1
|
Mikell J, Powell D. Illusory implications: incidental exposure to ideas can induce beliefs. ROYAL SOCIETY OPEN SCIENCE 2025; 12:240716. [PMID: 39845716 PMCID: PMC11750381 DOI: 10.1098/rsos.240716] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/29/2024] [Revised: 06/28/2024] [Accepted: 12/02/2024] [Indexed: 01/24/2025]
Abstract
Numerous psychological findings have shown that incidental exposure to ideas makes those ideas seem more true, a finding commonly referred to as the 'illusory truth' effect. Under many accounts of the illusory truth effect, initial exposure to a statement provides a metacognitive feeling of 'fluency' or familiarity that, upon subsequent exposure, leads people to infer that the statement is more likely to be true. However, genuine beliefs do not only affect truth judgements about individual statements, they also imply other beliefs and drive decision-making. Here, we consider whether exposure to 'premise' statements affects people's truth ratings for novel 'implied' statements, a pattern of findings we call the 'illusory implication' effect. We argue these effects would constitute evidence for genuine belief change from incidental exposure and identify a handful of existing findings that offer preliminary support for this claim. Building upon these, we conduct three new preregistered experiments to further test this hypothesis, finding additional evidence that exposure to 'premise' statements affected participants' truth ratings for novel 'implied' statements, including for considerably more distant implications than those previously explored. Our findings suggest that the effects of incidental exposure reach further than previously thought, with potentially consequential implications for concerns around mis- and dis-information.
Collapse
|
2
|
Abel M, Bäuml KHT. Item-method directed forgetting and perceived truth of news headlines. Memory 2023; 31:1371-1386. [PMID: 37819019 DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2023.2267191] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/05/2023] [Accepted: 09/06/2023] [Indexed: 10/13/2023]
Abstract
Research on item-method directed forgetting (IMDF) suggests that memories can be voluntarily forgotten. IMDF is however usually examined with relatively simple study materials, such as single words or pictures. In the present study, we examined voluntary forgetting of news headlines from (presumably) untrustworthy sources. Experiment 1 found intact IMDF when to-be-forgotten headlines were characterised as untrustworthy and to-be-remembered headlines were characterised as trustworthy. Experiment 2 separated remember/forget cues and trustworthiness prompts. Forget cues alone had a large effect on memory, but only a small reducing effect on perceived truth. In contrast, trustworthiness prompts alone had essentially no effect on memory, but a large effect on perceived truth. Finally, Experiment 3 fully crossed forget/remember cues and trustworthiness prompts, revealing that forget cues can reduce memory irrespective of whether headlines are characterised as trustworthy or untrustworthy. Moreover, forget cues may bias source attributions, which can explain their small reducing effect on perceived truth. Overall, this work suggests that news headlines can be voluntarily forgotten. At least when people are motivated to forget information from untrustworthy sources, such forgetting may be helpful for curtailing the spread of false information.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Magdalena Abel
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Regensburg University, Regensburg, Germany
| | - Karl-Heinz T Bäuml
- Department of Experimental Psychology, Regensburg University, Regensburg, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Bell R, Mieth L, Buchner A. Coping with high advertising exposure: a source-monitoring perspective. Cogn Res Princ Implic 2022; 7:82. [PMID: 36064819 PMCID: PMC9444107 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-022-00433-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/27/2021] [Accepted: 08/22/2022] [Indexed: 11/15/2022] Open
Abstract
Consumers are exposed to large amounts of advertising every day. One way to avoid being manipulated is to monitor the sources of persuasive messages. In the present study it was tested whether high exposure to advertising affects the memory and guessing processes underlying source attributions. Participants were exposed to high or low proportions of advertising messages that were intermixed with product statements from a trustworthy source. In a subsequent memory test, participants had to remember the sources of these statements. In Experiments 1 and 2, high advertising exposure led to increased source memory and decreased recognition of the statements in comparison to low advertising exposure. High advertising exposure also induced an increased tendency toward guessing that statements whose sources were not remembered came from advertising. The results of Experiment 3 suggest that the presence of advertising, relative to its absence, leads to a skeptical guessing bias. Being exposed to advertising thus has pronounced effects on the memory and guessing processes underlying source attributions. These changes in source monitoring can be interpreted as coping mechanisms that serve to protect against the persuasive influence of advertising messages.
Collapse
|
4
|
Zhou LT, Zhang J, Tan L, Huang HZ, Zhou Y, Liu ZQ, Lu Y, Zhu LQ, Yao C, Liu D. Elevated Levels of miR-144-3p Induce Cholinergic Degeneration by Impairing the Maturation of NGF in Alzheimer's Disease. Front Cell Dev Biol 2021; 9:667412. [PMID: 33898468 PMCID: PMC8063700 DOI: 10.3389/fcell.2021.667412] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/13/2021] [Accepted: 03/11/2021] [Indexed: 12/11/2022] Open
Abstract
Cholinergic degeneration is one of the key pathological hallmarks of Alzheimer’s disease (AD), a condition that is characterized by synaptic disorders and memory impairments. Nerve growth factor (NGF) is secreted in brain regions that receive projections from the basal forebrain cholinergic neurons. The trophic effects of NGF rely on the appropriate maturation of NGF from its precursor, proNGF. The ratio of proNGF/NGF is known to be increased in patients with AD; however, the mechanisms that underlie this observation have yet to be elucidated. Here, we demonstrated that levels of miR-144-3p are increased in the hippocampi and the medial prefrontal cortex of an APP/PS1 mouse model of AD. These mice also exhibited cholinergic degeneration (including the loss of cholinergic fibers, the repression of choline acetyltransferase (ChAT) activity, the reduction of cholinergic neurons, and an increased number of dystrophic neurites) and synaptic/memory deficits. The elevated expression of miR-144-3p specifically targets the mRNA of tissue plasminogen activator (tPA) and reduces the expression of tPA, thus resulting in the abnormal maturation of NGF. The administration of miR-144-3p fully replicated the cholinergic degeneration and synaptic/memory deficits observed in the APP/PS1 mice. The injection of an antagomir of miR-144-3p into the hippocampi partially rescued cholinergic degeneration and synaptic/memory impairments by restoring the levels of tPA protein and by correcting the ratio of proNGF/NGF. Collectively, our research revealed potential mechanisms for the disturbance of NGF maturation and cholinergic degeneration in AD and identified a potential therapeutic target for AD.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lan-Ting Zhou
- Department of Pathophysiology, School of Basic Medicine, Tongji Medical College, Huazhong University of Science and Technology, Wuhan, China.,Collaborative Innovation Center for Brain Science, The Institute of Brain Research, Huazhong University of Science and Technology, Wuhan, China
| | - Juan Zhang
- Department of Pathophysiology, School of Basic Medicine, Tongji Medical College, Huazhong University of Science and Technology, Wuhan, China.,Collaborative Innovation Center for Brain Science, The Institute of Brain Research, Huazhong University of Science and Technology, Wuhan, China
| | - Lu Tan
- Department of Pathophysiology, School of Basic Medicine, Tongji Medical College, Huazhong University of Science and Technology, Wuhan, China.,Collaborative Innovation Center for Brain Science, The Institute of Brain Research, Huazhong University of Science and Technology, Wuhan, China
| | - He-Zhou Huang
- Department of Pathophysiology, School of Basic Medicine, Tongji Medical College, Huazhong University of Science and Technology, Wuhan, China.,Collaborative Innovation Center for Brain Science, The Institute of Brain Research, Huazhong University of Science and Technology, Wuhan, China
| | - Yang Zhou
- Department of Pathophysiology, School of Basic Medicine, Tongji Medical College, Huazhong University of Science and Technology, Wuhan, China.,Collaborative Innovation Center for Brain Science, The Institute of Brain Research, Huazhong University of Science and Technology, Wuhan, China
| | - Zhi-Qiang Liu
- Department of Pathophysiology, School of Basic Medicine, Tongji Medical College, Huazhong University of Science and Technology, Wuhan, China.,Collaborative Innovation Center for Brain Science, The Institute of Brain Research, Huazhong University of Science and Technology, Wuhan, China
| | - Youming Lu
- Collaborative Innovation Center for Brain Science, The Institute of Brain Research, Huazhong University of Science and Technology, Wuhan, China
| | - Ling-Qiang Zhu
- Department of Pathophysiology, School of Basic Medicine, Tongji Medical College, Huazhong University of Science and Technology, Wuhan, China.,Collaborative Innovation Center for Brain Science, The Institute of Brain Research, Huazhong University of Science and Technology, Wuhan, China
| | - Chengye Yao
- Department of Neurology, Union Hospital, Tongji Medical College, Huazhong University of Science and Technology, Wuhan, China
| | - Dan Liu
- Collaborative Innovation Center for Brain Science, The Institute of Brain Research, Huazhong University of Science and Technology, Wuhan, China
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Jalbert M, Schwarz N, Newman E. Only half of what i’ll tell you is true: Expecting to encounter falsehoods reduces illusory truth. JOURNAL OF APPLIED RESEARCH IN MEMORY AND COGNITION 2020. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jarmac.2020.08.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
|
6
|
Abstract
Misinformation causes serious harm, from sowing doubt in modern medicine to inciting violence. Older adults are especially susceptible - they shared the most fake news during the 2016 US election. The most intuitive explanation for this pattern blames cognitive deficits. While older adults forget where they learned information, fluency remains intact and decades of accumulated knowledge helps them evaluate claims. Thus, cognitive declines cannot fully explain older adults' engagement with fake news. Late adulthood also involves social changes, including general trust, difficulty detecting lies, and less emphasis on accuracy when communicating. In addition, older adults are relative newcomers to social media, who may struggle to spot sponsored content or manipulated images. In a post-truth world, interventions should consider older adults' shifting social goals and gaps in their digital literacy.
Collapse
|
7
|
Béna J, Carreras O, Terrier P. L’effet de vérité induit par la répétition : revue critique de l’hypothèse de familiarité. ANNEE PSYCHOLOGIQUE 2019. [DOI: 10.3917/anpsy1.193.0397] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/14/2022]
|
8
|
De keersmaecker J, Dunning D, Pennycook G, Rand DG, Sanchez C, Unkelbach C, Roets A. Investigating the Robustness of the Illusory Truth Effect Across Individual Differences in Cognitive Ability, Need for Cognitive Closure, and Cognitive Style. PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN 2019; 46:204-215. [DOI: 10.1177/0146167219853844] [Citation(s) in RCA: 49] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
People are more inclined to believe that information is true if they have encountered it before. Little is known about whether this illusory truth effect is influenced by individual differences in cognition. In seven studies (combined N = 2,196), using both trivia statements (Studies 1-6) and partisan news headlines (Study 7), we investigate moderation by three factors that have been shown to play a critical role in epistemic processes: cognitive ability (Studies 1, 2, 5), need for cognitive closure (Study 1), and cognitive style, that is, reliance on intuitive versus analytic thinking (Studies 1, 3-7). All studies showed a significant illusory truth effect, but there was no evidence for moderation by any of the cognitive measures across studies. These results indicate that the illusory truth effect is robust to individual differences in cognitive ability, need for cognitive closure, and cognitive style.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | | | - David G. Rand
- Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, USA
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
9
|
Malone C, Deason RG, Palumbo R, Heyworth N, Tat M, Budson AE. False memories in patients with mild cognitive impairment and mild Alzheimer's disease dementia: Can cognitive strategies help? J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2019; 41:204-218. [PMID: 30179518 PMCID: PMC6399077 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2018.1513453] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/08/2018] [Accepted: 08/05/2018] [Indexed: 02/07/2023]
Abstract
Alzheimer's disease (AD) is a progressive neurodegenerative disorder that presents predominantly with impairments in learning and memory. Patients with AD are also susceptible to false memories, a clinically relevant memory distortion where a patient remembers an incorrect memory that they believe to be true. The use of cognitive strategies to improve memory performance among patients with AD by reducing false memories has taken on added importance given the lack of disease-modifying agents for AD. However, existing evidence suggests that cognitive strategies to reduce false memories in patients with AD are of limited effectiveness, although these strategies may be useful at earlier stages of the disease. The purpose of this review is to examine experimental findings of false memories and associated memory processes in patients with mild cognitive impairment due to AD and mild AD dementia. Cognitive strategies to reduce false memories in these patient populations are also reviewed. Approaches to clinically relevant future research are suggested and discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Christopher Malone
- Center for Translational Cognitive Neuroscience, VA Boston Healthcare System, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Rebecca G. Deason
- Department of Psychology, Texas State University, San Marcos, TX, USA
| | - Rocco Palumbo
- Center for Translational Cognitive Neuroscience, VA Boston Healthcare System, Boston, MA, USA
- Department of Neurology, Boston University Alzheimer’s Disease Center, Boston University School of Medicine, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Nadine Heyworth
- Center for Translational Cognitive Neuroscience, VA Boston Healthcare System, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Michelle Tat
- Center for Translational Cognitive Neuroscience, VA Boston Healthcare System, Boston, MA, USA
- Department of Neurology, Boston University Alzheimer’s Disease Center, Boston University School of Medicine, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Andrew E. Budson
- Center for Translational Cognitive Neuroscience, VA Boston Healthcare System, Boston, MA, USA
- Department of Neurology, Boston University Alzheimer’s Disease Center, Boston University School of Medicine, Boston, MA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
El Haj M, Antoine P. Context Memory in Alzheimer's Disease: The "Who, Where, and When". Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2018; 33:158-167. [PMID: 28666337 DOI: 10.1093/arclin/acx062] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/20/2017] [Accepted: 06/10/2017] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Objective Context memory, a component of episodic system, refers to the ability to retrieve conditions under which an event has occurred, such as who was present during that event and where and when it occurred. Context memory has been found to be compromised in older adults, an issue that we investigated in Alzheimer's disease (AD). Methods Thirty-one participants with AD and 35 older adults were asked to generate three autobiographical events. Afterward, they were asked to remember the names of all people who were evoked during the events, and the names for any location that was mentioned during the events. Participants were also asked to remember the year, season, month and day of the week when the events occurred. Results Compared to older adults, participants with AD showed lower memory for "who" (p < .001), "where" (p < .05), and "when" (p < .01). Compared to "who" and "where", both participants with AD and older adults showed pronounced difficulties in remembering the "when". Conclusion these findings highlight difficulties in remembering temporal information as an indication of context memory decline in AD. The difficulties in retrieving temporal information are discussed in terms of timing failures and hippocampal degenerations in AD.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mohamad El Haj
- Univ. Lille, CNRS, CHU Lille, UMR 9193 - SCALab - Sciences Cognitives et Sciences Affectives, F-59000 Lille, France.,CHU de Lille, Unité de Psychogériatrie, Pôle de Gérontologie, 59037 Lille, France
| | - Pascal Antoine
- CHU de Lille, Unité de Psychogériatrie, Pôle de Gérontologie, 59037 Lille, France
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Abstract
Background Alzheimer's disease (AD) is a neurodegenerative disease characterized by a gradual loss of memory. Specifically, context aspects of memory are impaired in AD. Our review sheds light on the neurocognitive mechanisms of this memory component that forms the core of episodic memory function. Summary Context recall, an element of episodic memory, refers to remembering the context in which an event has occurred, such as from whom or to whom information has been transmitted. Key Messages Our review raises crucial questions. For example, (1) which context element is more prone to being forgotten in the disease? (2) How do AD patients fail to bind context features together? (3) May distinctiveness heuristic or decisions based on metacognitive expectations improve context retrieval in these patients? (4) How does cueing at retrieval enhance reinstating of encoding context in AD? By addressing these questions, our work contributes to the understanding of the memory deficits in AD.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mohamad El Haj
- Laboratoire Epsylon, EA 4556, Université Paul-Valery, Montpellier III, Montpellier, France ; Laboratoire de Psychologie des Pays de la Loire, L'Université Nantes Angers Le Mans (L'UNAM), Angers, France ; Neuropsychology and Auditory Cognition, Department of Psychology, University of Lille 3, Lille, France
| | - Roy P C Kessels
- Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Nijmegen, Nijmegen, The Netherlands ; Vincent van Gogh Institute for Psychiatry, Korsakoff Clinic, Venray, The Netherlands ; Department of Medical Psychology, Radboud University Nijmegen Medical Centre, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Using pictures and words to understand recognition memory deterioration in amnestic mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease: a review. Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep 2013; 12:687-94. [PMID: 22927024 DOI: 10.1007/s11910-012-0310-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 32] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/20/2022]
Abstract
Difficulty recognizing previously encountered stimuli is one of the earliest signs of incipient Alzheimer's disease (AD). Work over the last 10 years has focused on how patients with AD and those in the prodromal stage of amnestic mild cognitive impairment make recognition decisions for visual and verbal stimuli. Interestingly, both groups of patients demonstrate markedly better memory for pictures over words, to a degree that is significantly greater in magnitude than their healthy older counterparts. Understanding this phenomenon not only helps to conceptualize how memory breaks down in AD, but also potentially provides the basis for future interventions. This review critically examines recent recognition memory work using pictures and words in the context of the dual-process theory of recognition and current hypotheses of cognitive breakdown in the course of very early AD.
Collapse
|
13
|
Simmons-Stern NR, Deason RG, Brandler BJ, Frustace BS, O’Connor MK, Ally BA, Budson AE. Music-based memory enhancement in Alzheimer's disease: promise and limitations. Neuropsychologia 2012; 50:3295-303. [PMID: 23000133 PMCID: PMC3567773 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.09.019] [Citation(s) in RCA: 40] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/31/2012] [Revised: 08/16/2012] [Accepted: 09/12/2012] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
In a previous study (Simmons-Stern, Budson & Ally, 2010), we found that patients with Alzheimer's disease (AD) better recognized visually presented lyrics when the lyrics were also sung rather than spoken at encoding. The present study sought to further investigate the effects of music on memory in patients with AD by making the content of the song lyrics relevant for the daily life of an older adult and by examining how musical encoding alters several different aspects of episodic memory. Patients with AD and healthy older adults studied visually presented novel song lyrics related to instrumental activities of daily living (IADL) that were accompanied by either a sung or a spoken recording. Overall, participants performed better on a memory test of general lyric content for lyrics that were studied sung as compared to spoken. However, on a memory test of specific lyric content, participants performed equally well for sung and spoken lyrics. We interpret these results in terms of a dual-process model of recognition memory such that the general content questions represent a familiarity-based representation that is preferentially sensitive to enhancement via music, while the specific content questions represent a recollection-based representation unaided by musical encoding. Additionally, in a test of basic recognition memory for the audio stimuli, patients with AD demonstrated equal discrimination for sung and spoken stimuli. We propose that the perceptual distinctiveness of musical stimuli enhanced metamemorial awareness in AD patients via a non-selective distinctiveness heuristic, thereby reducing false recognition while at the same time reducing true recognition and eliminating the mnemonic benefit of music. These results are discussed in the context of potential music-based memory enhancement interventions for the care of patients with AD.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nicholas R. Simmons-Stern
- Center for Translational Cognitive Neuroscience, VA Boston Healthcare System, Boston, MA, United States
- Boston University Alzheimer’s Disease Center, Department of Neurology, Boston University School of Medicine, Boston, MA, United States
| | - Rebecca G. Deason
- Center for Translational Cognitive Neuroscience, VA Boston Healthcare System, Boston, MA, United States
- Boston University Alzheimer’s Disease Center, Department of Neurology, Boston University School of Medicine, Boston, MA, United States
| | - Brian J. Brandler
- Center for Translational Cognitive Neuroscience, VA Boston Healthcare System, Boston, MA, United States
- Boston University Alzheimer’s Disease Center, Department of Neurology, Boston University School of Medicine, Boston, MA, United States
| | - Bruno S. Frustace
- Center for Translational Cognitive Neuroscience, VA Boston Healthcare System, Boston, MA, United States
- Boston University Alzheimer’s Disease Center, Department of Neurology, Boston University School of Medicine, Boston, MA, United States
| | - Maureen K. O’Connor
- Center for Translational Cognitive Neuroscience, VA Boston Healthcare System, Boston, MA, United States
- Boston University Alzheimer’s Disease Center, Department of Neurology, Boston University School of Medicine, Boston, MA, United States
- Department of Psychology, Bedford Veterans Administration Hospital, Bedford, MA, United States
| | - Brandon A. Ally
- Departments of Neurology, Psychology, & Psychiatry, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN, United States
| | - Andrew E. Budson
- Center for Translational Cognitive Neuroscience, VA Boston Healthcare System, Boston, MA, United States
- Boston University Alzheimer’s Disease Center, Department of Neurology, Boston University School of Medicine, Boston, MA, United States
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Stark AC, Budson AE. Managing memory impairment in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Neurodegener Dis Manag 2012. [DOI: 10.2217/nmt.12.53] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
Abstract
SUMMARY Memory impairment in patients with Alzheimer’s disease (AD) may be treated with cholinesterase inhibitors. Attention, alertness, initiative and memory (indirectly) may be treated with memantine. In addition to pharmacologic treatments, nonpharmacologic treatments, including external systems and devices, participation in social activities and exercise, as well as the use of habits, pictures and strategies, improve memory. We believe that although memory loss cannot be halted, both pharmacologic and nonpharmacologic treatments may help to improve AD patients’ memory and allow them to continue independent activities. Patients with mild cognitive impairment due to AD may be treated with nonpharmacologic therapies and with off-label cholinesterase inhibitors.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Aleksandra C Stark
- VA Boston Healthcare System, Department of Neurology, Boston University School of Medicine, Alzheimer’s Disease Center, Boston, MA, USA
| | - Andrew E Budson
- VA Boston Healthcare System, Department of Neurology, Boston University School of Medicine, Alzheimer’s Disease Center, Boston, MA, USA
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
El Haj M, Fasotti L, Allain P. Source monitoring in Alzheimer's disease. Brain Cogn 2012; 80:185-91. [PMID: 22885691 DOI: 10.1016/j.bandc.2012.06.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 44] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/05/2012] [Revised: 06/24/2012] [Accepted: 06/30/2012] [Indexed: 10/28/2022]
Abstract
Source monitoring is the process of making judgments about the origin of memories. There are three categories of source monitoring: reality monitoring (discrimination between self- versus other-generated sources), external monitoring (discrimination between several external sources), and internal monitoring (discrimination between two types of self-generated sources). We investigated whether Alzheimer's Disease (AD) patients, when compared with young and older adults, are impaired at the same level on the three source monitoring categories. We designed three tasks, one for each source monitoring category. In the first task, aimed at reality monitoring, participants had to remember whether objects were previously placed in a bag by themselves or by the experimenter. In the second task, assessing external monitoring, participants had to remember whether the experimenter had previously placed objects in the bag with a black or white gloved hand. In the third task, measuring internal monitoring, participants had to remember whether they had previously placed or imagined themselves placing objects in the bag. Participants showed worse performances in the external and internal monitoring tasks, when compared with reality monitoring. The external monitoring deficit was even more pronounced in AD patients. Regression analyses showed that variation in the external monitoring performances was reliably predicted by inhibition. Our results emphasize the role of inhibitory processes in AD patients' source monitoring decline. The close relation between source and inhibitory decline in AD is interpreted in terms of a common neural base for both concepts.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mohamad El Haj
- LUNAM Université, Université d'Angers, Laboratoire de Psychologie des Pays de la Loire, France.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
16
|
MacDuffie KE, Atkins AS, Flegal KE, Clark CM, Reuter-Lorenz PA. Memory distortion in Alzheimer's disease: deficient monitoring of short- and long-term memory. Neuropsychology 2012; 26:509-16. [PMID: 22746309 PMCID: PMC3389800 DOI: 10.1037/a0028684] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022] Open
Abstract
OBJECTIVE This study measured distortions of memory during short-term memory (STM) and long-term memory (LTM) versions of a semantically associated word list learning paradigm. Performance of patients with mild-to-moderate Alzheimer's disease (AD; MMSE ≥16) was compared with performance of age-matched, healthy older adult participants. METHOD In a STM version of the Deese-Roediger-McDermott (DRM) task, participants viewed four-word lists and were prompted for recall after a brief interval. The LTM task tested recall memory for 12-word lists. RESULTS Compared with the healthy group, the AD participants show greater impairment on the LTM task than on the STM task, although veridical recall is significantly reduced on both tasks. Furthermore, on both memory tasks, (1) participants with AD generate more nonsemantic intrusions than healthy older adult participants, and (2) semantic intrusion rate, when computed as a proportion of total recall, does not differ between groups. Notably, nonsemantic intrusions are consistently high for AD participants across both STM and LTM despite a marked difference in recall accuracy (65% and 23%, respectively). CONCLUSIONS STM impairment with some preserved semantic processing is evident in AD. The extent and variety of intrusions reported by AD participants indicates a breakdown in their ability to monitor and constrain their recall responses, even within seconds of initial learning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Katherine E MacDuffie
- Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Duke University, 417 Chapel Drive, Box 90086, Durham, NC 27708, USA.
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
17
|
Henkel LA, Mattson ME. Reading is believing: The truth effect and source credibility. Conscious Cogn 2011; 20:1705-21. [DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2011.08.018] [Citation(s) in RCA: 43] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/08/2011] [Revised: 08/01/2011] [Accepted: 08/26/2011] [Indexed: 10/17/2022]
|
18
|
Alzheimer's disease and memory-monitoring impairment: Alzheimer's patients show a monitoring deficit that is greater than their accuracy deficit. Neuropsychologia 2011; 49:2609-18. [PMID: 21620877 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.05.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 23] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/17/2011] [Revised: 05/10/2011] [Accepted: 05/12/2011] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
Abstract
We assessed the ability of two groups of patients with mild Alzheimer's disease (AD) and two groups of older adults to monitor the likely accuracy of recognition judgments and source identification judgments about who spoke something earlier. Alzheimer's patients showed worse performance on both memory judgments and were less able to monitor with confidence ratings the likely accuracy of both kinds of memory judgments, as compared to a group of older adults who experienced the identical study and test conditions. Critically, however, when memory performance was made comparable between the AD patients and the older adults (e.g., by giving AD patients extra exposures to the study materials), AD patients were still greatly impaired at monitoring the likely accuracy of their recognition and source judgments. This result indicates that the monitoring impairment in AD patients is actually worse than their memory impairment, as otherwise there would have been no differences between the two groups in monitoring performance when there were no differences in accuracy. We discuss the brain correlates of this memory-monitoring deficit and also propose a Remembrance-Evaluation model of memory-monitoring.
Collapse
|
19
|
Silverberg NB, Ryan LM, Carrillo MC, Sperling R, Petersen RC, Posner HB, Snyder PJ, Hilsabeck R, Gallagher M, Raber J, Rizzo A, Possin K, King J, Kaye J, Ott BR, Albert MS, Wagster MV, Schinka JA, Cullum CM, Farias ST, Balota D, Rao S, Loewenstein D, Budson AE, Brandt J, Manly JJ, Barnes L, Strutt A, Gollan TH, Ganguli M, Babcock D, Litvan I, Kramer JH, Ferman TJ. Assessment of cognition in early dementia. Alzheimers Dement 2011; 7:e60-e76. [PMID: 23559893 PMCID: PMC3613863 DOI: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.05.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 55] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/21/2022]
Abstract
Better tools for assessing cognitive impairment in the early stages of Alzheimer's disease (AD) are required to enable diagnosis of the disease before substantial neurodegeneration has taken place and to allow detection of subtle changes in the early stages of progression of the disease. The National Institute on Aging and the Alzheimer's Association convened a meeting to discuss state of the art methods for cognitive assessment, including computerized batteries, as well as new approaches in the pipeline. Speakers described research using novel tests of object recognition, spatial navigation, attentional control, semantic memory, semantic interference, prospective memory, false memory and executive function as among the tools that could provide earlier identification of individuals with AD. In addition to early detection, there is a need for assessments that reflect real-world situations in order to better assess functional disability. It is especially important to develop assessment tools that are useful in ethnically, culturally and linguistically diverse populations as well as in individuals with neurodegenerative disease other than AD.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nina B. Silverberg
- Division of Neuroscience, National Institute on Aging, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda MD
| | - Laurie M. Ryan
- Division of Neuroscience, National Institute on Aging, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda MD
| | | | - Reisa Sperling
- Center for Alzheimer Research and Treatment, Department of Neurology, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA
| | | | | | - Peter J. Snyder
- Rhode Island Hospital and Department of Neurology, Alpert Medical School of Brown University, Providence, RI
| | - Robin Hilsabeck
- Department of Psychiatry, University of Texas Health Science Center at San Antonio and Psychology Service, South Texas Veterans Health Care System
| | - Michela Gallagher
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD
| | - Jacob Raber
- Department of Behavioral Neuroscience and Neurology and Division of Neuroscience, ONPRC, Oregon Health and Science University
| | - Albert Rizzo
- University of Southern California, Institute for Creative Technologies, Playa Vista, CA
| | - Katherine Possin
- Department of Neurology, University of California, San Francisco, CA
| | - Jonathan King
- Division of Behavioral and Social Research, National Institute on Aging, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda MD
| | - Jeffrey Kaye
- Departments of Neurology and Biomedical Engineering, Layton Aging & Alzheimer’s Disease Center, Oregon Center for Aging & Technology, Oregon Health & Science University and Portland Veteran’s Affairs Medical Center, Portland, OR
| | - Brian R. Ott
- Department of Cognitive, Linguistic and Psychological Sciences, Brown University, Street, Providence, RI
| | - Marilyn S. Albert
- Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD
| | - Molly V. Wagster
- Division of Neuroscience, National Institute on Aging, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda MD
| | - John A. Schinka
- James A. Haley VA Medical Center and Department of Psychiatry, University of South Florida, Tampa, FL
| | - C. Munro Cullum
- University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center at Dallas, TX
| | - Sarah T. Farias
- Department of Neurology, University of California, Davis, Sacramento, California
| | - David Balota
- Department of Psychology, Washington University, St. Louis, Missouri
| | - Stephen Rao
- Schey Center for Cognitive Neuroimaging, Neurological Institute, Cleveland Clinic, Cleveland, OH
| | - David Loewenstein
- Wien Center for Alzheimer's Disease and Memory Disorders, Mount Sinai Medical Center, Miami Beach, FL
| | - Andrew E. Budson
- Department of Neurology, Boston University Alzheimer’s Disease Center, Boston University School of Medicine, Bedford, MA and Center for Translational Cognitive Neuroscience, Geriatric Research Education Clinical Center, Edith Nourse Rogers Memorial Veterans Hospital, Bedford, MA
| | - Jason Brandt
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, The Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD
| | - Jennifer J. Manly
- Taub Institute for Research on Alzheimer’s Disease and the Aging Brain, Columbia University Medical Center, New York, NY
| | - Lisa Barnes
- Rush Alzheimer’s Disease Center, Department of Neurological Sciences, and Department of Behavioral Sciences, 4Rush Institute for Healthy Aging, Rush University Medical Center, Chicago, IL
| | | | - Tamar H. Gollan
- Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, CA
| | - Mary Ganguli
- Departments of Psychiatry and Neurology, School of Medicine, University of Pittsburgh; Department of Epidemiology, Graduate School of Public Health, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA
| | - Debra Babcock
- National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD
| | - Irene Litvan
- Division of Movement Disorders, Department of Neurology, University of Louisville, Louisville, KY
| | - Joel H. Kramer
- Department of Neurology, University of California, San Francisco, CA
| | - Tanis J. Ferman
- Department of Psychiatry and Psychology, Mayo Clinic, College of Medicine, Jacksonville, FL
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Zhang F, Geng H. What can false memory tell us about memory impairments in Alzheimer’s disease? CHINESE SCIENCE BULLETIN-CHINESE 2010. [DOI: 10.1007/s11434-010-4164-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
|
21
|
O'Connor MK, Ally BA. Using stimulus form change to understand memorial familiarity for pictures and words in patients with mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychologia 2010; 48:2068-74. [PMID: 20362596 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.03.027] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/06/2010] [Revised: 03/19/2010] [Accepted: 03/25/2010] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
Although it is generally accepted that patients with amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI) and patients with Alzheimer's disease (AD) have significantly impaired recollection, recent evidence has been mixed as to whether these patients demonstrate impaired memorial familiarity. Recent work suggests that familiarity may remain intact for pictures, but not for words. Further, a recent event-related potential (ERP) study suggests that enhanced conceptual processing of pictures may underlie this intact familiarity. However, to date there has been no direct comparison of perceptual and conceptual-based familiarity for pictures and words in patients with aMCI and AD. To investigate this issue, patients with aMCI, patients with AD, and healthy older adults underwent four study-test conditions of word-word, picture-picture, word-picture, and picture-word. When stimuli undergo form change, it has been suggested that only conceptual processing can help support recognition in the absence of recollection. Our results showed that patients successfully relied on perceptual and conceptual-based familiarity to improve recognition for the within format conditions over the across format conditions. Further, results suggested that patients with aMCI and AD are able to use enhanced conceptual processing of pictures compared to words to allow them to overcome the deleterious effects of form change in a similar manner as controls. These results help us begin to understand which aspects of memory are impaired and which remain relatively intact in patients with aMCI and AD. This understanding can then in turn help us to assess, conceptualize, and build behavioral interventions to help treat these patients.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Maureen K O'Connor
- Center for Translational Cognitive Neuroscience, Geriatric Research Education Clinical Center, Bedford VA Hospital, Bedford, MA 01730, United States
| | | |
Collapse
|
22
|
Dechêne A, Stahl C, Hansen J, Wänke M. The Truth About the Truth: A Meta-Analytic Review of the Truth Effect. PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY REVIEW 2009; 14:238-57. [DOI: 10.1177/1088868309352251] [Citation(s) in RCA: 228] [Impact Index Per Article: 14.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
Repetition has been shown to increase subjective truth ratings of trivia statements. This truth effect can be measured in two ways: (a) as the increase in subjective truth from the first to the second encounter ( within-items criterion) and (b) as the difference in truth ratings between repeated and other new statements ( between-items criterion). Qualitative differences are assumed between the processes underlying both criteria. A meta-analysis of the truth effect was conducted that compared the two criteria. In all, 51 studies of the repetition-induced truth effect were included in the analysis. Results indicate that the between-items effect is larger than the within-items effect. Moderator analyses reveal that several moderators affect both effects differentially. This lends support to the notion that different psychological comparison processes may underlie the two effects. The results are discussed within the processing fluency account of the truth effect.
Collapse
|
23
|
Algarabel S, Escudero J, Mazón JF, Pitarque A, Fuentes M, Peset V, Lacruz L. Familiarity-based recognition in the young, healthy elderly, mild cognitive impaired and Alzheimer's patients☆. Neuropsychologia 2009; 47:2056-64. [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.03.016] [Citation(s) in RCA: 37] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/14/2008] [Revised: 03/02/2009] [Accepted: 03/19/2009] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
|
24
|
The occurrence of a deficit in contextual fear extinction in adult amyloid-over-expressing TASTPM mice is independent of the strength of conditioning but can be prevented by mild novel cage stress. Behav Brain Res 2009; 200:83-90. [PMID: 19162086 DOI: 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.12.037] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/10/2008] [Accepted: 12/23/2008] [Indexed: 11/20/2022]
Abstract
In the amyloid over-expressing TASTPM mouse model of Alzheimer's disease, impaired contextual fear memory occurs early, and is preceded, at 4 months of age, by a deficit in extinction of contextual fear that is resistant to improvement by repeated mild novel cage stress. The first aim of this study was thus to establish whether the extinction deficit could be prevented if the novel cage procedure was applied prior to its onset. The second aim was to establish whether the occurrence of the extinction deficit was dependent on the robustness of the conditioning protocol. We first compared 3-month-old wild-type and TASTPM mice for acquisition, retention and extinction of contextual fear and then, looked at the impact of 5 weeks of novel cage stress (4 x 1 h/week) applied from 3 months onwards, on age-related changes in these behaviours evaluated at 4.5 months of age. In another experiment, we compared 4-month-old TASTPM and wild-type mice for the impact of a 2 and 5-pairing conditioning procedure on the three phases of contextual fear conditioning. In 4.5-month-old TASTPM mice, the deficit in extinction was alleviated by repeated novel cage stress, applied from prior to its onset at 3 months. At 4 months of age, the occurrence of an extinction deficit was independent of the strength of the conditioning procedure, in TASTPM mice, which even showed an increase in aversive memory under the 2-pairing condition. The robust early impairment in the extinction of contextual fear seen in adult TASTPM mice suggests that a deficit in cognitive flexibility is the first sign of behavioural pathology in this model of Alzheimer's disease.
Collapse
|
25
|
Budson AE, Mather M, Chong H. Memory for choices in Alzheimer's disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2006; 22:150-8. [PMID: 16770076 DOI: 10.1159/000093839] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 02/14/2006] [Indexed: 11/19/2022] Open
Abstract
Despite their cognitive impairment, patients with mild Alzheimer's disease (AD) often make important life choices. When making choices, people frequently attempt to directly compare the features of different options, rather than evaluating each option separately. Not every feature has an analogous (or alignable) feature in the other option, however. In 2005, Mather's group found that both younger and older adults filled in such gaps when remembering, creating features in the other option to contrast with existing features. In the present study, such effects of alignability on recognition memory were not found in patients with mild AD. This finding suggests that patients with mild AD are less likely to engage in feature-by-feature comparison processes across choice options, a change that may lead them to make qualitatively different choices than healthy older adults.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Andrew E Budson
- Geriatric Research Education Clinical Center, Edith Nourse Rogers Memorial Veterans Hospital, Bedford, MA 01730, USA.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|