1
|
Nichols ES, Wild CJ, Owen AM, Soddu A. Cognition across the Lifespan: Investigating Age, Sex, and Other Sociodemographic Influences. Behav Sci (Basel) 2021; 11:bs11040051. [PMID: 33924660 PMCID: PMC8070049 DOI: 10.3390/bs11040051] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/22/2021] [Revised: 04/10/2021] [Accepted: 04/11/2021] [Indexed: 11/16/2022] Open
Abstract
Maintaining cognitive health across the lifespan has been the focus of a multi-billion-dollar industry. In order to guide treatment and interventions, a clear understanding of the way that proficiency in different cognitive domains develops and declines in both sexes across the lifespan is necessary. Additionally, there are sex differences in a range of other factors, including psychiatric illnesses such as anxiety, depression, and substance use, that are also known to affect cognition, although the scale of this interaction is unknown. Our objective was to assess differences in cognitive function across the lifespan in men and women in a large, representative sample. Leveraging online cognitive testing, a sample of 9451 men and 9451 women ranging in age from 12 to 69 (M = 28.21) matched on socio-demographic factors were studied. Segmented regression was used to model three cognitive domains—working memory, verbal abilities, and reasoning. Sex differences in all three domains were minimal; however, after broadening the sample in terms of socio-demographic factors, sex differences appeared. These results suggest that cognition across the lifespan differs for men and women, but is greatly influenced by environmental factors. We discuss these findings within a framework that describes sex differences in cognition as likely guided by a complex interplay between biology and environment.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Emily S. Nichols
- Faculty of Education, Western University, London, ON N6A 3K7, Canada
- Brain and Mind Institute, Western University, London, ON N6A 3K7, Canada; (C.J.W.); (A.M.O.); (A.S.)
- Correspondence: ; Tel.: +1-519-661-2111 (ext. 89151)
| | - Conor J. Wild
- Brain and Mind Institute, Western University, London, ON N6A 3K7, Canada; (C.J.W.); (A.M.O.); (A.S.)
| | - Adrian M. Owen
- Brain and Mind Institute, Western University, London, ON N6A 3K7, Canada; (C.J.W.); (A.M.O.); (A.S.)
- Department of Physiology and Pharmacology, Western University, London, ON N6A 3K7, Canada
- Department of Psychology, Western University, London, ON N6A 3K7, Canada
| | - Andrea Soddu
- Brain and Mind Institute, Western University, London, ON N6A 3K7, Canada; (C.J.W.); (A.M.O.); (A.S.)
- Department of Physics and Astronomy, Western University, London, ON N6A 3K7, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Chen L, Wang Y, Wen H. Numerical Magnitude Processing in Deaf Adolescents and Its Contribution to Arithmetical Ability. Front Psychol 2021; 12:584183. [PMID: 33841229 PMCID: PMC8026863 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.584183] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/17/2020] [Accepted: 03/02/2021] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Although most deaf individuals could use sign language or sign/spoken language mix, hearing loss would still affect their language acquisition. Compensatory plasticity holds that the lack of auditory stimulation experienced by deaf individuals, such as congenital deafness, can be met by enhancements in visual cognition. And the studies of hearing individuals have showed that visual form perception is the cognitive mechanism that could explain the association between numerical magnitude processing and arithmetic computation. Therefore, we examined numerical magnitude processing and its contribution to arithmetical ability in deaf adolescents, and explored the differences between the congenital and acquired deafness. 112 deaf adolescents (58 congenital deafness) and 58 hearing adolescents performed a series of cognitive and mathematical tests, and it was found there was no significant differences between the congenital group and the hearing group, but congenital group outperformed acquired group in numerical magnitude processing (reaction time) and arithmetic computation. It was also found there was a close association between numerical magnitude processing and arithmetic computation in all deaf adolescents, and after controlling for the demographic variables (age, gender, onset of hearing loss) and general cognitive abilities (non-verbal IQ, processing speed, reading comprehension), numerical magnitude processing could predict arithmetic computation in all deaf adolescents but not in congenital group. The role of numerical magnitude processing (symbolic and non-symbolic) in deaf adolescents' mathematical performance should be paid attention in the training of arithmetical ability.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lilan Chen
- School of Psychology, Hainan Normal University, Haikou, China
| | - Yan Wang
- Faculty of Education, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China
| | - Hongbo Wen
- Collaborative Innovation Center of Assessment Toward Basic Education Quality, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Zhang T, Chen C, Chen C, Wei W. Gender differences in the development of semantic and spatial processing of numbers. BRITISH JOURNAL OF DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2020; 38:391-414. [PMID: 32212402 DOI: 10.1111/bjdp.12329] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/27/2018] [Revised: 02/19/2020] [Indexed: 01/29/2023]
Abstract
This study recruited kindergarteners and first graders to investigate gender and grade differences in semantic and spatial processing of number magnitude. Results based on the Bayesian statistics showed that (1) there was extreme evidence in favour of grade differences in both semantic processing and spatial processing; (2) there were no gender differences in semantic processing; and (3) boys developed earlier than girls in spatial processing of numbers, especially for the more difficult task. These results are discussed in terms of gender differences in cognitive mechanisms underlying semantic and spatial processing of number magnitude.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tingyan Zhang
- Department of Psychology and Behavioral Sciences, Zhejiang University, Hang Zhou, China
| | - Chuansheng Chen
- Department of Psychology and Social Behavior, University of California, Irvine, California
| | - Chen Chen
- Department of Psychology and Behavioral Sciences, Zhejiang University, Hang Zhou, China
| | - Wei Wei
- Department of Psychology and Behavioral Sciences, Zhejiang University, Hang Zhou, China
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Mediation Relationships Among Gender, Spatial Ability, Math Anxiety, and Math Achievement. EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY REVIEW 2019. [DOI: 10.1007/s10648-019-09487-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
|
5
|
Hutchison JE, Lyons IM, Ansari D. More Similar Than Different: Gender Differences in Children's Basic Numerical Skills Are the Exception Not the Rule. Child Dev 2018; 90:e66-e79. [DOI: 10.1111/cdev.13044] [Citation(s) in RCA: 37] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Affiliation(s)
| | - Ian M. Lyons
- University of Western Ontario
- Georgetown University
| | | |
Collapse
|
6
|
Abstract
Visual-spatial working memory measures are widely used in clinical and experimental settings. Furthermore, it has been argued that the male advantage in spatial abilities can be explained by a sex difference in visual-spatial working memory. Therefore, sex differences in visual-spatial working memory have important implication for research, theory, and practice, but they have yet to be quantified. The present meta-analysis quantified the magnitude of sex differences in visual-spatial working memory and examined variables that might moderate them. The analysis used a set of 180 effect sizes from healthy males and females drawn from 98 samples ranging in mean age from 3 to 86 years. Multilevel meta-analysis was used on the overall data set to account for non-independent effect sizes. The data also were analyzed in separate task subgroups by means of multilevel and mixed-effects models. Results showed a small but significant male advantage (mean d = 0.155, 95 % confidence interval = 0.087-0.223). All the tasks produced a male advantage, except for memory for location, where a female advantage emerged. Age of the participants was a significant moderator, indicating that sex differences in visual-spatial working memory appeared first in the 13-17 years age group. Removing memory for location tasks from the sample affected the pattern of significant moderators. The present results indicate a male advantage in visual-spatial working memory, although age and specific task modulate the magnitude and direction of the effects. Implications for clinical applications, cognitive model building, and experimental research are discussed.
Collapse
|
7
|
Reinert RM, Huber S, Nuerk HC, Moeller K. Sex differences in number line estimation: The role of numerical estimation. Br J Psychol 2016; 108:334-350. [DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12203] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/24/2015] [Revised: 02/29/2016] [Indexed: 12/12/2022]
Affiliation(s)
| | - Stefan Huber
- Knowledge Media Research Center; Tuebingen Germany
| | - Hans-Christoph Nuerk
- Knowledge Media Research Center; Tuebingen Germany
- Department of Psychology; Eberhard-Karls University; Tuebingen Germany
| | - Korbinian Moeller
- Knowledge Media Research Center; Tuebingen Germany
- Department of Psychology; Eberhard-Karls University; Tuebingen Germany
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Hill F, Mammarella IC, Devine A, Caviola S, Passolunghi MC, Szűcs D. Maths anxiety in primary and secondary school students: Gender differences, developmental changes and anxiety specificity. LEARNING AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES 2016. [DOI: 10.1016/j.lindif.2016.02.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 149] [Impact Index Per Article: 18.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
|
9
|
Wasner M, Nuerk HC, Martignon L, Roesch S, Moeller K. Finger gnosis predicts a unique but small part of variance in initial arithmetic performance. J Exp Child Psychol 2016; 146:1-16. [PMID: 26895483 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2016.01.006] [Citation(s) in RCA: 31] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/17/2015] [Revised: 01/05/2016] [Accepted: 01/07/2016] [Indexed: 11/24/2022]
Abstract
Recent studies indicated that finger gnosis (i.e., the ability to perceive and differentiate one's own fingers) is associated reliably with basic numerical competencies. In this study, we aimed at examining whether finger gnosis is also a unique predictor for initial arithmetic competencies at the beginning of first grade-and thus before formal math instruction starts. Therefore, we controlled for influences of domain-specific numerical precursor competencies, domain-general cognitive ability, and natural variables such as gender and age. Results from 321 German first-graders revealed that finger gnosis indeed predicted a unique and relevant but nevertheless only small part of the variance in initial arithmetic performance (∼1%-2%) as compared with influences of general cognitive ability and numerical precursor competencies. Taken together, these results substantiated the notion of a unique association between finger gnosis and arithmetic and further corroborate the theoretical idea of finger-based representations contributing to numerical cognition. However, the only small part of variance explained by finger gnosis seems to limit its relevance for diagnostic purposes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mirjam Wasner
- Department of Psychology, Eberhard Karls University, 72076 Tuebingen, Germany.
| | - Hans-Christoph Nuerk
- Department of Psychology, Eberhard Karls University, 72076 Tuebingen, Germany; Leibniz-Institut für Wissensmedien, 72076 Tuebingen, Germany
| | - Laura Martignon
- Institute of Mathematics and Computing, University of Education, 71634 Ludwigsburg, Germany
| | | | - Korbinian Moeller
- Department of Psychology, Eberhard Karls University, 72076 Tuebingen, Germany; Leibniz-Institut für Wissensmedien, 72076 Tuebingen, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Trezise K, Reeve RA. Cognition-emotion interactions: patterns of change and implications for math problem solving. Front Psychol 2014; 5:840. [PMID: 25132830 PMCID: PMC4116786 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00840] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/28/2014] [Accepted: 07/14/2014] [Indexed: 11/17/2022] Open
Abstract
Surprisingly little is known about whether relationships between cognitive and emotional states remain stable or change over time, or how different patterns of stability and/or change in the relationships affect problem solving abilities. Nevertheless, cross-sectional studies show that anxiety/worry may reduce working memory (WM) resources, and the ability to minimize the effects anxiety/worry is higher in individuals with greater WM capacity. To investigate the patterns of stability and/or change in cognition-emotion relations over time and their implications for problem solving, 126 14-year-olds’ algebraic WM and worry levels were assessed twice in a single day before completing an algebraic math problem solving test. We used latent transition analysis to identify stability/change in cognition-emotion relations, which yielded a six subgroup solution. Subgroups varied in WM capacity, worry, and stability/change relationships. Among the subgroups, we identified a high WM/low worry subgroup that remained stable over time and a high WM/high worry, and a moderate WM/low worry subgroup that changed to low WM subgroups over time. Patterns of stability/change in subgroup membership predicted algebraic test results. The stable high WM/low worry subgroup performed best and the low WM capacity-high worry “unstable across time” subgroup performed worst. The findings highlight the importance of assessing variations in cognition-emotion relationships over time (rather than assessing cognition or emotion states alone) to account for differences in problem solving abilities.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kelly Trezise
- Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences, University of Melbourne Parkville, VIC, Australia
| | - Robert A Reeve
- Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences, University of Melbourne Parkville, VIC, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Relationships between 9-Year-Olds' Math and Literacy Worries and Academic Abilities. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2012. [DOI: 10.1155/2012/359089] [Citation(s) in RCA: 30] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
We investigated whether 9-year-olds experience math and/or literacy worries and, if they do, whether it is related to problem-solving abilities. Fifty-eight children judged the correctness of math, literacy, and mental rotation problems that differed in difficulty and rated their worry level about the correctness of judgments. Nonverbal IQ, general math, and literacy abilities were also assessed. Results showed children's worry ratings varied as a function of task and problem difficulty. Latent class analyses of math and literacy worry ratings revealed high-, moderate- and low-worry subgroups in both domains. The high-worry math subgroup exhibited poorer math performance than the other math subgroups, demonstrating a link between math worry and math performance. No relationship was found between worry literacy subgroups and literacy performance. Moreover, no relationship was found between teachers’ rating of children's academic and general worry and children’s own worry ratings. The relevance of the findings for understanding math and literacy worry is discussed.
Collapse
|
12
|
|