1
|
Varma S, Sanford EM, Marupudi V, Shaffer O, Brooke Lea R. Recruitment of magnitude representations to understand graded words. Cogn Psychol 2024; 153:101673. [PMID: 39094253 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2024.101673] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/27/2022] [Revised: 06/17/2024] [Accepted: 07/22/2024] [Indexed: 08/04/2024]
Abstract
Language understanding and mathematics understanding are two fundamental forms of human thinking. Prior research has largely focused on the question of how language shapes mathematical thinking. The current study considers the converse question. Specifically, it investigates whether the magnitude representations that are thought to anchor understanding of number are also recruited to understand the meanings of graded words. These are words that come in scales (e.g., Anger) whose members can be ordered by the degree to which they possess the defining property (e.g., calm, annoyed, angry, furious). Experiment 1 uses the comparison paradigm to find evidence that the distance, ratio, and boundary effects that are taken as evidence of the recruitment of magnitude representations extend from numbers to words. Experiment 2 uses a similarity rating paradigm and multi-dimensional scaling to find converging evidence for these effects in graded word understanding. Experiment 3 evaluates an alternative hypothesis - that these effects for graded words simply reflect the statistical structure of the linguistic environment - by using machine learning models of distributional word semantics: LSA, word2vec, GloVe, counterfitted word vectors, BERT, RoBERTa, and GPT-2. These models fail to show the full pattern of effects observed of humans in Experiment 2, suggesting that more is needed than mere statistics. This research paves the way for further investigations of the role of magnitude representations in sentence and text comprehension, and of the question of whether language understanding and number understanding draw on shared or independent magnitude representations. It also informs the role of machine learning models in cognitive psychology research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sashank Varma
- School of Interactive Computing, Georgia Institute of Technology, United States; School of Psychology, Georgia Institute of Technology, United States.
| | - Emily M Sanford
- Department of Psychology, University of California - Berkeley, United States.
| | - Vijay Marupudi
- School of Interactive Computing, Georgia Institute of Technology, United States.
| | - Olivia Shaffer
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Louisville, United States.
| | - R Brooke Lea
- Department of Psychology, Macalester College, United States.
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
García-Orza J, Gutiérrez-Cordero I, Rodríguez-Montenegro I, Álvarez-Montesinos JA. Children's comparison of different-length numbers: Managing different attributes in multidigit number processing. J Exp Child Psychol 2024; 240:105827. [PMID: 38194820 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2023.105827] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/13/2023] [Revised: 11/08/2023] [Accepted: 11/09/2023] [Indexed: 01/11/2024]
Abstract
In everyday life the comparison of numbers usually occurs between numbers with different numbers of digits. However, experimental research here is scarce. Recent research has shown that adults respond faster to congruent pairs (the initial digit in the number with more digits is larger, e.g., 2384 vs. 107) than to incongruent pairs (the initial digit is larger in the number with fewer digits, e.g., 2675 vs. 398). This has been interpreted as support for the processing of multiple attributes in parallel and against serial accounts. The current research asked whether there is a change in the relevance of these attributes as school grades increase. School-age children from the second to sixth grades (N = 206) were presented with pairs of numbers that had either the same number of digits (3 vs. 3 or 4 vs. 4) or a different number of digits (3 vs. 4). In this latter condition, the stimuli, matched by distance, could be either length/digit congruent (e.g., 2384 vs. 107) or length/digit incongruent (e.g., 2675 vs. 398). Linear mixed models showed a length/digit congruity effect from second graders. Interestingly, in the response time measure, congruity interacted with school grade and the side in which the larger number of the pair was presented. Whereas these results support a model that considers number comparison as a process that weighs different attributes in parallel, it is also argued that developmental changes are associated with differences in the level of automatization of the componential skills involved in the comparison.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Javier García-Orza
- Numerical Cognition Lab, Faculty of Psychology and Speech Therapy, Universidad de Málaga, 29010 Málaga, Spain; Instituto de Investigación Biomédica de Málaga (IBIMA), 29590 Málaga, Spain; Department of Basic Psychology, Faculty of Psychology and Speech Therapy, Universidad de Málaga, 29010 Málaga, Spain.
| | - Ismael Gutiérrez-Cordero
- Numerical Cognition Lab, Faculty of Psychology and Speech Therapy, Universidad de Málaga, 29010 Málaga, Spain; Department of Basic Psychology, Faculty of Psychology and Speech Therapy, Universidad de Málaga, 29010 Málaga, Spain; Cognitive Neurology and Aphasia Unit, Centro de Investigaciones Médico-Sanitarias, Universidad de Málaga, 29071 Málaga. Spain
| | - Ismael Rodríguez-Montenegro
- Numerical Cognition Lab, Faculty of Psychology and Speech Therapy, Universidad de Málaga, 29010 Málaga, Spain
| | | |
Collapse
|
3
|
Alonso-Díaz S, Penagos-Londoño GI. Reduced choice-confidence in negative numerals. PLoS One 2022; 17:e0272796. [PMID: 36190954 PMCID: PMC9529092 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0272796] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/09/2021] [Accepted: 07/26/2022] [Indexed: 11/07/2022] Open
Abstract
Negative numbers are central in math. However, they are abstract, hard to learn, and manipulated slower than positive numbers regardless of math ability. It suggests that confidence, namely the post-decision estimate of being correct, should be lower than positives. We asked participants to pick the larger single-digit numeral in a pair and collected their implicit confidence with button pressure (button pressure was validated with three empirical signatures of confidence). We also modeled their choices with a drift-diffusion decision model to compute the post-decision estimate of being correct. We found that participants had relatively low confidence with negative numerals. Given that participants compared with high accuracy the basic base-10 symbols (0–9), reduced confidence may be a general feature of manipulating abstract negative numerals as they produce more uncertainty than positive numerals per unit of time.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Santiago Alonso-Díaz
- Department of Economics, Pontificia Universidad Javeriana, Bogotá, Colombia
- * E-mail:
| | | |
Collapse
|
4
|
Holistic representation of negative numbers: Evidence from duration comparison tasks. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2019; 193:123-131. [PMID: 30622021 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2018.12.012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/24/2018] [Revised: 11/26/2018] [Accepted: 12/21/2018] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
Abstract
Whether negative numbers are represented componentially as two separate components (a digit and a sign) or represented holistically as a whole is still under debate. The present study investigated the representation of negative numbers via duration comparison tasks that might eliminate the possible influences of the strategies participants usually use when processing numbers. In the duration comparison task, participants are required to compare the durations of two numbers that were presented sequentially, thus the numerical value is irrelevant to the task. Our results showed that negative numbers were processed holistically when positive numbers and negative numbers were presented in separate blocks (Exp. 1). But negative numbers showed a componential representation when positive numbers were intermixed with negative numbers within the same block (Exp. 2). This inconsistency might be due to the spatial separation of the polarity sign and digit components in negative numbers which may remind participants that negative numbers are formed by two components when there are positive numbers as a comparison in the same block. Therefore, when the spatial separation of the polarity sign and digit components in negative numbers was eliminated by using colors to mark the polarity (Exp. 3), the negative numbers were processed holistically even when positive numbers and negative numbers were intermixed. These results suggest that negative numbers are represented holistically and may be prone to be represented componentially when intermixed with positive numbers within the same block.
Collapse
|
5
|
Patel P, Varma S. How the Abstract Becomes Concrete: Irrational Numbers Are Understood Relative to Natural Numbers and Perfect Squares. Cogn Sci 2018; 42:1642-1676. [PMID: 29900573 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.12619] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/25/2017] [Revised: 03/14/2018] [Accepted: 03/28/2018] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
Mathematical cognition research has largely emphasized concepts that can be directly perceived or grounded in visuospatial referents. These include concrete number systems like natural numbers, integers, and rational numbers. Here, we investigate how a more abstract number system, the irrationals denoted by radical expressions like 2, is understood across three tasks. Performance on a magnitude comparison task suggests that people interpret irrational numbers (specifically, the radicands of radical expressions) as natural numbers. Strategy self-reports during a number line estimation task reveal that the spatial locations of irrationals are determined by referencing neighboring perfect squares. Finally, perfect squares facilitate the evaluation of arithmetic expressions. These converging results align with a constellation of related phenomena spanning tasks and number systems of varying complexity. Accordingly, we propose that the task-specific recruitment of more concrete representations to make sense of more abstract concepts (referential processing) is an important mechanism for teaching and learning mathematics.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Purav Patel
- Department of Educational Psychology, University of Minnesota
| | - Sashank Varma
- Department of Educational Psychology, University of Minnesota
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Mende MA, Shaki S, Fischer MH. Commentary: The mental representation of integers: An abstract-to-concrete shift in the understanding of mathematical concepts. Front Psychol 2018. [PMID: 29535662 PMCID: PMC5835043 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00209] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022] Open
Affiliation(s)
- Melinda A Mende
- Division of Cognitive Sciences, Department of Psychology, University of Potsdam, Potsdam, Germany
| | - Samuel Shaki
- Department of Behavioral Sciences, Ariel University, Ariel, Israel
| | - Martin H Fischer
- Division of Cognitive Sciences, Department of Psychology, University of Potsdam, Potsdam, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Kanayet FJ, Mattarella-Micke A, Kohler PJ, Norcia AM, McCandliss BD, McClelland JL. Distinct Representations of Magnitude and Spatial Position within Parietal Cortex during Number-Space Mapping. J Cogn Neurosci 2017; 30:200-218. [PMID: 29040015 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_01199] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/04/2022]
Abstract
Mapping numbers onto space is foundational to mathematical cognition. These cognitive operations are often conceptualized in the context of a "mental number line" and involve multiple brain regions in or near the intraparietal sulcus (IPS) that have been implicated both in numeral and spatial cognition. Here we examine possible differentiation of function within these brain areas in relating numbers to spatial positions. By isolating the planning phase of a number line task and introducing spatiotopic mapping tools from fMRI into mental number line task research, we are able to focus our analysis on the neural activity of areas in anterior IPS (aIPS) previously associated with number processing and on spatiotopically organized areas in and around posterior IPS (pIPS), while participants prepare to place a number on a number line. Our results support the view that the nonpositional magnitude of a numerical symbol is coded in aIPS, whereas the position of a number in space is coded in posterior areas of IPS. By focusing on the planning phase, we are able to isolate activation related to the cognitive, rather than the sensory-motor, aspects of the task. Also, to allow the separation of spatial position from magnitude, we tested both a standard positive number line (0 to 100) and a zero-centered mixed number line (-100 to 100). We found evidence of a functional dissociation between aIPS and pIPS: Activity in aIPS was associated with a landmark distance effect not modulated by spatial position, whereas activity in pIPS revealed a contralateral preference effect.
Collapse
|
8
|
Abstract
Past research has shown that numbers are associated with order in time such that performance in a numerical comparison task is enhanced when number pairs appear in ascending order, when the larger number follows the smaller one. This was found in the past for the integers 1–9 ( Ben-Meir, Ganor-Stern, & Tzelgov, 2013 ; Müller & Schwarz, 2008 ). In the present study we explored whether the advantage for processing numbers in ascending order exists also for fractions and negative numbers. The results demonstrate this advantage for fraction pairs and for integer-fraction pairs. However, the opposite advantage for descending order was found for negative numbers and for positive-negative number pairs. These findings are interpreted in the context of embodied cognition approaches and current theories on the mental representation of fractions and negative numbers.
Collapse
|
9
|
Gullick MM, Wolford G. Understanding less than nothing: children's neural response to negative numbers shifts across age and accuracy. Front Psychol 2013; 4:584. [PMID: 24058350 PMCID: PMC3767892 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00584] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/27/2013] [Accepted: 08/13/2013] [Indexed: 11/28/2022] Open
Abstract
We examined the brain activity underlying the development of our understanding of negative numbers, which are amounts lacking direct physical counterparts. Children performed a paired comparison task with positive and negative numbers during an fMRI session. As previously shown in adults, both pre-instruction fifth-graders and post-instruction seventh-graders demonstrated typical behavioral and neural distance effects to negative numbers, where response times and parietal and frontal activity increased as comparison distance decreased. We then determined the factors impacting the distance effect in each age group. Behaviorally, the fifth-grader distance effect for negatives was significantly predicted only by positive comparison accuracy, indicating that children who were generally better at working with numbers were better at comparing negatives. In seventh-graders, negative number comparison accuracy significantly predicted their negative number distance effect, indicating that children who were better at working with negative numbers demonstrated a more typical distance effect. Across children, as age increased, the negative number distance effect increased in the bilateral IPS and decreased frontally, indicating a frontoparietal shift consistent with previous numerical development literature. In contrast, as negative comparison task accuracy increased, the parietal distance effect increased in the left IPS and decreased in the right, possibly indicating a change from an approximate understanding of negatives' values to a more exact, precise representation (particularly supported by the left IPS) with increasing expertise. These shifts separately indicate the effects of increasing maturity generally in numeric processing and specifically in negative number understanding.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Margaret M Gullick
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Dartmouth College Hanover, NH, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
10
|
Gullick MM, Wolford G, Temple E. Understanding less than nothing: Neural distance effects for negative numbers. Neuroimage 2012; 62:542-54. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.04.058] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/04/2011] [Revised: 04/16/2012] [Accepted: 04/29/2012] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
|
11
|
Kong F, Zhao J, You X. Components representation of negative numbers: Evidence from auditory stimuli detection and number classification tasks. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 2012; 65:691-701. [DOI: 10.1080/17470218.2011.622048] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/14/2022]
Abstract
Past research suggested that negative numbers could be represented in terms of their components in the visual modality. The present study examined the processing of negative numbers in the auditory modality and whether it is affected by context. Experiment 1 employed a stimuli detection task where only negative numbers were presented binaurally. Experiment 2 employed the same task, but both positive and negative numbers were mixed as cues. A reverse attentional spatial–numerical association of response codes (SNARC) effect for negative numbers was obtained in these two experiments. Experiment 3 employed a number classification task where only negative numbers were presented binaurally. Experiment 4 employed the same task, but both positive and negative numbers were mixed. A reverse SNARC effect for negative numbers was obtained in these two experiments. These findings suggest that negative numbers in the auditory modality are generated from the set of positive numbers, thus supporting a components representation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Feng Kong
- Department of Psychology, Shaanxi Normal University, Xi'an, China
| | - Jingjing Zhao
- Department of Psychology, Shaanxi Normal University, Xi'an, China
| | - Xuqun You
- Department of Psychology, Shaanxi Normal University, Xi'an, China
- Shaanxi Provincial Key Laboratory of Behavior & Cognitive Neuroscience and Department of Psychology, Shaanxi Normal University, Xi'an, China
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Blair KP, Rosenberg-Lee M, Tsang JM, Schwartz DL, Menon V. Beyond natural numbers: negative number representation in parietal cortex. Front Hum Neurosci 2012; 6:7. [PMID: 22363276 PMCID: PMC3277269 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00007] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/01/2011] [Accepted: 01/18/2012] [Indexed: 11/29/2022] Open
Abstract
Unlike natural numbers, negative numbers do not have natural physical referents. How does the brain represent such abstract mathematical concepts? Two competing hypotheses regarding representational systems for negative numbers are a rule-based model, in which symbolic rules are applied to negative numbers to translate them into positive numbers when assessing magnitudes, and an expanded magnitude model, in which negative numbers have a distinct magnitude representation. Using an event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging design, we examined brain responses in 22 adults while they performed magnitude comparisons of negative and positive numbers that were quantitatively near (difference <4) or far apart (difference >6). Reaction times (RTs) for negative numbers were slower than positive numbers, and both showed a distance effect whereby near pairs took longer to compare. A network of parietal, frontal, and occipital regions were differentially engaged by negative numbers. Specifically, compared to positive numbers, negative number processing resulted in greater activation bilaterally in intraparietal sulcus (IPS), middle frontal gyrus, and inferior lateral occipital cortex. Representational similarity analysis revealed that neural responses in the IPS were more differentiated among positive numbers than among negative numbers, and greater differentiation among negative numbers was associated with faster RTs. Our findings indicate that despite negative numbers engaging the IPS more strongly, the underlying neural representation are less distinct than that of positive numbers. We discuss our findings in the context of the two theoretical models of negative number processing and demonstrate how multivariate approaches can provide novel insights into abstract number representation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kristen P Blair
- Stanford University School of Education, Stanford University Stanford, CA, USA
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
13
|
Ganor-Stern D. Fractions but not negative numbers are represented on the mental number line. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2012; 139:350-7. [PMID: 22192439 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2011.11.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/30/2011] [Revised: 11/19/2011] [Accepted: 11/21/2011] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
The present study is the first to directly compare numerical representations of positive numbers, negative numbers and unit fractions. The results show that negative numbers and unit fractions were not represented in the same way. Distance effects were found when positive numbers were compared with fractions but not when they were compared with negative numbers, thus suggesting that unit fractions but not negative numbers were represented on the number line with positive numbers. As indicated by the semantic congruity effect, negative numbers were perceived to be small, positive numbers were perceived as large, while unit fractions were perceived neither as large nor small. Comparisons between negative numbers were faster than between unit fractions, possibly due to the smaller differences between the holistic magnitudes of the unit fractions. Finally, comparing unit fractions to 1 was faster than comparing them to 0, consistent with the idea that unit fractions are perceived as entities smaller than 1 (Kallai & Tzelgov, 2009). The results are consistent with the idea of a mental division between numbers that represent a quantity (positive numbers and unit fractions) and those that do not (negative numbers).
Collapse
|
14
|
Zhang YU, You X. Extending the Mental Number Line—How Do Negative Numbers Contribute? Perception 2012; 41:1323-35. [DOI: 10.1068/p7081] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
Abstract
Previous studies suggest that there is an association between positive numbers and space; however, there is less agreement for negative numbers. The main purpose of the present study was to investigate the nature of the processing and representation of negative numbers, and the association between negative numbers and space. Results of the two experiments show that low-level processing (perception) of negative numbers can induce spatial shifts of attention. Whether this is caused by their numerical value or absolute value depends on the numerical context and task requirements, indicating that there are both components and holistic processing, and representation for negative numbers. The representation is automatically associated with leftward space; the coding and representation of the mental number line is adaptable to the specific numerical context and task requirements. The mental number line, therefore, can extend to the left side of zero, thus supporting the context-dependent view.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Y u Zhang
- School of Psychology, Shaanxi Normal University, Xi'an 710062, China
| | - Xuqun You
- School of Psychology, Shaanxi Normal University, Xi'an 710062, China
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Abstract
The present study examined the processing of unit fractions and the extent to which it is affected by context. Using a numerical comparison task we found evidence for a holistic representation of unit fractions when the immediate context of the fractions was emphasized, that is when the stimuli set included in addition to the unit fractions also the numbers 0 and 1. The holistic representation was indicated by the semantic congruity effect for comparisons of pairs of fractions and by the distance effect in comparisons of a fraction and 0 and 1. Consistent with previous results (Bonato, Fabbri, Umilta, & Zorzi, 2007) there was no evidence for a holistic representation of unit fractions when the stimulus set included only fractions. These findings suggest that fraction processing is context-dependent. Finally, the present results are discussed in the context of processing other complex numbers beyond the first decade.
Collapse
|
16
|
Varma S, Schwartz DL. The mental representation of integers: an abstract-to-concrete shift in the understanding of mathematical concepts. Cognition 2011; 121:363-85. [PMID: 21939966 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2011.08.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 29] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/02/2010] [Revised: 08/02/2011] [Accepted: 08/08/2011] [Indexed: 10/17/2022]
Abstract
Mathematics has a level of structure that transcends untutored intuition. What is the cognitive representation of abstract mathematical concepts that makes them meaningful? We consider this question in the context of the integers, which extend the natural numbers with zero and negative numbers. Participants made greater and lesser judgments of pairs of integers. Experiment 1 demonstrated an inverse distance effect: When comparing numbers across the zero boundary, people are faster when the numbers are near together (e.g., -1 vs. 2) than when they are far apart (e.g., -1 vs. 7). This result conflicts with a straightforward symbolic or analog magnitude representation of integers. We therefore propose an analog-x hypothesis: Mastering a new symbol system restructures the existing magnitude representation to encode its unique properties. We instantiate analog-x in a reflection model: The mental negative number line is a reflection of the positive number line. Experiment 2 replicated the inverse distance effect and corroborated the model. Experiment 3 confirmed a developmental prediction: Children, who have yet to restructure their magnitude representation to include negative magnitudes, use rules to compare negative numbers. Taken together, the experiments suggest an abstract-to-concrete shift: Symbolic manipulation can transform an existing magnitude representation so that it incorporates additional perceptual-motor structure, in this case symmetry about a boundary. We conclude with a second symbolic-magnitude model that instantiates analog-x using a feature-based representation, and that begins to explain the restructuring process.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sashank Varma
- Department of Educational Psychology, University of Minnesota, 165 Education Sciences Building, 56 East River Rd., Minneapolis, MN 55455, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|
17
|
Chassy P, Grodd W. Comparison of quantities: core and format-dependent regions as revealed by fMRI. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2011; 22:1420-30. [PMID: 21878489 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhr219] [Citation(s) in RCA: 38] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Abstract
The perception and handling of numbers is central to education. Numerous imaging studies have focused on how quantities are encoded in the brain. Yet, only a few studies have touched upon number mining: the ability to extract the magnitude encoded in a visual stimulus. This article aims to characterize how analogue (i.e., disks and dots) and symbolic (i.e., positive and negative integers) formats influence number mining and the representation of quantities. Sixteen adult volunteers completed a comparison task while we recorded the blood oxygen level-dependent response using functional magnetic resonance imaging. The results revealed that a restricted set of specific subdivisions in the right intraparietal sulcus is activated in all conditions. With respect to magnitude assessment, the results show that 1) analogue stimuli are predominantly processed in the right hemisphere and that 2) symbolic stimuli encompass the analogue system and further recruit areas in the left hemisphere. Crucially, we found that polarity is encoded independently from magnitude. We refine the triple-code model by integrating our findings.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Philippe Chassy
- Institute of Medical Psychology and Behavioral Neurobiology, Faculty of Medicine, University Hospital Tübingen, 72076 Tübingen, Germany.
| | | |
Collapse
|
18
|
Krajcsi A, Igács J. Processing negative numbers by transforming negatives to positive range and by sign shortcut. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2010. [DOI: 10.1080/09541440903211113] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
|
19
|
Meert G, Grégoire J, Noël MP. Comparing 5/7 and 2/9: Adults can do it by accessing the magnitude of the whole fractions. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2010; 135:284-92. [PMID: 20797686 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.07.014] [Citation(s) in RCA: 52] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/06/2009] [Revised: 07/15/2010] [Accepted: 07/24/2010] [Indexed: 11/26/2022] Open
Abstract
This study investigated adults' ability to compare the magnitude of fractions without common components (e.g., 5/7 and 3/8), and the representation accessed in that process. We hypothesized that the absence of common components would enhance access to the magnitude of the fractions (i.e., a holistic representation) rather than a direct comparison of the numerators or the denominators. This hypothesis was tested in four between-subject conditions. Two types of experimental pairs were used that differed in the congruity of the magnitude of the denominator and the magnitude of the fraction. Each type of experimental pair was presented either alone or with filler pairs that introduced variability into the congruity of the components. In all four conditions, accuracy was above chance and the effect of the distance between the fractions on response times was significant, indicating an access to the magnitude of the fractions. Nevertheless, the variability of the congruity of the components had also a significant effect on performance, suggesting that the relative magnitude of the components was also processed. In conclusion, the representation of the fraction magnitude is hybrid, rather than purely holistic, in a magnitude-comparison task on fractions without common components.
Collapse
|
20
|
Ganor-Stern D, Pinhas M, Kallai A, Tzelgov J. Holistic Representation of Negative Numbers is Formed When Needed for the Task. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 2010; 63:1969-81. [DOI: 10.1080/17470211003721667] [Citation(s) in RCA: 22] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
Abstract
Past research suggested that negative numbers are represented in terms of their components—the polarity marker and the number (e.g., Fischer & Rottmann, 2005; Ganor-Stern & Tzelgov, 2008). The present study shows that a holistic representation is formed when needed for the task requirement. Specifically, performing the numerical comparison task on positive and negative numbers presented sequentially required participants to hold both the polarity and the number magnitude in memory. Such a condition resulted in a holistic representation of negative numbers, as indicated by the distance and semantic congruity effects. This holistic representation was added to the initial components representation, thus producing a hybrid holistic-components representation.
Collapse
|
21
|
Abstract
Are negative numbers processed differently from positive numbers in arithmetic problems? In two experiments, adults ( N = 66) solved standard addition and subtraction problems such as 3 + 4 and 7 – 4 and recasted versions that included explicit negative signs—that is, 3 – (–4), 7 + (–4), and (–4) + 7. Solution times on the recasted problems were slower than those on standard problems, but the effect was much larger for addition than subtraction. The negative sign may prime subtraction in both kinds of recasted problem. Problem size effects were the same or smaller in recasted than in standard problems, suggesting that the recasted formats did not interfere with mental calculation. These results suggest that the underlying conceptual structure of the problem (i.e., addition vs. subtraction) is more important for solution processes than the presence of negative numbers.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Runa Das
- Department of Psychology, Carleton University, Ottawa, ON, Canada
| | - Jo-Anne LeFevre
- Department of Psychology and Institute of Cognitive Science, Carleton University, Ottawa, ON, Canada
| | | |
Collapse
|
22
|
Tzelgov J, Ganor-Stern D, Maymon-Schreiber K. The Representation of Negative Numbers: Exploring the Effects of Mode of Processing and Notation. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 2009; 62:605-24. [DOI: 10.1080/17470210802034751] [Citation(s) in RCA: 38] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
Abstract
The representation of negative numbers was explored during intentional processing (i.e., when participants performed a numerical comparison task) and during automatic processing (i.e., when participants performed a physical comparison task). Performance in both cases suggested that negative numbers were not represented as a whole but rather their polarity and numerical magnitudes were represented separately. To explore whether this was due to the fact that polarity and magnitude are marked by two spatially separated symbols, participants were trained to mark polarity by colour. In this case there was still evidence for a separate representation of polarity and magnitude. However, when a different set of stimuli was used to refer to positive and negative numbers, and polarity was not marked separately, participants were able to represent polarity and magnitude together when numerical processing was performed intentionally but not when it was conducted automatically. These results suggest that notation is only partly responsible for the components representation of negative numbers and that the concept of negative numbers can be grasped only through that of positive numbers.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Joseph Tzelgov
- Achva Academic College and Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beer-Sheva, Israel
| | | | | |
Collapse
|