1
|
Béghin G, Markovits H. The effects of strength and activation level of belief on belief-biased reasoning. Psychon Bull Rev 2024; 31:1782-1788. [PMID: 38285122 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-024-02461-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 01/11/2024] [Indexed: 01/30/2024]
Abstract
The belief bias effect designates the tendency to judge the validity of a conclusion based on its believability. Most studies have compared highly believable with unbelievable conclusions when examining belief-biased reasoning. In two studies, we examine a hypothesis raised by Banks (2013, Cognitive Science, 37[3], 544-577), who postulated that level of activation of belief should affect the extent to which reasoning is biased. Here, a first study found that moderately believable conclusions produced a significant biasing effect, but one that was smaller than with highly believable conclusions. The second study found that activating the category referenced by a conclusion significantly increased the biasing effect of the same conclusion. These results suggest that strength of belief is a necessary parameter of any model of belief bias. They also show that context can affect the extent to which belief affects reasoning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gaetan Béghin
- Département de psychologie, Université du Québec à Montréal, C.P. 8888, Succ. "A", Montréal, Québec, H3C 3P8, Canada
| | - Henry Markovits
- Département de psychologie, Université du Québec à Montréal, C.P. 8888, Succ. "A", Montréal, Québec, H3C 3P8, Canada.
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Béghin G, Markovits H. Interpretation of ambiguous trials along with reasoning strategy is related to causal judgements in zero-contingency learning. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 2023; 76:2704-2717. [PMID: 36718805 PMCID: PMC10663643 DOI: 10.1177/17470218231155897] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/04/2022] [Revised: 01/06/2023] [Accepted: 01/09/2023] [Indexed: 02/01/2023]
Abstract
The dual strategy model suggests that people can use either a Statistical or a Counterexample reasoning strategy, which reflects two qualitatively different ways of processing information. This model has been shown to capture individual differences in a wide array of tasks, such as contingency learning. Here, we examined whether this extends to individual differences in the interpretation of contingency information where effects are ambiguous. Previous studies, using perceptually complex stimuli, have shown that the way in which participants interpret ambiguous effects predicts causal judgements. In two studies, we attempted to replicate this effect using a small number of clearly identifiable cues. Results show that the interpretation of ambiguous effects as effect present is related to final contingency judgements. In addition, results showed that Statistical reasoners had a stronger tendency to interpret ambiguous effects as effect present than Counterexample reasoners, which mediates the difference in contingency judgements.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gaëtan Béghin
- Université du Québec à Montréal, Montreal, QC, Canada
| | | |
Collapse
|
3
|
Stupple EJN, Ball LJ. Explaining normative-deliberative gaps is essential to dual-process theorizing. Behav Brain Sci 2023; 46:e143. [PMID: 37462192 DOI: 10.1017/s0140525x22003107] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 07/20/2023]
Abstract
We discuss significant challenges to assumptions of exclusivity and highlight methodological and conceptual pitfalls in inferring deliberative processes from reasoning responses. Causes of normative-deliberative gaps are considered (e.g., disputed or misunderstood normative standards, strategy preferences, task interpretations, cognitive ability, mindware and thinking dispositions) and a soft normativist approach is recommended for developing the dual-process 2.0 architecture.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Edward J N Stupple
- School of Psychology, College of Health, Psychology and Social Care, University of Derby, Derby, UK ; https://www.derby.ac.uk/staff/ed-stupple/
| | - Linden J Ball
- School of Psychology & Computer Science, University of Central Lancashire, Preston, UK ; https://www.uclan.ac.uk/academics/professor-linden-ball
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Ghasemi O, Handley SJ, Howarth S. Illusory intuitive inferences: Matching heuristics explain logical intuitions. Cognition 2023; 235:105417. [PMID: 36870202 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2023.105417] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/26/2022] [Revised: 02/15/2023] [Accepted: 02/16/2023] [Indexed: 03/06/2023]
Abstract
The capacity to evaluate logical arguments intuitively is a fundamental assumption of recent dual-process theories. One observation supporting this effect is the standard conflict effect on incongruent arguments under belief instruction. Conflict arguments are evaluated less accurately than non-conflict arguments, arguably because logic is intuitive and automatic enough to interfere with belief judgments. However, recent studies have challenged this interpretation by finding the same conflict effects when a matching heuristic cues the same response as logic, even on arguments with no logically valid structures. In this study, we test the matching heuristic hypothesis across 4 experiments (total N = 409) by manipulating the arguments propositions so that matching cues a response that is either (1) aligned or (2) misaligned with logic, or (3) cues no response at all. Consistent with the predictions of the matching heuristic, standard, reversed, and no conflict effects were found in those conditions, respectively. These results indicate that intuitively correct inferences which are assumed as evidence of logical intuitions are actually driven by a matching heuristic that cues responses aligned with logic. Alleged intuitive logic effects are reversed when the matching heuristic cues an opposing logical response or disappears when there are no matching cues. Therefore, it appears as though the operation of a matching heuristic, rather than an intuitive access to logic, drives logical intuitions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Omid Ghasemi
- School of Psychological Sciences, Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia; School of Psychology, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia.
| | - Simon J Handley
- School of Psychological Sciences, Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia
| | - Stephanie Howarth
- School of Psychological Sciences, Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Fonseca C, Pettitt J, Woollard A, Rutherford A, Bickmore W, Ferguson-Smith A, Hurst LD. People with more extreme attitudes towards science have self-confidence in their understanding of science, even if this is not justified. PLoS Biol 2023; 21:e3001915. [PMID: 36693040 PMCID: PMC10045565 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pbio.3001915] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/11/2022] [Accepted: 11/14/2022] [Indexed: 01/25/2023] Open
Abstract
People differ greatly in their attitudes towards well-evidenced science. What characterises this variation? Here, we consider this issue in the context of genetics and allied sciences. While most prior research has focused on the relationship between attitude to science and what people know about it, recent evidence suggests that individuals with strongly negative attitudes towards specific genetic technologies (genetic modification (GM) technology and vaccines) commonly do not objectively understand the science, but, importantly, believe that they do. Here, using data from a probability survey of United Kingdom adults, we extend this prior work in 2 regards. First, we ask whether people with more extreme attitudes, be they positive or negative, are more likely to believe that they understand the science. Second, as negativity to genetics is commonly framed around issues particular to specific technologies, we ask whether attitudinal trends are contingent on specification of technology. We find (1) that individuals with strongly positive or negative attitudes towards genetics more strongly believe that they well understand the science; but (2) only for those most positive to the science is this self-confidence warranted; and (3) these effects are not contingent on specification of any particular technologies. These results suggest a potentially general model to explain why people differ in their degree of acceptance or rejection of science, this being that the more someone believes they understand the science, the more confident they will be in their acceptance or rejection of it. While there are more technology nonspecific opponents who also oppose GM technology than expected by chance, most GM opponents fit a different demographic. For the most part, opposition to GM appears not to reflect a smokescreen concealing a broader underlying negativity.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Jonathan Pettitt
- School of Medicine, Medical Sciences and Nutrition, University of Aberdeen, Institute of Medical Sciences, Aberdeen, United Kingdom
| | - Alison Woollard
- Biochemistry Department, University of Oxford, Oxford, United Kingdom
| | - Adam Rutherford
- Department of Genetics, Evolution and Environment, University College London, London, United Kingdom
| | - Wendy Bickmore
- MRC Human Genetics Unit, Institute of Genetics and Cancer, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, United Kingdom
| | | | - Laurence D. Hurst
- The Milner Centre for Evolution, Department and Biology and Biochemistry, University of Bath, Bath, United Kingdom
- Wissenshaftskolleg zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
- * E-mail:
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Quilty-Dunn J, Porot N, Mandelbaum E. The best game in town: The reemergence of the language-of-thought hypothesis across the cognitive sciences. Behav Brain Sci 2022; 46:e261. [PMID: 36471543 DOI: 10.1017/s0140525x22002849] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 13.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/12/2022]
Abstract
Mental representations remain the central posits of psychology after many decades of scrutiny. However, there is no consensus about the representational format(s) of biological cognition. This paper provides a survey of evidence from computational cognitive psychology, perceptual psychology, developmental psychology, comparative psychology, and social psychology, and concludes that one type of format that routinely crops up is the language-of-thought (LoT). We outline six core properties of LoTs: (i) discrete constituents; (ii) role-filler independence; (iii) predicate-argument structure; (iv) logical operators; (v) inferential promiscuity; and (vi) abstract content. These properties cluster together throughout cognitive science. Bayesian computational modeling, compositional features of object perception, complex infant and animal reasoning, and automatic, intuitive cognition in adults all implicate LoT-like structures. Instead of regarding LoT as a relic of the previous century, researchers in cognitive science and philosophy-of-mind must take seriously the explanatory breadth of LoT-based architectures. We grant that the mind may harbor many formats and architectures, including iconic and associative structures as well as deep-neural-network-like architectures. However, as computational/representational approaches to the mind continue to advance, classical compositional symbolic structures - that is, LoTs - only prove more flexible and well-supported over time.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jake Quilty-Dunn
- Department of Philosophy and Philosophy-Neuroscience-Psychology Program, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO, USA. , sites.google.com/site/jakequiltydunn/
| | - Nicolas Porot
- Africa Institute for Research in Economics and Social Sciences, Mohammed VI Polytechnic University, Rabat, Morocco. , nicolasporot.com
| | - Eric Mandelbaum
- Departments of Philosophy and Psychology, The Graduate Center & Baruch College, CUNY, New York, NY, USA. , ericmandelbaum.com
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Reasoning strategies and prior knowledge effects in contingency learning. Mem Cognit 2022; 50:1269-1283. [DOI: 10.3758/s13421-022-01319-w] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 04/21/2022] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
|
8
|
Basieva I, Pandey V, Khrennikova P. More Causes Less Effect: Destructive Interference in Decision Making. ENTROPY 2022; 24:e24050725. [PMID: 35626608 PMCID: PMC9141002 DOI: 10.3390/e24050725] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/29/2022] [Revised: 05/05/2022] [Accepted: 05/17/2022] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
We present a new experiment demonstrating destructive interference in customers’ estimates of conditional probabilities of product failure. We take the perspective of a manufacturer of consumer products and consider two situations of cause and effect. Whereas, individually, the effect of the causes is similar, it is observed that when combined, the two causes produce the opposite effect. Such negative interference of two or more product features may be exploited for better modeling of the cognitive processes taking place in customers’ minds. Doing so can enhance the likelihood that a manufacturer will be able to design a better product, or a feature within it. Quantum probability has been used to explain some commonly observed “non-classical” effects, such as the disjunction effect, question order effect, violation of the sure-thing principle, and the Machina and Ellsberg paradoxes. In this work, we present results from a survey on the impact of multiple observed symptoms on the drivability of a vehicle. The symptoms are assumed to be conditionally independent. We demonstrate that the response statistics cannot be directly explained using classical probability, but quantum formulation easily models it, as it allows for both positive and negative “interference” between events. Since quantum formalism also accounts for classical probability’s predictions, it serves as a richer paradigm for modeling decision making behavior in engineering design and behavioral economics.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Irina Basieva
- International Center for Mathematical Modeling in Physics and Cognitive Science, Linnaeus University, S-35195 Växjö, Sweden
- Correspondence:
| | - Vijitashwa Pandey
- Industrial and Systems Engineering Department, Oakland University, Rochester, MI 48309, USA;
| | | |
Collapse
|
9
|
Zhang P, Du J, Wang L, Fei M, Yang T, Pardalos PM. A human learning optimization algorithm with reasoning learning. Appl Soft Comput 2022. [DOI: 10.1016/j.asoc.2022.108816] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
|
10
|
Shao J, Tikiri Banda D, Baratgin J. A Study on the Sufficient Conditional and the Necessary Conditional With Chinese and French Participants. Front Psychol 2022; 13:787588. [PMID: 35282197 PMCID: PMC8907880 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.787588] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/30/2021] [Accepted: 01/10/2022] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
According to the weak version of linguistic relativity, also called the Sapir-Whorf hypothesis, the features of an individual’s native language influence his worldview and perception. We decided to test this hypothesis on the sufficient conditional and the necessary conditional, expressed differently in Chinese and French. In Chinese, connectors for both conditionals exist and are used in everyday life, while there is only a connector for the sufficient conditional in French. A first hypothesis follows from linguistic relativity: for the necessary conditional, better logic performance is expected in Chinese participants rather than French participants. As a second hypothesis, for all participants, we expect performance on the sufficient conditional to be better than on the necessary conditional. Indeed, despite the isomorphism of the two conditionals, they differ in how information is processed for reasoning. We decided to study reasoning under uncertainty as it reflects reality more accurately. To do so, we analyzed the coherence of participants using de Finetti’s theory for deduction under uncertainty. The results of our study show no significant difference in performance between Chinese and French participants, neither on the sufficient conditional nor on the necessary conditional. Thus, our first hypothesis derived from the weak version of linguistic relativity is not confirmed. In contrast, our results confirm the second hypothesis in two out of three inference schemas.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jing Shao
- Laboratory Cognition Humaine et Artificielle, Université Paris 8, Paris, France.,Université de Haute-Alsace, Mulhouse, France
| | - Dilane Tikiri Banda
- Probability, Assessment, Reasoning and Inferences Studies (P-A-R-I-S) Association, Paris, France
| | - Jean Baratgin
- Laboratory Cognition Humaine et Artificielle, Université Paris 8, Paris, France.,Probability, Assessment, Reasoning and Inferences Studies (P-A-R-I-S) Association, Paris, France
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Pothos EM, Pleskac TJ. Rethinking Rationality. Top Cogn Sci 2022; 14:451-466. [PMID: 35261177 DOI: 10.1111/tops.12585] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/17/2021] [Revised: 10/13/2021] [Accepted: 10/13/2021] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
We seek to understand rational decision making and if it exists whether finite (bounded) agents may be able to achieve its principles. This aim has been a singular objective throughout much of human science and philosophy, with early discussions identified since antiquity. More recently, there has been a thriving debate based on differing perspectives on rationality, including adaptive heuristics, Bayesian theory, quantum theory, resource rationality, and probabilistic language of thought. Are these perspectives on rationality mutually exclusive? Are they all needed? Do they undermine an aim to have rational standards in decision situations like politics, medicine, legal proceedings, and others, where there is an expectation and need for decision making as close to "optimal" as possible? This special issue brings together representative contributions from the currently predominant views on rationality, with a view to evaluate progress on these and related questions.
Collapse
|
12
|
Salvador A, Arnal LH, Vinckier F, Domenech P, Gaillard R, Wyart V. Premature commitment to uncertain decisions during human NMDA receptor hypofunction. Nat Commun 2022; 13:338. [PMID: 35039498 PMCID: PMC8763907 DOI: 10.1038/s41467-021-27876-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/10/2020] [Accepted: 12/21/2021] [Indexed: 11/15/2022] Open
Abstract
Making accurate decisions based on unreliable sensory evidence requires cognitive inference. Dysfunction of n-methyl-d-aspartate (NMDA) receptors impairs the integration of noisy input in theoretical models of neural circuits, but whether and how this synaptic alteration impairs human inference and confidence during uncertain decisions remains unknown. Here we use placebo-controlled infusions of ketamine to characterize the causal effect of human NMDA receptor hypofunction on cognitive inference and its neural correlates. At the behavioral level, ketamine triggers inference errors and elevated decision uncertainty. At the neural level, ketamine is associated with imbalanced coding of evidence and premature response preparation in electroencephalographic (EEG) activity. Through computational modeling of inference and confidence, we propose that this specific pattern of behavioral and neural impairments reflects an early commitment to inaccurate decisions, which aims at resolving the abnormal uncertainty generated by NMDA receptor hypofunction.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alexandre Salvador
- Laboratoire de Neurosciences Cognitives et Computationnelles, Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale, Paris, France
- Département d'Études Cognitives, École Normale Supérieure, Université PSL, Paris, France
- Université de Paris, Paris, France
- Département de Psychiatrie, Service Hospitalo-Universitaire, GHU Paris Psychiatrie et Neurosciences, Paris, France
| | - Luc H Arnal
- Institut de l'Audition, Inserm unit 1120, Institut Pasteur, Paris, France
| | - Fabien Vinckier
- Université de Paris, Paris, France
- Département de Psychiatrie, Service Hospitalo-Universitaire, GHU Paris Psychiatrie et Neurosciences, Paris, France
- Équipe Motivation, Cerveau et Comportement, Institut du Cerveau, Sorbonne Université, Paris, France
| | - Philippe Domenech
- Équipe Neurophysiologie des Comportements Répétitifs, Institut du Cerveau, Sorbonne Université, Paris, France
- Département Médico-Universitaire de Psychiatrie et d'Addictologie, CHU AP-HP Henri Mondor, Université Paris-Est Créteil, Créteil, France
| | - Raphaël Gaillard
- Université de Paris, Paris, France
- Département de Psychiatrie, Service Hospitalo-Universitaire, GHU Paris Psychiatrie et Neurosciences, Paris, France
- Unité de Neuropathologie Expérimentale, Département de Santé Globale, Institut Pasteur, Paris, France
| | - Valentin Wyart
- Laboratoire de Neurosciences Cognitives et Computationnelles, Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale, Paris, France.
- Département d'Études Cognitives, École Normale Supérieure, Université PSL, Paris, France.
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Inferences from the negation of counterfactual and semifactual conditionals. Mem Cognit 2021; 50:1090-1102. [PMID: 34846638 PMCID: PMC8631256 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-021-01252-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 10/10/2021] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
Abstract
Our goal was to study how people understand the negation of counterfactuals (such as “Antonio denied/said that it is false that if Messi had played, then Barcelona would have won”) and semifactuals (such as “Antonio denied that even if Messi had played, Barcelona would have won”). Previous studies have shown that participants negated basic conditionals using small-scope interpretations by endorsing a new conditional with the negated consequent, but also by making large-scope interpretations, endorsing a conjunction with the negated consequent. Three experiments showed that when participants were asked whether the negation of a counterfactual (Experiments 1 and 2) or semifactual (Experiment 3) conditional was followed by a new conditional, they made a small-scope interpretation, endorsing the same conditional with the negated consequent (e.g., “if/even if Messi had played, Barcelona would not have won”). However, they also accepted the conditional with the negated antecedent for semifactuals (e.g., “even if Messi had not played, Barcelona would have won”). When participants were asked whether the negation of a counterfactual or semifactual conditional is followed by a conjunction, they endorsed the conjunction with both the negated antecedent and the consequent (e.g., “Messi did not play and Barcelona did not win”), but again they accepted the conjunction with the negated antecedent only for semifactuals (e.g., “Messi did not play and Barcelona did win”). These results have implications for the main theories of reasoning.
Collapse
|
14
|
Abstract
The decisions we make are shaped by a lifetime of learning. Past experience guides the way that we encode information in neural systems for perception and valuation, and determines the information we retrieve when making decisions. Distinct literatures have discussed how lifelong learning and local context shape decisions made about sensory signals, propositional information, or economic prospects. Here, we build bridges between these literatures, arguing for common principles of adaptive rationality in perception, cognition, and economic choice. We discuss how a single common framework, based on normative principles of efficient coding and Bayesian inference, can help us understand a myriad of human decision biases, including sensory illusions, adaptive aftereffects, choice history biases, central tendency effects, anchoring effects, contrast effects, framing effects, congruency effects, reference-dependent valuation, nonlinear utility functions, and discretization heuristics. We describe a simple computational framework for explaining these phenomena. Expected final online publication date for the Annual Review of Psychology, Volume 73 is January 2022. Please see http://www.annualreviews.org/page/journal/pubdates for revised estimates.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Christopher Summerfield
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford OX2 6GG, United Kingdom;
| | - Paula Parpart
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford OX2 6GG, United Kingdom;
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Zhang M, Wang L, Zou F, Wang Y, Wu X. The Brain Structure and Intrinsic Characters of Falsification Thinking in Conditional Proposition Testing. Front Hum Neurosci 2021; 15:684470. [PMID: 34497498 PMCID: PMC8419331 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2021.684470] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/24/2021] [Accepted: 07/21/2021] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Wason's selection task (WST) as a representative of the field of conditional proposition testing has been explored by multiple disciplines for more than 50 years, but the neural basis of its key falsification thinking remains unclear. Considering that the accuracy of individuals in WST has stability over time, we believe that falsification thinking has a specific brain structural basis and intrinsic neural characteristics. To test this hypothesis, we studied individuals who were able to complete the WST using T1-weighted MRI (using voxel-based morphology (VBM) analysis) and resting electroencephalogram (EEG) (using microstate analysis, which can reflect stable cognitive characteristics of individuals) techniques. First, VBM analysis found that, compared with the verification group, the gray matter volume (GMV) of the left inferior temporal gyrus and the right superior temporal region of the falsification group was larger, whereas the GMV in the cerebellum of the verification group was significantly larger than that of the falsification group. Subsequently, the results of the microstate analysis of the resting EEG data showed that the contribution of class A of the falsification group, which is closely related to the language network, is significantly higher than that of the verification group. Our structural MRI and resting EEG results consistently show that the structure and intrinsic activity pattern of the temporal lobe in individuals with falsification thinking are specific. Furthermore, the findings may provide potential insights into the role of the temporal lobe (which is also a brain region of language processing) in thought.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Meng Zhang
- Department of Psychology, Xinxiang Medical University, Xinxiang, China.,Department of Psychiatry, Henan Mental Hospital, The Second Affiliated Hospital of Xinxiang Medical University, Xinxiang, China
| | - Li Wang
- Department of Psychology, Xinxiang Medical University, Xinxiang, China
| | - Feng Zou
- Department of Psychology, Xinxiang Medical University, Xinxiang, China
| | - Yufeng Wang
- Department of Psychology, Xinxiang Medical University, Xinxiang, China
| | - Xin Wu
- Department of Psychology, Xinxiang Medical University, Xinxiang, China
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Thompson VA, Markovits H. Reasoning strategy vs cognitive capacity as predictors of individual differences in reasoning performance. Cognition 2021; 217:104866. [PMID: 34450394 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104866] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/13/2020] [Revised: 07/12/2021] [Accepted: 07/14/2021] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
The dual strategy model posits that reasoners rely on two information processing strategies when making inferences: The statistical strategy generates a rapid probabilistic estimate based on associative access to a wide array of information, and the counterexample strategy uses a more focused representation allowing for a search for potential counterexamples. In this paper, we focused on individual differences in strategy use as a predictor of performance on four reasoning tasks: Belief bias, base rate neglect, conjunction fallacy, and denominator neglect. Predictions from the strategy use model were contrasted with predictions from Dual Process Theories, which suggest that individual differences in performance reflect variations in cognitive ability. In each of four studies, a large number (N ≈ 200) completed one of the above reasoning tasks, a strategy use diagnostic questionnaire, and measures of IQ, cognitive reflection, and numeracy. In three of four studies, individual differences in strategy use predicted differences in reasoning performance when the effects of the other variables were eliminated. Bayesian analysis indicated that none of the individual differences measures predicted a significant portion of variance on the conjunction fallacy task, and that strategy use was a strong predictor on the remaining three tasks. This research suggests that the type of strategy that is adopted paves a road to successful reasoning that is independent of cognitive capacity.
Collapse
|
17
|
Dotlačil J, de Haan P. Parsing Model and a Rational Theory of Memory. Front Psychol 2021; 12:657705. [PMID: 34248751 PMCID: PMC8261045 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.657705] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/23/2021] [Accepted: 05/03/2021] [Indexed: 11/20/2022] Open
Abstract
This paper explores how the rational theory of memory summarized in Anderson (1991) can inform the computational psycholinguistic models of human parsing. It is shown that transition-based parsing is particularly suitable to be combined with Anderson's theory of memory systems. The combination of the rational theory of memory with the transition-based parsers results in a model of sentence processing that is data-driven and can be embedded in the cognitive architecture Adaptive Control of Thought-Rational (ACT-R). The predictions of the parser are tested against qualitative data (garden-path sentences) and a self-paced reading corpus (the Natural Stories corpus).
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jakub Dotlačil
- Utrecht Institute of Linguistics, Utrecht University, Utrecht, Netherlands
| | - Puck de Haan
- Artificial Intelligence, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Wojciechowski BW, Izydorczyk B, Blasiak P, Yearsley JM, White LC, Pothos EM. Constructive Biases in Clinical Judgment. Top Cogn Sci 2021; 14:508-527. [PMID: 34080786 DOI: 10.1111/tops.12547] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/05/2021] [Revised: 05/15/2021] [Accepted: 05/19/2021] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
With a pair of oppositely valenced stimuli, rating the first one sometimes leads to a more extreme evaluation for the second (e.g., if the second is negatively valenced, rating the first stimulus would lead to a more negative rating for the second). We considered an evaluation bias in the case of clinical diagnosis relating to eating disorders. A population sample which included experienced clinical psychologists and psychiatrists showed partial evidence of an evaluation bias, when judging descriptions of individuals designed to be consistent with eating disorders or not. Quantum probability theory, the probability rules from quantum mechanics without any of the physics, is particularly well-suited to modeling the evaluation bias (and constructive influences generally), because a measurement (or judgment) can change the state of the system. We applied a previous quantum model to the present result, an extension of the model embodying noisy processes, and belief adjustment model. We discuss how model fits inform an examination of rationality in the observed behavior.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Bartosz W Wojciechowski
- Faculty of Management and Social Communication, Institute of Applied Psychology, Jagiellonian University
| | - Bernadetta Izydorczyk
- Faculty of Management and Social Communication, Institute of Applied Psychology, Jagiellonian University
| | - Pawel Blasiak
- Institute of Nuclear Physics, Polish Academy of Sciences
| | - James M Yearsley
- School of Arts and Social Sciences, Department of Psychology, City, University of London
| | - Lee C White
- School of Arts and Social Sciences, Department of Psychology, City, University of London
| | - Emmanuel M Pothos
- School of Arts and Social Sciences, Department of Psychology, City, University of London
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Béghin G, Gagnon-St-Pierre É, Markovits H. A dual strategy account of individual differences in information processing in contingency judgments. JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2021. [DOI: 10.1080/20445911.2021.1900200] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
|
20
|
Interacting with volatile environments stabilizes hidden-state inference and its brain signatures. Nat Commun 2021; 12:2228. [PMID: 33850124 PMCID: PMC8044147 DOI: 10.1038/s41467-021-22396-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/06/2019] [Accepted: 03/05/2021] [Indexed: 11/26/2022] Open
Abstract
Making accurate decisions in uncertain environments requires identifying the generative cause of sensory cues, but also the expected outcomes of possible actions. Although both cognitive processes can be formalized as Bayesian inference, they are commonly studied using different experimental frameworks, making their formal comparison difficult. Here, by framing a reversal learning task either as cue-based or outcome-based inference, we found that humans perceive the same volatile environment as more stable when inferring its hidden state by interaction with uncertain outcomes than by observation of equally uncertain cues. Multivariate patterns of magnetoencephalographic (MEG) activity reflected this behavioral difference in the neural interaction between inferred beliefs and incoming evidence, an effect originating from associative regions in the temporal lobe. Together, these findings indicate that the degree of control over the sampling of volatile environments shapes human learning and decision-making under uncertainty. Here, the authors show that humans perceive uncertain environments as more stable when actively interacting with them than when observing them. Magnetoencephalographic signals in the temporal lobe were associated with the increased stability of beliefs during active sampling.
Collapse
|
21
|
Jones DL, Nelson JD, Opitz B. Increased Anxiety is Associated with Better Learning from Negative Feedback. PSYCHOLOGY LEARNING AND TEACHING-PLAT 2021. [DOI: 10.1177/1475725720965761] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
Anxiety is one of the most prevalent mental health problems; it is known to impede cognitive functioning. It is believed to alter preferences for feedback-based learning in anxious and non-anxious learners. Thus, the present study measured feedback processing in adults ( N = 30) with and without anxiety symptoms using a probabilistic learning task. Event-related potential (ERP) measures were used to assess how the bias for either positive or negative feedback learning is reflected by the feedback-related negativity component (FRN), an ERP extracted from the electroencephalogram. Anxious individuals, identified by means of the Penn State Worry Questionnaire, showed a diminished FRN and increased accuracy after negative compared to positive feedback. Non-anxious individuals exhibited the reversed pattern with better learning from positive feedback, highlighting their preference for positive feedback. Our ERP results imply that impairments with feedback-based learning in anxious individuals are due to alterations in the mesolimbic dopaminergic system. Our finding that anxious individuals seem to favor negative as opposed to positive feedback has important implications for teacher–student feedback communication.
Collapse
|
22
|
Nickerson RS, Butler SF, Barch DH. Looking behind: Turning cards in the selection task. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 2021; 74:1451-1464. [PMID: 33629644 DOI: 10.1177/17470218211001293] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
Wason's selection task requires that one imagine which of four cards, each of which has a letter on one side and a number on the other, one would have to turn over to determine whether a statement about the cards is true or false. For example, one might see four cards showing T, H, 6, and 4 and be asked to say which card or cards one would have to turn over to determine whether a statement in the form of If a card has T on one side, it has 4 on the other is true. In the great majority of experiments with this task no cards are actually turned. This limits the conclusions that can be drawn from experimental results. In two experiments participants actually turned (had a computer turn) virtual cards so as to show what they contained on their originally hidden sides. Participants were given a monetary incentive to do well on the task, and they performed it, with trial-by-trial feedback, many times. Performance was much better than is typically obtained with the more common way of performing the task. Results also demonstrate the importance of the precise wording of the statement to be evaluated and how a misinterpretation could help account for a tendency for people to turn only a single card even when the turning of two is required. Results prompt several questions of a theoretical nature and are discussed as they relate to recent theoretical treatments of the selection task.
Collapse
|
23
|
Pothos EM, Lewandowsky S, Basieva I, Barque-Duran A, Tapper K, Khrennikov A. Information overload for (bounded) rational agents. Proc Biol Sci 2021; 288:20202957. [PMID: 33529555 DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2020.2957] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Bayesian inference offers an optimal means of processing environmental information and so an advantage in natural selection. We consider the apparent, recent trend in increasing dysfunctional disagreement in, for example, political debate. This is puzzling because Bayesian inference benefits from powerful convergence theorems, precluding dysfunctional disagreement. Information overload is a plausible factor limiting the applicability of full Bayesian inference, but what is the link with dysfunctional disagreement? Individuals striving to be Bayesian-rational, but challenged by information overload, might simplify by using Bayesian networks or the separation of questions into knowledge partitions, the latter formalized with quantum probability theory. We demonstrate the massive simplification afforded by either approach, but also show how they contribute to dysfunctional disagreement.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Emmanuel M Pothos
- Department of Psychology, City, University of London, London EC1V 0HB, UK
| | | | - Irina Basieva
- Department of Psychology, City, University of London, London EC1V 0HB, UK
| | - Albert Barque-Duran
- Department of Computer Science, Universitat de Lleida, Carrer de Jaume II, 67, 25001 Lleida, Spain
| | - Katy Tapper
- Department of Psychology, City, University of London, London EC1V 0HB, UK
| | - Andrei Khrennikov
- International Center for Mathematical Modeling in Physics and Cognitive Science, Linnaeus University, Universitetplatsen 1, 351 95 Växjö, Sweden
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Cruz N, Hahn U, Fenton N, Lagnado D. Explaining Away, Augmentation, and the Assumption of Independence. Front Psychol 2020; 11:502751. [PMID: 33224043 PMCID: PMC7670039 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.502751] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/04/2019] [Accepted: 09/17/2020] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
In reasoning about situations in which several causes lead to a common effect, a much studied and yet still not well-understood inference is that of explaining away. Assuming that the causes contribute independently to the effect, if we learn that the effect is present, then this increases the probability that one or more of the causes are present. But if we then learn that a particular cause is present, this cause "explains" the presence of the effect, and the probabilities of the other causes decrease again. People tend to show this explaining away effect in their probability judgments, but to a lesser extent than predicted by the causal structure of the situation. We investigated further the conditions under which explaining away is observed. Participants estimated the probability of a cause, given the presence or the absence of another cause, for situations in which the effect was either present or absent, and the evidence about the effect was either certain or uncertain. Responses were compared to predictions obtained using Bayesian network modeling as well as a sensitivity analysis of the size of normative changes in probability under different information conditions. One of the conditions investigated: when there is certainty that the effect is absent, is special because under the assumption of causal independence, the probabilities of the causes remain invariant, that is, there is no normative explaining away or augmentation. This condition is therefore especially diagnostic of people's reasoning about common-effect structures. The findings suggest that, alongside earlier explanations brought forward in the literature, explaining away may occur less often when the causes are assumed to interact in their contribution to the effect, and when the normative size of the probability change is not large enough to be subjectively meaningful. Further, people struggled when given evidence against negative evidence, resembling a double negation effect.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nicole Cruz
- Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck, University of London, London, United Kingdom
| | - Ulrike Hahn
- Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck, University of London, London, United Kingdom
| | - Norman Fenton
- School of Electronic Engineering and Computer Science, Queen Mary University of London, London, United Kingdom
| | - David Lagnado
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University College London, London, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
Bottemanne H, Morlaàs O, Schmidt L, Fossati P. [Coronavirus: Predictive brain and terror management]. Encephale 2020; 46:S107-S113. [PMID: 32517998 PMCID: PMC7242918 DOI: 10.1016/j.encep.2020.05.012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/17/2020] [Accepted: 05/20/2020] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
Emerging infectious diseases like Covid-19 cause a major threat to global health. When confronted with new pathogens, individuals generate several beliefs about the epidemic phenomenon. Many studies have shown that individual protective behaviors largely depend on these beliefs. Due to the absence of treatment and vaccine against these emerging pathogens, the relation between these beliefs and these behaviors represents a crucial issue for public health policies. In the premises of the Covid-19 pandemic, several preliminary studies have highlighted a delay in the perception of risk by individuals, which potentially holds back the implementing of the necessary precautionary measures: people underestimated the risks associated with the virus, and therefore also the importance of complying with sanitary guidelines. During the peak of the pandemic, the salience of the threat and of the risk of mortality could then have transformed the way people generate their beliefs. This potentially leads to upheavals in the way they understand the world. Here, we propose to explore the evolution of beliefs and behaviors during the Covid-19 crisis, using the theory of predictive coding and the theory of terror management, two influential frameworks in cognitive science and in social psychology.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- H Bottemanne
- Control-Interoception-Attention team, Institut du cerveau et de la moelle épinière (ICM), UMR 7225/UMR_S 1127, Sorbonne University/CNRS/INSERM, Paris, France; Department for adult psychiatry, Pitié-Salpêtrière hospital, Assistance Publique-Hôpitaux de Paris (AP-HP), Paris, France.
| | - O Morlaàs
- Control-Interoception-Attention team, Institut du cerveau et de la moelle épinière (ICM), UMR 7225/UMR_S 1127, Sorbonne University/CNRS/INSERM, Paris, France
| | - L Schmidt
- Control-Interoception-Attention team, Institut du cerveau et de la moelle épinière (ICM), UMR 7225/UMR_S 1127, Sorbonne University/CNRS/INSERM, Paris, France
| | - P Fossati
- Control-Interoception-Attention team, Institut du cerveau et de la moelle épinière (ICM), UMR 7225/UMR_S 1127, Sorbonne University/CNRS/INSERM, Paris, France; Department for adult psychiatry, Pitié-Salpêtrière hospital, Assistance Publique-Hôpitaux de Paris (AP-HP), Paris, France
| |
Collapse
|
26
|
|
27
|
Perspectives on Correctness in Probabilistic Inference from Psychology. SPANISH JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY 2019; 22:E55. [PMID: 31868162 DOI: 10.1017/sjp.2019.48] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
Abstract
Research into decision making has enabled us to appreciate that the notion of correctness is multifaceted. Different normative framework for correctness can lead to different insights about correct behavior. We illustrate the shifts for correctness insights with two tasks, the Wason selection task and the conjunction fallacy task; these tasks have had key roles in the development of logical reasoning and decision making research respectively. The Wason selection task arguably has played an important part in the transition from understanding correctness using classical logic to classical probability theory (and information theory). The conjunction fallacy has enabled a similar shift from baseline classical probability theory to quantum probability. The focus of this overview is the latter, as it represents a novel way for understanding probabilistic inference in psychology. We conclude with some of the current challenges concerning the application of quantum probability theory in psychology in general and specifically for the problem of understanding correctness in decision making.
Collapse
|
28
|
Human confidence judgments reflect reliability-based hierarchical integration of contextual information. Nat Commun 2019; 10:5430. [PMID: 31780659 PMCID: PMC6882790 DOI: 10.1038/s41467-019-13472-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/15/2018] [Accepted: 11/07/2019] [Indexed: 11/08/2022] Open
Abstract
Our immediate observations must be supplemented with contextual information to resolve ambiguities. However, the context is often ambiguous too, and thus it should be inferred itself to guide behavior. Here, we introduce a novel hierarchical task (airplane task) in which participants should infer a higher-level, contextual variable to inform probabilistic inference about a hidden dependent variable at a lower level. By controlling the reliability of past sensory evidence through varying the sample size of the observations, we find that humans estimate the reliability of the context and combine it with current sensory uncertainty to inform their confidence reports. Behavior closely follows inference by probabilistic message passing between latent variables across hierarchical state representations. Commonly reported inferential fallacies, such as sample size insensitivity, are not present, and neither did participants appear to rely on simple heuristics. Our results reveal uncertainty-sensitive integration of information at different hierarchical levels and temporal scales. Because our immediate observations are often ambiguous, we must use the context (prior beliefs) to guide inference, but the context may also be uncertain. Here, the authors show that humans can accurately estimate the reliability of the context and combine it with sensory uncertainty to form their decisions and estimate confidence.
Collapse
|
29
|
Generality of likelihood ratio decisions. Cognition 2019; 191:103931. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2019.03.023] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/19/2019] [Accepted: 03/28/2019] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
|
30
|
The Cognitive Ecology of Stimulus Ambiguity: A Predator-Prey Perspective. Trends Ecol Evol 2019; 34:1048-1060. [PMID: 31416642 DOI: 10.1016/j.tree.2019.07.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 26] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/01/2019] [Revised: 07/01/2019] [Accepted: 07/08/2019] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
Organisms face the cognitive challenge of making decisions based on imperfect information. Predators and prey, in particular, are confronted with ambiguous stimuli when foraging and avoiding attacks. These challenges are accentuated by variation imposed by environmental, physiological, and cognitive factors. While the cognitive factors influencing perceived ambiguity are often assumed to be fixed, contemporary findings reveal that perceived ambiguity is instead the dynamic outcome of interactive cognitive processes. Here, we present a framework that integrates recent advances in neurophysiology and sensory ecology with a classic decision-making model, signal detection theory (SDT), to understand the cognitive mechanisms that shape perceived stimulus ambiguity in predators and prey. Since stimulus ambiguity is pervasive, the framework discussed here provides insights that extend into nonforaging contexts.
Collapse
|
31
|
Dornschneider S. High-Stakes Decision-Making Within Complex Social Environments: A Computational Model of Belief Systems in the Arab Spring. Cogn Sci 2019; 43:e12762. [PMID: 31310025 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.12762] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/15/2019] [Revised: 04/20/2018] [Accepted: 05/15/2019] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
People experiencing similar conditions may make different decisions, and their belief systems provide insight about these differences. An example of high-stakes decision-making within a complex social context is the Arab Spring, in which large numbers of people decided to protest and even larger numbers decided to stay at home. This study uses qualitative analyses of interview narratives and social media addressing individual decisions to develop a computational model tracing the cognitive decision-making process. The model builds on work by Abelson and Carroll (1965), Schank and Abelson ( a1977), and Axelrod (1976) to systematically trace the inferences connecting beliefs to decisions. The findings show that protest decisions were often based on positive emotions such as pride, hope, courage, and solidarity, triggered by beliefs about successful protest and self-sacrifice. By contrast, decisions to stay at home were triggered by beliefs about safety, state approval, and living conditions. As one participant said, "When I heard about the revolution in Tunisia, my heart was filled with solidarity for the people." In the words of a non-participant: "When people are killed, we must be careful. There are more important things than protest: safety and stability." This study of individual explanations about events identifies the role of emotions in high-stakes decision-making within complex social environments.
Collapse
|
32
|
Language Processing. Cognition 2019. [DOI: 10.1017/9781316271988.012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
|
33
|
Methods of Cognitive Psychology. Cognition 2019. [DOI: 10.1017/9781316271988.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
|
34
|
Cognitive Psychologists’ Approach to Research. Cognition 2019. [DOI: 10.1017/9781316271988.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
|
35
|
Visual Imagery. Cognition 2019. [DOI: 10.1017/9781316271988.013] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
|
36
|
Index. Cognition 2019. [DOI: 10.1017/9781316271988.018] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
|
37
|
Decision Making and Reasoning. Cognition 2019. [DOI: 10.1017/9781316271988.014] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
|
38
|
Attention. Cognition 2019. [DOI: 10.1017/9781316271988.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
|
39
|
Long-Term Memory Structure. Cognition 2019. [DOI: 10.1017/9781316271988.007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/05/2022]
|
40
|
Problem Solving. Cognition 2019. [DOI: 10.1017/9781316271988.015] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/05/2022]
|
41
|
Preface. Cognition 2019. [DOI: 10.1017/9781316271988.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
|
42
|
Sensory and Working Memory. Cognition 2019. [DOI: 10.1017/9781316271988.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
|
43
|
Memory Retrieval. Cognition 2019. [DOI: 10.1017/9781316271988.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
|
44
|
Visual Perception. Cognition 2019. [DOI: 10.1017/9781316271988.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/05/2022]
|
45
|
References. Cognition 2019. [DOI: 10.1017/9781316271988.017] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
|
46
|
Language Structure. Cognition 2019. [DOI: 10.1017/9781316271988.011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
|
47
|
Concepts and Categories. Cognition 2019. [DOI: 10.1017/9781316271988.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
|
48
|
Long-Term Memory Processes. Cognition 2019. [DOI: 10.1017/9781316271988.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
|
49
|
Glossary. Cognition 2019. [DOI: 10.1017/9781316271988.016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
|
50
|
Jubin J, Barrouillet P. Effects of context on the rate of conjunctive responses in the probabilistic truth table task. THINKING & REASONING 2019. [DOI: 10.1080/13546783.2018.1477689] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/14/2022]
|