts with. Instead, the model learns by recombining its primitives into hypotheses and confirming them statistically. As such, it accords better with earlier claims (Fodor, 1975, 1981) that learning does not increase expressive power. We also question the relevance of the simulation for children's learning. The model starts with a preselected set of15 primitives, and the procedure it learns differs from children's method. Finally, the partial knowledge of the positive integers that the model attains is consistent with an infinite number of nonstandard meanings-for example, that the integers stop after ten or loop from ten back to one.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lance J Rips
- Psychology Department, Northwestern University, 2029 Sheridan Road, Evanston, IL 60208, USA.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
29
|
Toll SWM, Van Luit JEH. Accelerating the early numeracy development of kindergartners with limited working memory skills through remedial education. RESEARCH IN DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES 2013; 34:745-755. [PMID: 23220051 DOI: 10.1016/j.ridd.2012.09.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/20/2012] [Accepted: 09/07/2012] [Indexed: 06/01/2023]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Young children with limited working memory skills are a special interest group among all children that score below average on early numeracy tests. This study examines the effect of accelerating the early numeracy development of these children through remedial education, by comparing them with children with typically working memory skills and early numeracy abilities below average. METHOD Selected from a sample of 933 children, children with early numeracy ability below average are assigned into four groups: two intervention groups with limited working memory skills (IL-group) or typical working memory skills (IT-group), and two control groups with limited working memory skills (CL-group) or typical working memory skills (CT-group). All four groups were followed for a period of 1.5 years. Four measurements were carried out. CONCLUSION The remedial program proved to be similarly effective for the IL-group and the IT-group. The findings are discussed in the light of several limitations and implications.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sylke W M Toll
- Department of Special Education, Utrecht University, PO Box 80.140, 3508 TC Utrecht, The Netherlands.
| | | |
Collapse
|
30
|
Sarnecka BW, Negen J. A number of options: rationalist, constructivist, and Bayesian insights into the development of exact-number concepts. ADVANCES IN CHILD DEVELOPMENT AND BEHAVIOR 2012. [PMID: 23205414 DOI: 10.1016/b978-0-12-397919-3.00009-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Abstract
The question of how human beings acquire exact-number concepts has interested cognitive developmentalists since the time of Piaget. The answer will owe something to both the rationalist and constructivist traditions. On the one hand, some aspects of numerical cognition (e.g. approximate number estimation and the ability to track small sets of one to four individuals) are innate or early-developing and are shared widely among species. On the other hand, only humans create representations of exact, large numbers such as 42, as distinct from both 41 and 43. These representations seem to be constructed slowly, over a period of months or years during early childhood. The task for researchers is to distinguish the innate representational resources from those that are constructed, and to characterize the construction process. Bayesian approaches can be useful to this project in at least three ways: (1) As a way to analyze data, which may have distinct advantages over more traditional methods (e.g. making it possible to find support for a nuli hypothesis); (2) as a way of modeling children's performance on specific tasks: Peculiarities of the task are captured as a prior; the child's knowledge is captured in the way the prior is updated; and behavior is captured as a posterior distribution; and (3) as a way of modeling learning itself, by providing a formal account of how learners might choose among alternative hypotheses.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Barbara W Sarnecka
- Department of Cognitive Sciences, University of California, Irvine, CA 92697-5100, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
31
|
PETERSON DAVID. Where the Sidewalk Ends: The Limits of Social Constructionism. JOURNAL FOR THE THEORY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOUR 2012. [DOI: 10.1111/j.1468-5914.2012.00493.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
|
32
|
Abstract
Human cognition is striking in its brilliance and its adaptability. How do we get that way? How do we move from the nearly helpless state of infants to the cognitive proficiency that characterizes adults? In this paper I argue, first, that analogical ability is the key factor in our prodigious capacity, and, second, that possession of a symbol system is crucial to the full expression of analogical ability.
Collapse
|
33
|
Pepperberg IM, Carey S. Grey parrot number acquisition: the inference of cardinal value from ordinal position on the numeral list. Cognition 2012; 125:219-32. [PMID: 22878117 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.07.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 32] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/04/2011] [Revised: 06/24/2012] [Accepted: 07/04/2012] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Abstract
A Grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus) had previously been taught to use English count words ("one" through "sih" [six]) to label sets of one to six individual items (Pepperberg, 1994). He had also been taught to use the same count words to label the Arabic numerals 1 through 6. Without training, he inferred the relationship between the Arabic numerals and the sets of objects (Pepperberg, 2006b). In the present study, he was then trained to label vocally the Arabic numerals 7 and 8 ("sih-none", "eight", respectively) and to order these Arabic numerals with respect to the numeral 6. He subsequently inferred the ordinality of 7 and 8 with respect to the smaller numerals and he inferred use of the appropriate label for the cardinal values of seven and eight items. These data suggest that he constructed the cardinal meanings of "seven" ("sih-none") and "eight" from his knowledge of the cardinal meanings of one through six, together with the place of "seven" ("sih-none") and "eight" in the ordered count list.
Collapse
|
34
|
Castronovo J, Göbel SM. Impact of high mathematics education on the number sense. PLoS One 2012; 7:e33832. [PMID: 22558077 PMCID: PMC3338810 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0033832] [Citation(s) in RCA: 105] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/21/2011] [Accepted: 02/21/2012] [Indexed: 01/29/2023] Open
Abstract
In adult number processing two mechanisms are commonly used: approximate estimation of quantity and exact calculation. While the former relies on the approximate number sense (ANS) which we share with animals and preverbal infants, the latter has been proposed to rely on an exact number system (ENS) which develops later in life following the acquisition of symbolic number knowledge. The current study investigated the influence of high level math education on the ANS and the ENS. Our results showed that the precision of non-symbolic quantity representation was not significantly altered by high level math education. However, performance in a symbolic number comparison task as well as the ability to map accurately between symbolic and non-symbolic quantities was significantly better the higher mathematics achievement. Our findings suggest that high level math education in adults shows little influence on their ANS, but it seems to be associated with a better anchored ENS and better mapping abilities between ENS and ANS.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Julie Castronovo
- Department of Psychology, University of Hull, Hull, United Kingdom.
| | | |
Collapse
|
35
|
Núñez R, Cooperrider K, Wassmann J. Number concepts without number lines in an indigenous group of Papua New Guinea. PLoS One 2012; 7:e35662. [PMID: 22558193 PMCID: PMC3338449 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0035662] [Citation(s) in RCA: 36] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/03/2011] [Accepted: 03/19/2012] [Indexed: 01/29/2023] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND The generic concept of number line, which maps numbers to unidimensional space, is a fundamental concept in mathematics, but its cognitive origins are uncertain. Two defining criteria of the number line are that (i) there is a mapping of each individual number (or numerosity) under consideration onto a specific location on the line, and (ii) that the mapping defines a unidimensional space representing numbers with a metric--a distance function. It has been proposed that the number line is based on a spontaneous universal human intuition, rooted directly in brain evolution, that maps number magnitude to linear space with a metric. To date, no culture lacking this intuition has been documented. METHODOLOGY/PRINCIPAL FINDINGS By means of a number line task, we investigated the universality proposal with the Yupno of Papua New Guinea. Unschooled adults did exhibit a number-to-space mapping (criterion i) but, strikingly, despite having precise cardinal number concepts, they located numbers only on the endpoints, thus failing to use the extent of the line. The produced mapping was bi-categorical and metric-free, in violation of criterion ii. In contrast, Yupnos with scholastic experience used the extent of the segment according to known standards, but they did so not as evenly as western controls, exhibiting a bias towards the endpoints. CONCLUSIONS/SIGNIFICANCE Results suggest that cardinal number concepts can exist independently from number line representations. They also suggest that the number line mapping, although ubiquitous in the modern world, is not universally spontaneous, but rather seems to be learned through--and continually reinforced by--specific cultural practices.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rafael Núñez
- Department of Cognitive Science, University of California San Diego, La Jolla, California, United States of America.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
36
|
Categorial compositionality III: F-(co)algebras and the systematicity of recursive capacities in human cognition. PLoS One 2012; 7:e35028. [PMID: 22514704 PMCID: PMC3325926 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0035028] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/06/2012] [Accepted: 03/10/2012] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
Human cognitive capacity includes recursively definable concepts, which are prevalent in domains involving lists, numbers, and languages. Cognitive science currently lacks a satisfactory explanation for the systematic nature of such capacities (i.e., why the capacity for some recursive cognitive abilities–e.g., finding the smallest number in a list–implies the capacity for certain others–finding the largest number, given knowledge of number order). The category-theoretic constructs of initial F-algebra, catamorphism, and their duals, final coalgebra and anamorphism provide a formal, systematic treatment of recursion in computer science. Here, we use this formalism to explain the systematicity of recursive cognitive capacities without ad hoc assumptions (i.e., to the same explanatory standard used in our account of systematicity for non-recursive capacities). The presence of an initial algebra/final coalgebra explains systematicity because all recursive cognitive capacities, in the domain of interest, factor through (are composed of) the same component process. Moreover, this factorization is unique, hence no further (ad hoc) assumptions are required to establish the intrinsic connection between members of a group of systematically-related capacities. This formulation also provides a new perspective on the relationship between recursive cognitive capacities. In particular, the link between number and language does not depend on recursion, as such, but on the underlying functor on which the group of recursive capacities is based. Thus, many species (and infants) can employ recursive processes without having a full-blown capacity for number and language.
Collapse
|
37
|
Coolidge FL, Overmann KA. Numerosity, Abstraction, and the Emergence of Symbolic Thinking. CURRENT ANTHROPOLOGY 2012. [DOI: 10.1086/664818] [Citation(s) in RCA: 49] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/03/2022]
|
38
|
Piantadosi ST, Tenenbaum JB, Goodman ND. Bootstrapping in a language of thought: a formal model of numerical concept learning. Cognition 2012; 123:199-217. [PMID: 22284806 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2011.11.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 69] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/31/2010] [Revised: 11/02/2011] [Accepted: 11/11/2011] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Abstract
In acquiring number words, children exhibit a qualitative leap in which they transition from understanding a few number words, to possessing a rich system of interrelated numerical concepts. We present a computational framework for understanding this inductive leap as the consequence of statistical inference over a sufficiently powerful representational system. We provide an implemented model that is powerful enough to learn number word meanings and other related conceptual systems from naturalistic data. The model shows that bootstrapping can be made computationally and philosophically well-founded as a theory of number learning. Our approach demonstrates how learners may combine core cognitive operations to build sophisticated representations during the course of development, and how this process explains observed developmental patterns in number word learning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Steven T Piantadosi
- Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, University of Rochester, United States.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
39
|
Number representation is influenced by numerical processing level: an ERP study. Exp Brain Res 2012; 218:27-39. [DOI: 10.1007/s00221-012-2998-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/04/2011] [Accepted: 01/01/2012] [Indexed: 10/14/2022]
|
40
|
Davidson K, Eng K, Barner D. Does learning to count involve a semantic induction? Cognition 2012; 123:162-73. [PMID: 22245033 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2011.12.013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 76] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/02/2010] [Revised: 12/21/2011] [Accepted: 12/23/2011] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
We tested the hypothesis that, when children learn to correctly count sets, they make a semantic induction about the meanings of their number words. We tested the logical understanding of number words in 84 children that were classified as "cardinal-principle knowers" by the criteria set forth by Wynn (1992). Results show that these children often do not know (1) which of two numbers in their count list denotes a greater quantity, and (2) that the difference between successive numbers in their count list is 1. Among counters, these abilities are predicted by the highest number to which they can count and their ability to estimate set sizes. Also, children's knowledge of the principles appears to be initially item-specific rather than general to all number words, and is most robust for very small numbers (e.g., 5) compared to larger numbers (e.g., 25), even among children who can count much higher (e.g., above 30). In light of these findings, we conclude that there is little evidence to support the hypothesis that becoming a cardinal-principle knower involves a semantic induction over all items in a child's count list.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kathryn Davidson
- Department of Linguistics, University of California, San Diego, CA 92093, United States.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
41
|
Jordan NC, Glutting J, Dyson N, Hassinger-Das B, Irwin C. Building Kindergartners' Number Sense: A Randomized Controlled Study. JOURNAL OF EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY 2012; 104:647-660. [PMID: 25866417 DOI: 10.1037/a0029018] [Citation(s) in RCA: 28] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Math achievement in elementary school is mediated by performance and growth in number sense during kindergarten. The aim of the present study was to test the effectiveness of a targeted small group number sense intervention for high-risk kindergartners from low-income communities. Children were randomly assigned to one of three groups (n = 44 in each group): a number sense intervention group, a language intervention group, or a business as usual control group. Accounting for initial skill level in mathematical knowledge, children who received the number sense intervention performed better than controls at immediate post test, with meaningful effects on measures of number competencies and general math achievement. Many of the effects held eight weeks after the intervention was completed, suggesting that children internalized what they had learned. There were no differences between the language and control groups on any math-related measures.
Collapse
|
42
|
Moeller K, Martignon L, Wessolowski S, Engel J, Nuerk HC. Effects of finger counting on numerical development - the opposing views of neurocognition and mathematics education. Front Psychol 2011; 2:328. [PMID: 22144969 PMCID: PMC3225925 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00328] [Citation(s) in RCA: 46] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/28/2011] [Accepted: 10/24/2011] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Children typically learn basic numerical and arithmetic principles using finger-based representations. However, whether or not reliance on finger-based representations is beneficial or detrimental is the subject of an ongoing debate between researchers in neurocognition and mathematics education. From the neurocognitive perspective, finger counting provides multisensory input, which conveys both cardinal and ordinal aspects of numbers. Recent data indicate that children with good finger-based numerical representations show better arithmetic skills and that training finger gnosis, or "finger sense," enhances mathematical skills. Therefore neurocognitive researchers conclude that elaborate finger-based numerical representations are beneficial for later numerical development. However, research in mathematics education recommends fostering mentally based numerical representations so as to induce children to abandon finger counting. More precisely, mathematics education recommends first using finger counting, then concrete structured representations and, finally, mental representations of numbers to perform numerical operations. Taken together, these results reveal an important debate between neurocognitive and mathematics education research concerning the benefits and detriments of finger-based strategies for numerical development. In the present review, the rationale of both lines of evidence will be discussed.
Collapse
|
43
|
Squeezing, striking, and vocalizing: Is number representation fundamentally spatial? Cognition 2011; 120:225-35. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2011.05.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 21] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/13/2010] [Revised: 04/12/2011] [Accepted: 05/05/2011] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
|
44
|
Abstract
AbstractThe Origin of Concepts sets out an impressive defense of the view that children construct entirely new systems of concepts. We offer here two questions about this theory. First, why doesn't the bootstrapping process provide a pattern for translating between the old and new systems, contradicting their claimed incommensurability? Second, can the bootstrapping process properly distinguish meaning change from belief change?
Collapse
|
45
|
Abstract
Many authors in the field of numerical cognition have adopted a rather nativist view that all humans share the intuition that numbers map onto space and, more specifically, that an oriented left-to-right mental number line (MNL) is localized bilaterally in the intraparietal sulcus of the human brain. We review results from archaeological and historical (diachronic) studies as well as cross-cultural (synchronic) ones and contends that these claims are not well founded. The data actually suggest that the MNL is not innate. We argue that the MNL—and number-to-space mappings in general—emerges outside of natural selection proper requiring top-down dynamics that are culturally and historically mediated through high-order cognitive mechanisms such as fictive motion, conceptual mappings, and external representational media. These mechanisms, which are not intrinsically numerical and usually are acquired through education, are not genetically determined and are biologically realized through the systematic consolidation of specific brain phenotypes that support number-to-space mappings.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rafael E. Núñez
- Department of Cognitive Science, University of California, San Diego, San Diego, California, USA,
| |
Collapse
|
46
|
Affiliation(s)
- Kerry E Jordan
- Department of Psychology, Utah State University, Logan, UT 84322-2810, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
47
|
Johnson MH. Interactive specialization: a domain-general framework for human functional brain development? Dev Cogn Neurosci 2011; 1:7-21. [PMID: 22436416 PMCID: PMC6987575 DOI: 10.1016/j.dcn.2010.07.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 405] [Impact Index Per Article: 31.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/11/2010] [Revised: 07/15/2010] [Accepted: 07/16/2010] [Indexed: 12/18/2022] Open
Abstract
A domain-general framework for interpreting data on human functional brain development is presented. Assumptions underlying the general theory and predictions derived from it are discussed. Developmental functional neuroimaging data from the domains of face processing, social cognition, word learning and reading, executive control, and brain resting states are used to assess these predictions. Finally, potential criticisms of the framework are addressed and challenges for the future presented.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mark H Johnson
- Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, Birkbeck, University of London, Malet Street, London WC1E 7HX, UK.
| |
Collapse
|
48
|
Piazza M. Neurocognitive start-up tools for symbolic number representations. Trends Cogn Sci 2010; 14:542-51. [PMID: 21055996 DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.09.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 265] [Impact Index Per Article: 18.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/19/2010] [Revised: 09/26/2010] [Accepted: 09/28/2010] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
|
49
|
Foundational numerical capacities and the origins of dyscalculia. Trends Cogn Sci 2010; 14:534-41. [PMID: 20971676 DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.09.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 175] [Impact Index Per Article: 12.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/30/2010] [Revised: 09/28/2010] [Accepted: 09/28/2010] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
|
50
|
Abstract
AbstractWith his massive redeployment hypothesis (MRH), Anderson claims that novel cognitive functions are likely to rely on pre-existing circuits already possessing suitable resources. Here, we put forward recent findings from studies in numerical cognition in order to show that the role of sensorimotor experience in the ontogenetical development of a new function has been largely underestimated in Anderson's proposal.
Collapse
|