1
|
Perea-García JO, Teuben A, Caspar KR. Look past the cooperative eye hypothesis: reconsidering the evolution of human eye appearance. Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc 2025. [PMID: 40366110 DOI: 10.1111/brv.70033] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/03/2024] [Revised: 04/24/2025] [Accepted: 04/28/2025] [Indexed: 05/15/2025]
Abstract
The external appearance of the human eye has been prominently linked to the evolution of complex sociocognitive functions in our species. The cooperative eye hypothesis (CEH) proposes that human eyeballs, with their weakly expressed conjunctival and scleral pigmentation, are uniquely conspicuous and evolved under selective pressures to behave cooperatively, therefore signalling attentiveness to conspecifics. Non-human primates are instead assumed to display less-salient eye morphologies that help mask their gaze to facilitate competitive, rather than cooperative actions. Here, we argue that the CEH, although continuing to be influential, lacks robust empirical support. Over the past two decades, multidisciplinary research has undermined its original rationale and central premises: human eye pigmentation does not uniquely stand out among primates, it is not uniform at species level and the available evidence does not conclusively suggest that it facilitates gaze following to notable extents. Hence, the CEH currently provides a theoretical framework that risks confusing, rather than informing, inferences about the evolution of human external eye appearance and its selective drivers. In a call to move past it, we review alternative hypotheses with the potential to elucidate the emergence of the human ocular phenotype from the considerable spectrum of diversity found within the primate order.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Juan Olvido Perea-García
- Center for Language Evolution Studies, Nicolaus Copernicus University in Toruń, Fosa Staromiejska 3, Toruń, 87-100, Poland
- University Institute for Health and Biomedical Research (IUIBS), Universidad Las Palmas de Gran Canaria, Las Palmas, Spain
| | - Aurora Teuben
- University of Amsterdam, Science Park 904, Amsterdam, 1098 XH, The Netherlands
| | - Kai R Caspar
- Institute of Cell Biology, Heinrich Heine University Düsseldorf, Universitätsstr. 1, Düsseldorf, D-40225, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Linke L, Krause LM, Horstmann G. Perceiving social signals: The similarity of direct gazing and pointing. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2025; 257:105057. [PMID: 40347737 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2025.105057] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/13/2024] [Revised: 04/29/2025] [Accepted: 04/30/2025] [Indexed: 05/14/2025] Open
Abstract
Gazing and pointing can have overlapping functions in social interaction. The perception of both social cues is remarkably precise. Nevertheless, the perception of direct gaze is characterized by an area of direct gaze rather than one single gaze direction. In fact, observers accept a range of gaze directions as direct. Here, we investigate whether there is an analogous area of direct pointing. Three experiments examine an area of direct pointing (about 5-9°) and compare it to the area of direct gaze. We find this area to be similar, but not equal, in shape and size. Furthermore, we examine the influence of different pointing gestures on the area of direct pointing. Results indicate a shift of the area of direct pointing dependent on the used limb (left or right). The results are discussed with respect to common underlying mechanisms of the perception of direct gaze and direct pointing.
Collapse
|
3
|
Shi J, Wu T, Chen L, Wang Z, Yan L, Sun YHP, Jin H. Eye configuration influences the detection advantage of direct gaze. Perception 2025:3010066251332097. [PMID: 40239185 DOI: 10.1177/03010066251332097] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 04/18/2025]
Abstract
Research has shown that individuals detect direct eyes (those directed to the observer) more quickly and accurately than averted eyes (those directed elsewhere), a phenomenon known as the direct-gaze advantage. However, the underlying mechanisms of this advantage remain poorly understood. The current study employed a visual search task to investigate whether the direct-gaze advantage in capturing attention is contingent upon a specific configuration (face configuration or eyes configuration). In two experiments, participants were presented with four images and asked to judge whether there was a target with a direct or averted gaze. Experiment 1 revealed that participants showed the direct-gaze advantage across three image types: intact faces (maintaining both two-eye and face configuration), scrambled faces with intact eyes (disrupting the face configuration while preserving the two-eye configuration), and fully scrambled faces (preserving only the single-eye configuration). Experiment 2 further demonstrated that participants showed the direct-gaze advantage for scrambled faces with intact eyes and fully scrambled faces under the upright condition. Interestingly, under the inverted condition, participants only showed the direct-gaze advantage for scrambled faces with intact eyes. These findings indicate that the direct-gaze advantage is influenced by the configuration of two eyes and the configuration of a single eye, but it is not dependent on facial contexts.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Ting Wu
- Zhejiang Sci-Tech University, China
| | - Lei Chen
- Zhejiang Sci-Tech University, China
| | - Zhe Wang
- Zhejiang Sci-Tech University, China
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
4
|
Kawabe T, Akiyama R, Yokosaka T. The influence of eye position on the animacy impression of a cube-shaped robot in motion. Iperception 2025; 16:20416695251323769. [PMID: 40129450 PMCID: PMC11931162 DOI: 10.1177/20416695251323769] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/17/2024] [Accepted: 02/09/2025] [Indexed: 03/26/2025] Open
Abstract
Human observers can sometimes attribute animacy or agency to non-living objects, such as robots, perceiving them as if they were alive. In particular, the movement pattern of non-living things is a key feature for perceiving life. It is also well known that the pattern of the eyes is also an important feature for the perception of the sense of life. The present study investigated how the animacy impression of a cube-shaped robot moving along the Perlin noise trajectory could be influenced by the visual patterns of the eyes, such as eye positions and gaze directions. The eyes were presented on the top surface of the cube-shaped robot. Participants were asked to rate animacy impressions of the robot. These impressions included the impression of a live animal, having intention and moving in a self-propelled manner. These impressions were consistently higher when the eyes were presented on the side of the robot's direction of motion than when they were presented on the side orthogonal to, or opposite to, the robot's direction of motion. In general, the animacy impressions were largely comparable regardless of whether the robot's gaze direction aligned with, was orthogonal to, or opposed its motion direction. However, the impression of intention was stronger when the gaze direction at the front side of the object was consistent with the motion direction than when it was inconsistent. We discuss the evolutionary role of eye position in determining animacy impressions.
Collapse
|
5
|
Horstmann G. The overestimation of gaze for horizontal, vertical, and diagonal fixation points. Perception 2025; 54:57-64. [PMID: 39497548 DOI: 10.1177/03010066241291646] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/15/2024]
Abstract
A widely known result from gaze-perception research is the overestimation effect where gaze endpoints are seen farther to the side than they actually are. While horizontal gaze directions have been studied repeatedly, there is scarce research on other directions after early reports that vertical gaze is perceived accurately. It is argued that if participants base their judgment on the movements of the iris-pupil-complex in relation to eye size, vertical gaze should be overestimated similarly as horizontal gaze. This is what was found in the reported experiment. However, horizontal gaze was actually overestimated more than diagonal and vertical gaze. The small difference in overestimation between the axes may be explained by the horizontal-vertical illusion, entailing that horizontal extensions are seen as shorter than vertical extensions.
Collapse
|
6
|
Kohda M, Sogawa S, Sowersby W. The ability of teleost fishes to recognize individual faces suggests an early evolutionary origin in vertebrates. Front Psychol 2024; 15:1497386. [PMID: 39664642 PMCID: PMC11632839 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1497386] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/16/2024] [Accepted: 11/05/2024] [Indexed: 12/13/2024] Open
Abstract
The face is the most important area on the human body for visually differentiating between individuals. When encountering another person, humans initially gaze at and perceive the face holistically, utilizing first-order relational information and specific neural systems. Information such as identity and emotional state are then obtained from the face by distinguishing between small inter-individual differences, i.e., second-order relational information. Similar patterns and mechanisms underlying individual face recognition have been documented in primates, other social mammals, birds, and more recently in some fishes. Like humans, fish are capable of rapidly (<0.5 s) and accurately recognizing multiple familiar conspecifics by individual-specific variation in the face. Fish can also recognize faces from various distances and angles, providing evidence for mental representation of faces in this large and diverse vertebrate group. One species, the cleaner fish, has even demonstrated mirror self-recognition (MSR) via self-face recognition, strengthening the claim that non-human animals are capable of having mental images and concepts of faces. Here, we review the evidence for individual face recognition in fishes and speculate that face identification neural networks are both similar and widespread across vertebrates. Furthermore, we hypothesize that first-and second-order face recognition in vertebrates originated in bony fishes in the Paleozoic era ~450 Mya, when social systems first evolved, increasing the importance of individual recognition.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Masanori Kohda
- Laboratory of Animal Sociology, Department of Biology, Graduate School of Sciences, Osaka Metropolitan University, Osaka, Japan
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
7
|
Whitham W, Karstadt B, Anderson NC, Bischof WF, Schapiro SJ, Kingstone A, Coss R, Birmingham E, Yorzinski JL. Predator gaze captures both human and chimpanzee attention. PLoS One 2024; 19:e0311673. [PMID: 39570943 PMCID: PMC11581262 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0311673] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/30/2023] [Accepted: 09/23/2024] [Indexed: 11/24/2024] Open
Abstract
Primates can rapidly detect potential predators and modify their behavior based on the level of risk. The gaze direction of predators is one feature that primates can use to assess risk levels: recognition of a predator's direct stare indicates to prey that it has been detected and the level of risk is relatively high. Predation has likely shaped visual attention in primates to quickly assess the level of risk but we know little about the constellation of low-level (e.g., contrast, color) and higher-order (e.g., category membership, perceived threat) visual features that primates use to do so. We therefore presented human and chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) participants with photographs of potential predators (lions) and prey (impala) while we recorded their overt attention with an eye-tracker. The gaze of the predators and prey was either directed or averted. We found that both humans and chimpanzees visually fixated the eyes of predators more than those of prey. In addition, they directed the most attention toward the eyes of directed (rather than averted) predators. Humans, but not chimpanzees, gazed at the eyes of the predators and prey more than other features. Importantly, low-level visual features of the predators and prey did not provide a good explanation of the observed gaze patterns.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Will Whitham
- Department of Ecology and Conservation Biology, Texas A&M University, College Station, Texas, United States of America
- Department of Comparative Medicine, UT MD Anderson Cancer Center, Bastrop, Texas, United States of America
| | - Bradley Karstadt
- Faculty of Education, Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, British Columbia, Canada
| | - Nicola C. Anderson
- Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada
| | - Walter F. Bischof
- Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada
| | - Steven J. Schapiro
- Department of Comparative Medicine, UT MD Anderson Cancer Center, Bastrop, Texas, United States of America
| | - Alan Kingstone
- Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada
| | - Richard Coss
- Department of Psychology, University of California Davis, Davis, California, United States of America
| | - Elina Birmingham
- Faculty of Education, Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, British Columbia, Canada
| | - Jessica L. Yorzinski
- Department of Ecology and Conservation Biology, Texas A&M University, College Station, Texas, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Heesen R, Szenteczki MA, Kim Y, Kret ME, Atkinson AP, Upton Z, Clay Z. Impact of social context on human facial and gestural emotion expressions. iScience 2024; 27:110663. [PMID: 39555406 PMCID: PMC11565554 DOI: 10.1016/j.isci.2024.110663] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/11/2023] [Revised: 07/09/2024] [Accepted: 07/31/2024] [Indexed: 11/19/2024] Open
Abstract
Humans flexibly adapt expressions of emotional messages when interacting with others. However, detailed information on how specific parts of the face and hands move in socio-emotional contexts is missing. We identified individual gesture and facial movements (through automated face tracking) of N = 80 participants in the UK, produced while watching amusing, fearful, or neutral movie scenes either alone or with a social partner. Amusing and fearful scenes, more so than neutral scenes, led to an overall increase in facial and gesture movements, confirming emotional responding. Furthermore, social context facilitated movements in the lower instead of upper facial areas, as well as gesture use. These findings highlight emotional signaling components that likely underwent selection for communication, a result we discuss in comparison with the nonhuman primate literature. To facilitate ecologically valid and cross-cultural comparisons on human emotion communication, we additionally offer a new stimuli database of the recorded naturalistic facial expressions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Mark A. Szenteczki
- Laboratory of Functional Ecology, Institute of Biology, University of Neuchâtel, Neuchâtel, Switzerland
| | - Yena Kim
- Institute of Psychology, Leiden University, Leiden, the Netherlands
| | - Mariska E. Kret
- Institute of Psychology, Leiden University, Leiden, the Netherlands
| | | | - Zoe Upton
- Department of Psychology, Durham University, Durham, UK
| | - Zanna Clay
- Department of Psychology, Durham University, Durham, UK
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Perea-García JO, Berris D, Tan J, Kret ME. Pupil size and iris brightness interact to affect prosocial behaviour and affective responses. Cogn Emot 2024:1-16. [PMID: 39540646 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2024.2427340] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/01/2024] [Revised: 10/25/2024] [Accepted: 11/04/2024] [Indexed: 11/16/2024]
Abstract
Despite the tight link between the visibility of the iris and pupil, the perceived effects of these two have been studied largely in isolation. We demonstrate, across two experimental studies, that the effects of perceived pupil size are dependent on the visibility of the iris. In a first study, our participants donated more and had more positive impressions of portraits of non-human primates when these were manipulated to appear having larger pupils. Post-hoc inspection of our data suggested that the difference was greater for species with more conspicuous irises. In a second study, we concomitantly manipulated iris brightness and pupil size. Brighter irises and larger pupils elicited greater donations. Participants rated photographs with brighter irises as cuter, more attractive and friendlier, but only when they had dilated pupils. Our results have methodological implications for studies manipulating eye appearance, and help interpret results from previous studies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Daisy Berris
- Institute of Psychology, Cognitive Psychology Unit, Leiden University, Leiden, The Netherlands
| | - Jingzhi Tan
- Institute of Psychology, Cognitive Psychology Unit, Leiden University, Leiden, The Netherlands
| | - Mariska E Kret
- Institute of Psychology, Cognitive Psychology Unit, Leiden University, Leiden, The Netherlands
- Leiden Institute for Brain and Cognition, Leiden, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Sato W, Shimokawa K, Uono S, Minato T. Mentalistic attention orienting triggered by android eyes. Sci Rep 2024; 14:23143. [PMID: 39367157 PMCID: PMC11452688 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-024-75063-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/31/2024] [Accepted: 10/01/2024] [Indexed: 10/06/2024] Open
Abstract
The eyes play a special role in human communications. Previous psychological studies have reported reflexive attention orienting in response to another individual's eyes during live interactions. Although robots are expected to collaborate with humans in various social situations, it remains unclear whether robot eyes have the potential to trigger attention orienting similarly to human eyes, specifically based on mental attribution. We investigated this issue in a series of experiments using a live gaze-cueing paradigm with an android. In Experiment 1, the non-predictive cue was the eyes and head of an android placed in front of human participants. Light-emitting diodes in the periphery served as target signals. The reaction times (RTs) required to localize the valid cued targets were faster than those for invalid cued targets for both types of cues. In Experiment 2, the gaze direction of the android eyes changed before the peripheral target lights appeared with or without barriers that made the targets non-visible, such that the android did not attend to them. The RTs were faster for validly cued targets only when there were no barriers. In Experiment 3, the targets were changed from lights to sounds, which the android could attend to even in the presence of barriers. The RTs to the target sounds were faster with valid cues, irrespective of the presence of barriers. These results suggest that android eyes may automatically induce attention orienting in humans based on mental state attribution.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Wataru Sato
- Psychological Process Research Team, Guardian Robot Project, RIKEN, 2-2-2 Hikaridai, Seika-cho, Soraku-gun, Kyoto, 619-0288, Japan.
| | - Koh Shimokawa
- Psychological Process Research Team, Guardian Robot Project, RIKEN, 2-2-2 Hikaridai, Seika-cho, Soraku-gun, Kyoto, 619-0288, Japan
| | - Shota Uono
- Division of Disability Sciences, Institute of Human Sciences, University of Tsukuba, 1-1-1 Tennodai, Tsukuba, 305-8572, Ibaraki, Japan
| | - Takashi Minato
- Interactive Robot Research Team, Guardian Robot Project, RIKEN, 2-2-2 Hikaridai, Seika-cho, Soraku-gun, Kyoto, 619-0288, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Mayrand F, McCrackin SD, Ristic J. Intentional looks facilitate faster responding in observers. COMMUNICATIONS PSYCHOLOGY 2024; 2:90. [PMID: 39349763 PMCID: PMC11442367 DOI: 10.1038/s44271-024-00137-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/14/2024] [Accepted: 09/10/2024] [Indexed: 10/04/2024]
Abstract
Humans construct rich representations of other people's mental states. Here we investigated how intentionality in eye gaze affected perception and responses to gaze. Observers viewed videos of human gazers looking left or right. Unbeknownst to the observers, the gazers could either choose where to look (self-chosen gaze) or were explicitly instructed where to look (computer-instructed gaze). In Experiment 1, observers reported the direction of the gazer's upcoming look before the eye movement was initiated. Faster responses were found for self-chosen relative to computer-instructed gaze. In Experiments 2 and 3, observers responded by reporting the location of a peripheral target that appeared at the gazed-at or not gazed-at location. Faster responses were found for gazed-at relative to not gazed-at targets and at longer cue-target intervals for self-chosen relative to computer-instructed gaze. The examination of the eye movement kinematics indicated that self-chosen gaze shifts were marked by a larger magnitude of motion within the eye region prior to the eye movement occurring relative to computer-instructed ones. Thus, perceived intentionality in eye gaze facilitates responses in observers with the information about mental states communicated via subtle properties of eye motion.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Florence Mayrand
- Department of Psychology, McGill University, Montreal, QC, H3A 1G1, Canada.
| | - Sarah D McCrackin
- Department of Psychology, McGill University, Montreal, QC, H3A 1G1, Canada
| | - Jelena Ristic
- Department of Psychology, McGill University, Montreal, QC, H3A 1G1, Canada.
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Renet C, Randall W, Guterstam A. A motion aftereffect from viewing other people's gaze. Front Hum Neurosci 2024; 18:1444428. [PMID: 39183816 PMCID: PMC11341350 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2024.1444428] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/05/2024] [Accepted: 07/26/2024] [Indexed: 08/27/2024] Open
Abstract
Recent work suggests that our brains may generate subtle, false motion signals streaming from other people to the objects of their attention, aiding social cognition. For instance, brief exposure to static images depicting other people gazing at objects made subjects slower at detecting subsequent motion in the direction of gaze, suggesting that looking at someone else's gaze caused a directional motion adaptation. Here we confirm, using a more stringent method, that viewing static images of another person gazing in a particular direction, at an object, produced motion aftereffects in the opposite direction. The aftereffect was manifested as a change in perceptual decision threshold for detecting left vs. right motion. The effect disappeared when the person was looking away from the object. These findings suggest that the attentive gaze of others is encoded as an implied agent-to-object motion that is sufficiently robust to cause genuine motion aftereffects, though subtle enough to remain subthreshold.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Christian Renet
- Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
| | - William Randall
- Department of Psychology, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ, United States
| | - Arvid Guterstam
- Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Van der Cruyssen I, Ben-Shakhar G, Pertzov Y, Guy N, Cabooter Q, Gunschera LJ, Verschuere B. The validation of online webcam-based eye-tracking: The replication of the cascade effect, the novelty preference, and the visual world paradigm. Behav Res Methods 2024; 56:4836-4849. [PMID: 37648844 PMCID: PMC11289066 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-023-02221-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 08/10/2023] [Indexed: 09/01/2023]
Abstract
The many benefits of online research and the recent emergence of open-source eye-tracking libraries have sparked an interest in transferring time-consuming and expensive eye-tracking studies from the lab to the web. In the current study, we validate online webcam-based eye-tracking by conceptually replicating three robust eye-tracking studies (the cascade effect, n = 134, the novelty preference, n = 45, and the visual world paradigm, n = 32) online using the participant's webcam as eye-tracker with the WebGazer.js library. We successfully replicated all three effects, although the effect sizes of all three studies shrank by 20-27%. The visual world paradigm was conducted both online and in the lab, using the same participants and a standard laboratory eye-tracker. The results showed that replication per se could not fully account for the effect size shrinkage, but that the shrinkage was also due to the use of online webcam-based eye-tracking, which is noisier. In conclusion, we argue that eye-tracking studies with relatively large effects that do not require extremely high precision (e.g., studies with four or fewer large regions of interest) can be done online using the participant's webcam. We also make recommendations for how the quality of online webcam-based eye-tracking could be improved.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ine Van der Cruyssen
- Department of Clinical Psychology, University of Amsterdam, Nieuwe Achtergracht 129B, 1018, VZ, Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
- Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Mount Scopus, 91905, Jerusalem, Israel.
| | | | - Yoni Pertzov
- Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Mount Scopus, 91905, Jerusalem, Israel
| | - Nitzan Guy
- Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Mount Scopus, 91905, Jerusalem, Israel
| | - Quinn Cabooter
- Department of Clinical Psychology, University of Amsterdam, Nieuwe Achtergracht 129B, 1018, VZ, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
- Ghent University, Henri Dunantlaan 2, 9000, Ghent, Belgium
| | - Lukas J Gunschera
- Department of Clinical Psychology, University of Amsterdam, Nieuwe Achtergracht 129B, 1018, VZ, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| | - Bruno Verschuere
- Department of Clinical Psychology, University of Amsterdam, Nieuwe Achtergracht 129B, 1018, VZ, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Duran E, Perea-García JO, Piepenbrock D, Veefkind C, Kret ME, Massen JJM. Preliminary evidence that eye appearance in parrots (Psittaciformes) co-varies with latitude and altitude. Sci Rep 2024; 14:12859. [PMID: 38834673 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-024-63599-3] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/17/2023] [Accepted: 05/30/2024] [Indexed: 06/06/2024] Open
Abstract
External eye appearance in avian taxa has been proposed to be driven by social and ecological functions. Recent research in primates suggests, instead, that, photoprotective functions are important drivers of external eye appearance. Using similar methods, we examined the variation in external eye appearance of 132 parrot species (Psittaciformes) in relation to their ecology and sociality. Breeding systems, flock size and sexual dimorphism, as well as species' latitude and maximum living altitude, and estimated UV-B incidence in species' ranges were used to explore the contribution of social and ecological factors in driving external eye appearance. We measured the hue and brightness of visible parts of the eye and the difference in measurements of brightness between adjacent parts of the eye. We found no link between social variables and our measurements. We did, however, find a negative association between the brightness of the inner part of the iris and latitude and altitude. Darker inner irises were more prevalent farther away from the equator and for those species living at higher altitudes. We found no link between UV-B and brightness measurements of the iris, or tissue surrounding the eye. We speculate that these results are consistent with an adaptation for visual functions. While preliminary, these results suggest that external eye appearance in parrots is influenced by ecological, but not social factors.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Elif Duran
- Department of Psychology, Izmir University of Economics, Izmir, Turkey
| | - Juan Olvido Perea-García
- Institute of Psychology, Cognitive Psychology Unit, Leiden University, Wassenaarseweg 52, Leiden, 2333 AK, The Netherlands.
| | - Diede Piepenbrock
- Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Department of Biology, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands
| | - Celine Veefkind
- Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Department of Biology, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands
| | - Mariska E Kret
- Institute of Psychology, Cognitive Psychology Unit, Leiden University, Wassenaarseweg 52, Leiden, 2333 AK, The Netherlands
- Leiden Institute of Brain and Cognition (LIBC), Leiden, The Netherlands
| | - Jorg J M Massen
- Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Department of Biology, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands
- Vogelpark Avifauna, Alphen Aan Den Rijn, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
Kawagoe T, Teramoto W. The center of a face catches the eye in face perception. Exp Brain Res 2024; 242:1339-1348. [PMID: 38563980 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-024-06822-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/26/2023] [Accepted: 03/17/2024] [Indexed: 04/04/2024]
Abstract
Using the "Don't look" (DL) paradigm, wherein participants are asked not to look at a specific feature (i.e., eye, nose, and mouth), we previously documented that Easterners struggled to completely avoid fixating on the eyes and nose. Their underlying mechanisms for attractiveness may differ because the fixations on the eyes were triggered only reflexively, whereas fixations on the nose were consistently elicited. In this study, we predominantly focused on the nose, where the center-of-gravity (CoG) effect, which refers to a person's tendency to look near an object's CoG, could be confounded. Full-frontal and mid-profile faces were used because the latter's CoG did not correspond to the nose location. Although we hypothesized that these two effects are independent, the results indicated that, in addition to the successful tracing of previous studies, the CoG effect explains the nose-attracting effect. This study not only reveals this explanation but also raises a question regarding the CoG effect on Eastern participants.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Toshikazu Kawagoe
- School of Humanities and Science, Tokai University, Kumamoto Campus, Toroku 9- 1-1, Kumamoto City, Kumamoto, 862-8652, Japan.
| | - Wataru Teramoto
- Faculty of Humanities and Social Sciences, Kumamoto University, Kurokami 2-40-1, Kumamoto City, 860-8555, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Williams EH, Chakrabarti B. The integration of head and body cues during the perception of social interactions. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 2024; 77:776-788. [PMID: 37232389 PMCID: PMC10960325 DOI: 10.1177/17470218231181001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/21/2022] [Revised: 03/10/2023] [Accepted: 05/10/2023] [Indexed: 05/27/2023]
Abstract
Humans spend a large proportion of time participating in social interactions. The ability to accurately detect and respond to human interactions is vital for social functioning, from early childhood through to older adulthood. This detection ability arguably relies on integrating sensory information from the interactants. Within the visual modality, directional information from a person's eyes, head, and body are integrated to inform where another person is looking and who they are interacting with. To date, social cue integration research has focused largely on the perception of isolated individuals. Across two experiments, we investigated whether observers integrate body information with head information when determining whether two people are interacting, and manipulated frame of reference (one of the interactants facing observer vs. facing away from observer) and the eye-region visibility of the interactant. Results demonstrate that individuals integrate information from the body with head information when perceiving dyadic interactions, and that integration is influenced by the frame of reference and visibility of the eye-region. Interestingly, self-reported autistics traits were associated with a stronger influence of body information on interaction perception, but only when the eye-region was visible. This study investigated the recognition of dyadic interactions using whole-body stimuli while manipulating eye visibility and frame of reference, and provides crucial insights into social cue integration, as well as how autistic traits affect cue integration, during perception of social interactions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Elin H Williams
- Centre for Autism, School of Psychology and Clinical Language Sciences, University of Reading, Reading, UK
| | - Bhismadev Chakrabarti
- Centre for Autism, School of Psychology and Clinical Language Sciences, University of Reading, Reading, UK
- India Autism Centre, Kolkata, India
- Department of Psychology, Ashoka University, Sonipat, India
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Hoffmann A, Schiestl S, Sinske P, Gondan M, Sachse P, Maran T. Sharing and Receiving Eye-Contact Predicts Mate Choice After a 5-Minute Conversation: Evidence from a Speed-Dating Study. ARCHIVES OF SEXUAL BEHAVIOR 2024; 53:959-968. [PMID: 38379110 PMCID: PMC10920202 DOI: 10.1007/s10508-023-02806-0] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/23/2023] [Revised: 12/29/2023] [Accepted: 12/30/2023] [Indexed: 02/22/2024]
Abstract
In popular narratives, the first date with a potential mate often centers on their gaze as embodiment of interest and attraction. However, evidence is still lacking on the role of eye-contact as a potent signal in human social interaction in the context of dating. In addition, behavioral mechanisms of mate selection are not well understood. In the present study, we therefore examined mutual eye-contact and its influence on mate choice by applying dual mobile eye-tracking during naturalistic speed-dates. A total of 30 male and 30 female subjects attended four speed-dates each (N = 240). Subjects were more likely to choose those dating partners with whom they shared more eye-contact with. In addition, perceived attractiveness played an important role for mate choice. Interestingly, receiving but not giving eye-contact also predicted individual mate choice. Eye-contact thus acts as an important signal of romantic attraction when encountering a dating partner.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alexandra Hoffmann
- Department of Psychology, Institute of Psychology, Universität Innsbruck, Universitätsstraße 5-7, 6020, Innsbruck, Austria.
| | - Sabrina Schiestl
- Department of Psychology, Institute of Psychology, Universität Innsbruck, Universitätsstraße 5-7, 6020, Innsbruck, Austria
| | - Philipp Sinske
- Department of Psychology, Institute of Psychology, Universität Innsbruck, Universitätsstraße 5-7, 6020, Innsbruck, Austria
| | - Matthias Gondan
- Department of Psychology, Institute of Psychology, Universität Innsbruck, Universitätsstraße 5-7, 6020, Innsbruck, Austria
| | - Pierre Sachse
- Department of Psychology, Institute of Psychology, Universität Innsbruck, Universitätsstraße 5-7, 6020, Innsbruck, Austria
| | - Thomas Maran
- Entrepreneurship, Innovation and Management, Free University of Bozen, Bozen, Italy
- LeadershipWerk, Vaduz, Liechtenstein
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Ishikawa K, Oyama T, Tanaka Y, Okubo M. Perceiving social gaze produces the reversed congruency effect. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 2024:17470218241232981. [PMID: 38320865 DOI: 10.1177/17470218241232981] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/08/2024]
Abstract
Numerous studies have shown that the gaze of others produces a special attentional process, such as the eye contact effect or joint attention. This study investigated the attentional process triggered by various types of gaze stimuli (i.e., human, cat, fish, koala, and robot gaze). A total of 300 university students participated in five experiments. They performed a spatial Stroop task in which five types of gaze stimuli were presented as targets. Participants were asked to judge the direction of the target (left or right) irrespective of its location (left or right). The results showed that the social gaze targets (i.e., human and cat gaze) produced a reversed congruency effect. In contrast to the social gaze targets, the non-social gaze (i.e., fish and robot) target did not produce the reversed congruency effect (Experiments 2, 2B, 3, and 4). These results suggest that attention to the gaze of socially communicable beings (i.e., humans and cats) is responsible for the reversed congruency effect. Our findings support the notion that the theory of mind or social interaction plays an important role in producing specific attentional processes in response to gaze stimuli.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kenta Ishikawa
- Department of Psychology, Senshu University, Kawasaki, Japan
| | - Takato Oyama
- Graduate School of Humanities, Senshu University, Kawasaki, Japan
| | - Yoshihiko Tanaka
- Graduate School of Humanities, Senshu University, Kawasaki, Japan
| | - Matia Okubo
- Department of Psychology, Senshu University, Kawasaki, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Toutain M, Dollion N, Henry L, Grandgeorge M. How Do Children and Adolescents with ASD Look at Animals? A Scoping Review. CHILDREN (BASEL, SWITZERLAND) 2024; 11:211. [PMID: 38397322 PMCID: PMC10887101 DOI: 10.3390/children11020211] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/04/2023] [Revised: 01/05/2024] [Accepted: 02/04/2024] [Indexed: 02/25/2024]
Abstract
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is characterized by interaction and communication differences, entailing visual attention skill specificities. Interactions with animals, such as in animal-assisted interventions or with service dogs, have been shown to be beneficial for individuals with ASD. While interacting with humans poses challenges for them, engaging with animals appears to be different. One hypothesis suggests that differences between individuals with ASD's visual attention to humans and to animals may contribute to these interaction differences. We propose a scoping review of the research on the visual attention to animals of youths with ASD. The objective is to review the methodologies and tools used to explore such questions, to summarize the main results, to explore which factors may contribute to the differences reported in the studies, and to deduce how youth with ASD observe animals. Utilizing strict inclusion criteria, we examined databases between 1942 and 2023, identifying 21 studies in international peer-reviewed journals. Three main themes were identified: attentional engagement and detection, visual exploration, and behavior. Collectively, our findings suggest that the visual attention of youths with ASD towards animals appears comparable to that of neurotypical peers, at least in 2D pictures (i.e., eye gaze patterns). Future studies should explore whether these results extend to real-life interactions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Manon Toutain
- CNRS, EthoS (Éthologie Animale et Humaine)—UMR 6552, University Rennes, Normandie University, F-35000 Rennes, France; (L.H.); (M.G.)
| | - Nicolas Dollion
- Laboratoire C2S (Cognition Santé Société)—EA6291, Université Reims Champagne-Ardenne, F-51100 Reims, France;
| | - Laurence Henry
- CNRS, EthoS (Éthologie Animale et Humaine)—UMR 6552, University Rennes, Normandie University, F-35000 Rennes, France; (L.H.); (M.G.)
| | - Marine Grandgeorge
- CNRS, EthoS (Éthologie Animale et Humaine)—UMR 6552, University Rennes, Normandie University, F-35000 Rennes, France; (L.H.); (M.G.)
| |
Collapse
|
20
|
Linke L, Horstmann G. Differences in the perception of direct gaze between the externally and internally rotated eye. Perception 2024; 53:93-109. [PMID: 37964541 DOI: 10.1177/03010066231212156] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/16/2023]
Abstract
The perception of direct gaze provides multiple benefits for the observer. Previous studies have investigated how the information from both eyes is used to estimate gaze direction, showing that the perception of gaze direction differs when only the externally rotated eye versus only the internally rotated eye is visible. We examined the width and center of the area of direct gaze by presenting the observers with either the externally or internally rotated eye, or both eyes with the task to judge whether a computer avatar is looking at them. Two experiments yield evidence for a wider area of direct gaze for the externally rotated eye (around 6°) than for the internally rotated eye (around 4°). The area of direct gaze for both eyes was found to be the same as for the internally rotated eye, but smaller than for the externally rotated eye. When both eyes were present, our results indicate that the perception of direct gaze is more likely to follow the internally rotated eye. The discussion substantiates a new generalization that observers base their judgments on the more rotated eye, which can differ due to angle kappa and vergence, in our study it is the internally rotated eye.
Collapse
|
21
|
Matsushima T, Izumi T, Vallortigara G. The domestic chick as an animal model of autism spectrum disorder: building adaptive social perceptions through prenatally formed predispositions. Front Neurosci 2024; 18:1279947. [PMID: 38356650 PMCID: PMC10864568 DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2024.1279947] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/19/2023] [Accepted: 01/10/2024] [Indexed: 02/16/2024] Open
Abstract
Equipped with an early social predisposition immediately post-birth, humans typically form associations with mothers and other family members through exposure learning, canalized by a prenatally formed predisposition of visual preference to biological motion, face configuration, and other cues of animacy. If impaired, reduced preferences can lead to social interaction impairments such as autism spectrum disorder (ASD) via misguided canalization. Despite being taxonomically distant, domestic chicks could also follow a homologous developmental trajectory toward adaptive socialization through imprinting, which is guided via predisposed preferences similar to those of humans, thereby suggesting that chicks are a valid animal model of ASD. In addition to the phenotypic similarities in predisposition with human newborns, accumulating evidence on the responsible molecular mechanisms suggests the construct validity of the chick model. Considering the recent progress in the evo-devo studies in vertebrates, we reviewed the advantages and limitations of the chick model of developmental mental diseases in humans.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Toshiya Matsushima
- Department of Biology, Faculty of Science, Hokkaido University, Sapporo, Japan
- Faculty of Pharmaceutical Science, Health Science University of Hokkaido, Tobetsu, Japan
- Centre for Mind/Brain Sciences, University of Trento, Rovereto, Italy
| | - Takeshi Izumi
- Faculty of Pharmaceutical Science, Health Science University of Hokkaido, Tobetsu, Japan
| | | |
Collapse
|
22
|
Konno A, Aoki H, Suzuki E, Furuta S, Ueda S. Are dark-eyed dogs favoured by humans? Domestication as a potential driver of iris colour difference between dogs and wolves. ROYAL SOCIETY OPEN SCIENCE 2023; 10:230854. [PMID: 38126061 PMCID: PMC10731317 DOI: 10.1098/rsos.230854] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/04/2023] [Accepted: 11/27/2023] [Indexed: 12/23/2023]
Abstract
Comparative studies have shown that the eye morphology of primates has been shaped by a variety of selection pressures (e.g. communication, environmental factors). To comprehensively elucidate the complex links between ocular morphology and its evolutionary drive, attention should be paid to other phylogenetic groups. Here, we address a new question regarding the evolution of eye colour patterns in the oldest domesticated animal, namely, the domestic dog (Canis familiaris). In this study, we conducted an image analysis of dogs and their closest relatives, grey wolves (Canis lupus), to compare the colours of their irises, with the aim of assessing whether eye colours of dogs affect how humans perceived dogs. We found that the irises of dogs were significantly darker than those of wolves. We also found that facial images of dark-eyed dogs were perceived as more friendly and immature, potentially eliciting caregiving responses from humans. Our findings are consistent with our expectation that humans favour dark-eyed dogs over light-eyed ones and provide an updated hypothesis that dogs with dark eyes may have evolved by acquiring a facial trait that sends a non-threatening gaze signal to humans.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Akitsugu Konno
- Department of Animal Sciences, Teikyo University of Science, Yamanashi, Japan
| | - Hitomi Aoki
- Department of Animal Sciences, Teikyo University of Science, Yamanashi, Japan
| | - Emiri Suzuki
- Department of Animal Sciences, Teikyo University of Science, Yamanashi, Japan
| | - Seiya Furuta
- Department of Animal Sciences, Teikyo University of Science, Yamanashi, Japan
| | - Sayoko Ueda
- The Mt. Fuji Institute for Nature and Biology, Showa University, Yamanashi, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Ziman K, Kimmel SC, Farrell KT, Graziano MSA. Predicting the attention of others. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2023; 120:e2307584120. [PMID: 37812722 PMCID: PMC10589679 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.2307584120] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/05/2023] [Accepted: 09/05/2023] [Indexed: 10/11/2023] Open
Abstract
As social animals, people are highly sensitive to the attention of others. Seeing someone else gaze at an object automatically draws one's own attention to that object. Monitoring the attention of others aids in reconstructing their emotions, beliefs, and intentions and may play a crucial role in social alignment. Recently, however, it has been suggested that the human brain constructs a predictive model of other people's attention that is far more involved than a moment-by-moment monitoring of gaze direction. The hypothesized model learns the statistical patterns in other people's attention and extrapolates how attention is likely to move. Here, we tested the hypothesis of a predictive model of attention. Subjects saw movies of attention displayed as a bright spot shifting around a scene. Subjects were able to correctly distinguish natural attention sequences (based on eye tracking of prior participants) from altered sequences (e.g., played backward or in a scrambled order). Even when the attention spot moved around a blank background, subjects could distinguish natural from scrambled sequences, suggesting a sensitivity to the spatial-temporal statistics of attention. Subjects also showed an ability to recognize the attention patterns of different individuals. These results suggest that people possess a sophisticated model of the normal statistics of attention and can identify deviations from the model. Monitoring attention is therefore more than simply registering where someone else's eyes are pointing. It involves predictive modeling, which may contribute to our remarkable social ability to predict the mind states and behavior of others.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kirsten Ziman
- Princeton Neuroscience Institute, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ08544
| | - Sarah C. Kimmel
- Princeton Neuroscience Institute, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ08544
| | - Kathryn T. Farrell
- Princeton Neuroscience Institute, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ08544
| | - Michael S. A. Graziano
- Princeton Neuroscience Institute, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ08544
- Department of Psychology, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ08544
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Pollen AA, Kilik U, Lowe CB, Camp JG. Human-specific genetics: new tools to explore the molecular and cellular basis of human evolution. Nat Rev Genet 2023; 24:687-711. [PMID: 36737647 PMCID: PMC9897628 DOI: 10.1038/s41576-022-00568-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 48] [Impact Index Per Article: 24.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 12/08/2022] [Indexed: 02/05/2023]
Abstract
Our ancestors acquired morphological, cognitive and metabolic modifications that enabled humans to colonize diverse habitats, develop extraordinary technologies and reshape the biosphere. Understanding the genetic, developmental and molecular bases for these changes will provide insights into how we became human. Connecting human-specific genetic changes to species differences has been challenging owing to an abundance of low-effect size genetic changes, limited descriptions of phenotypic differences across development at the level of cell types and lack of experimental models. Emerging approaches for single-cell sequencing, genetic manipulation and stem cell culture now support descriptive and functional studies in defined cell types with a human or ape genetic background. In this Review, we describe how the sequencing of genomes from modern and archaic hominins, great apes and other primates is revealing human-specific genetic changes and how new molecular and cellular approaches - including cell atlases and organoids - are enabling exploration of the candidate causal factors that underlie human-specific traits.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alex A Pollen
- Eli and Edythe Broad Center of Regeneration Medicine and Stem Cell Research, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, USA.
- Department of Neurology, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, USA.
| | - Umut Kilik
- Institute of Human Biology (IHB), Roche Pharma Research and Early Development, Roche Innovation Center Basel, Basel, Switzerland
- University of Basel, Basel, Switzerland
| | - Craig B Lowe
- Department of Molecular Genetics and Microbiology, Duke University School of Medicine, Durham, NC, USA.
| | - J Gray Camp
- Institute of Human Biology (IHB), Roche Pharma Research and Early Development, Roche Innovation Center Basel, Basel, Switzerland.
- University of Basel, Basel, Switzerland.
| |
Collapse
|
25
|
Troje NF. Depth from motion parallax: Deictic consistency, eye contact, and a serious problem with Zoom. J Vis 2023; 23:1. [PMID: 37656465 PMCID: PMC10479236 DOI: 10.1167/jov.23.10.1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/31/2023] [Accepted: 07/24/2023] [Indexed: 09/02/2023] Open
Abstract
The dynamics of head and eye gaze between two or more individuals displayed during verbal and nonverbal face-to-face communication contains a wealth of information and is used for both volitionary and unconscious signaling. Current video communication systems convey visual signals about gaze behavior and other directional cues, but the information they carry is often spurious and potentially misleading. I discuss the consequences of this situation, identify the source of the problem as a more general lack of deictic consistency, and demonstrate that using display technologies that simulate motion parallax are both necessary and sufficient to alleviate it. I then devise an avatar-based remote communication solution that achieves deictic consistency and provides natural, dynamic eye contact for computer-mediated audiovisual communication.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nikolaus F Troje
- Centre for Vision Research and Department of Biology, York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
26
|
Sexton CL, Buckley C, Lieberfarb J, Subiaul F, Hecht EE, Bradley BJ. What Is Written on a Dog's Face? Evaluating the Impact of Facial Phenotypes on Communication between Humans and Canines. Animals (Basel) 2023; 13:2385. [PMID: 37508162 PMCID: PMC10376741 DOI: 10.3390/ani13142385] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/01/2023] [Revised: 07/15/2023] [Accepted: 07/20/2023] [Indexed: 07/30/2023] Open
Abstract
Facial phenotypes are significant in communication with conspecifics among social primates. Less is understood about the impact of such markers in heterospecific encounters. Through behavioral and physical phenotype analyses of domesticated dogs living in human households, this study aims to evaluate the potential impact of superficial facial markings on dogs' production of human-directed facial expressions. That is, this study explores how facial markings, such as eyebrows, patches, and widow's peaks, are related to expressivity toward humans. We used the Dog Facial Action Coding System (DogFACS) as an objective measure of expressivity, and we developed an original schematic for a standardized coding of facial patterns and coloration on a sample of more than 100 male and female dogs (N = 103), aged from 6 months to 12 years, representing eight breed groups. The present study found a statistically significant, though weak, correlation between expression rate and facial complexity, with dogs with plainer faces tending to be more expressive (r = -0.326, p ≤ 0.001). Interestingly, for adult dogs, human companions characterized dogs' rates of facial expressivity with more accuracy for dogs with plainer faces. Especially relevant to interspecies communication and cooperation, within-subject analyses revealed that dogs' muscle movements were distributed more evenly across their facial regions in a highly social test condition compared to conditions in which they received ambiguous cues from their owners. On the whole, this study provides an original evaluation of how facial features may impact communication in human-dog interactions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Courtney L Sexton
- Department of Population Health Sciences, Virginia-Maryland College of Veterinary Medicine, Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University, Blacksburg, VA 24061, USA
- Center for the Advanced Study of Human Paleobiology, Department of Anthropology, The George Washington University, Washington, DC 20052, USA
| | - Colleen Buckley
- Center for the Advanced Study of Human Paleobiology, Department of Anthropology, The George Washington University, Washington, DC 20052, USA
| | | | - Francys Subiaul
- Center for the Advanced Study of Human Paleobiology, Department of Anthropology, The George Washington University, Washington, DC 20052, USA
- Department of Speech, Language and Hearing Sciences, The George Washington University, Washington, DC 20052, USA
| | - Erin E Hecht
- Department of Human Evolutionary Biology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA
| | - Brenda J Bradley
- Center for the Advanced Study of Human Paleobiology, Department of Anthropology, The George Washington University, Washington, DC 20052, USA
| |
Collapse
|
27
|
Park SY, Holmqvist K, Niehorster DC, Huber L, Virányi Z. How to improve data quality in dog eye tracking. Behav Res Methods 2023; 55:1513-1536. [PMID: 35680764 PMCID: PMC10250523 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-022-01788-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 01/02/2022] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Pupil-corneal reflection (P-CR) eye tracking has gained a prominent role in studying dog visual cognition, despite methodological challenges that often lead to lower-quality data than when recording from humans. In the current study, we investigated if and how the morphology of dogs might interfere with tracking of P-CR systems, and to what extent such interference, possibly in combination with dog-unique eye-movement characteristics, may undermine data quality and affect eye-movement classification when processed through algorithms. For this aim, we have conducted an eye-tracking experiment with dogs and humans, and investigated incidences of tracking interference, compared how they blinked, and examined how differential quality of dog and human data affected the detection and classification of eye-movement events. Our results show that the morphology of dogs' face and eye can interfere with tracking methods of the systems, and dogs blink less often but their blinks are longer. Importantly, the lower quality of dog data lead to larger differences in how two different event detection algorithms classified fixations, indicating that the results of key dependent variables are more susceptible to choice of algorithm in dog than human data. Further, two measures of the Nyström & Holmqvist (Behavior Research Methods, 42(4), 188-204, 2010) algorithm showed that dog fixations are less stable and dog data have more trials with extreme levels of noise. Our findings call for analyses better adjusted to the characteristics of dog eye-tracking data, and our recommendations help future dog eye-tracking studies acquire quality data to enable robust comparisons of visual cognition between dogs and humans.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Soon Young Park
- Comparative Cognition, Messerli Research Institute, University of Veterinary Medicine Vienna, Vienna, Austria.
- Medical University Vienna, Vienna, Austria.
- University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria.
| | - Kenneth Holmqvist
- Institute of Psychology, Nicolaus Copernicus University in Torun, Torun, Poland
- Department of Psychology, Regensburg University, Regensburg, Germany
- Department of Computer Science and Informatics, University of the Free State, Bloemfontein, South Africa
| | - Diederick C Niehorster
- Lund University Humanities Lab and Department of Psychology, Lund University, Lund, Sweden
| | - Ludwig Huber
- Comparative Cognition, Messerli Research Institute, University of Veterinary Medicine Vienna, Vienna, Austria
- Medical University Vienna, Vienna, Austria
- University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
| | - Zsófia Virányi
- Comparative Cognition, Messerli Research Institute, University of Veterinary Medicine Vienna, Vienna, Austria
- Medical University Vienna, Vienna, Austria
- University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
| |
Collapse
|
28
|
Yu C, Ishibashi K, Iwanaga K. Effects of fearful face presentation time and observer's eye movement on the gaze cue effect. J Physiol Anthropol 2023; 42:8. [PMID: 37248516 DOI: 10.1186/s40101-023-00325-4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/30/2022] [Accepted: 05/12/2023] [Indexed: 05/31/2023] Open
Abstract
BACKGROUND There are many conflicting findings on the gaze cueing effect (GCE) of emotional facial expressions. This study aimed to investigate whether an averted gaze, accompanied by a fearful expression of different durations, could enhance attentional orientation, as measured by a participant's eye movements. METHODS Twelve participants (3 females) completed the gaze cue task, reacting to a target location after observing changes in the gaze and expression of a face illustrated on a computer screen. Meanwhile, participants' eye movements were monitored by electrooculography. The GCE was calculated by reaction time as an indicator of attention shift. RESULTS The analysis of the overall data did not find a significant effect of fearful facial expressions on the GCE. However, analysis of trial data that excluded a participant's eye movement data showed that brief (0, 100 ms) presentation of the fearful facial expression enhanced the GCE compared to that during a neutral facial expression, although when the presentation time of the fearful expression was increased to 200 or 400 ms, the GCE of the fearful expression was at the same level as when model showed a neutral expression. CONCLUSIONS The results suggest that the attention-enhancing effect of gaze cues induced by rapidly presented fearful expressions occurs only when the effect of eye movement trials is excluded. This effect may be mediated by reflexively neural circuits in the amygdala that process threatening stimuli. However, as the expression duration increased, the fearful expression's attention-enhancing effect decreased. We suggest that future studies on the emotion modulation of GCE should consider the negative effects of participants' saccades and blinks on the experimental results.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Chuntai Yu
- Graduate School of Science and Engineering, Chiba University, 1-33 Yayoi-Cho, Inage-Ku, Chiba, 263-8522, Japan
| | - Keita Ishibashi
- Design Research Institute, Chiba University, 1-33 Yayoi-Cho, Inage-Ku, Chiba, 263-8522, Japan
| | - Koichi Iwanaga
- Design Research Institute, Chiba University, 1-33 Yayoi-Cho, Inage-Ku, Chiba, 263-8522, Japan.
| |
Collapse
|
29
|
Van Lange PAM, Manesi Z. Reputation Reminders: When do Eye Cues Promote Prosocial Behavior? THE SPANISH JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY 2023; 26:e8. [PMID: 37127294 DOI: 10.1017/sjp.2023.4] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/03/2023]
Abstract
The watching eyes effect has gained significant attention in recent years both from scientists and from policy makers and professionals in the field. The phenomenon posits that the mere presence of eye cues can promote prosocial behavior. However, there is a growing debate about the generality of the effect across various measures and contexts. This review seeks to combine various distinct -and formerly isolated- perspectives by identifying four key components for effective interventions based on the watching eyes effect: Anonymity, crowdedness, costs, and exposure. Eye cues need to reduce perceived anonymity, be placed in non-crowded places, target low-cost prosocial acts and appear for a short amount of time. Next to these conditions, we discuss implications for other cues to reputation and recommend directions that will stimulate further research and applications in society.
Collapse
|
30
|
Humanity at first sight: Exploring the relationship between others' pupil size and ascriptions of humanity. JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 2023. [DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2023.104455] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/11/2023]
|
31
|
Breu MS, Ramezanpour H, Dicke PW, Thier P. A frontoparietal network for volitional control of gaze following. Eur J Neurosci 2023; 57:1723-1735. [PMID: 36967647 DOI: 10.1111/ejn.15975] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/24/2022] [Revised: 03/13/2023] [Accepted: 03/22/2023] [Indexed: 03/29/2023]
Abstract
Gaze following is a major element of non-verbal communication and important for successful social interactions. Human gaze following is a fast and almost reflex-like behaviour, yet it can be volitionally controlled and suppressed to some extent if inappropriate or unnecessary, given the social context. In order to identify the neural basis of the cognitive control of gaze following, we carried out an event-related fMRI experiment, in which human subjects' eye movements were tracked while they were exposed to gaze cues in two distinct contexts: A baseline gaze following condition in which subjects were instructed to use gaze cues to shift their attention to a gazed-at spatial target and a control condition in which the subjects were required to ignore the gaze cue and instead to shift their attention to a distinct spatial target to be selected based on a colour mapping rule, requiring the suppression of gaze following. We could identify a suppression-related blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) response in a frontoparietal network comprising dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC), orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), the anterior insula, precuneus, and posterior parietal cortex (PPC). These findings suggest that overexcitation of frontoparietal circuits in turn suppressing the gaze following patch might be a potential cause of gaze following deficits in clinical populations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- M S Breu
- Cognitive Neurology Laboratory, Hertie Institute for Clinical Brain Research, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
| | - H Ramezanpour
- Cognitive Neurology Laboratory, Hertie Institute for Clinical Brain Research, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
| | - P W Dicke
- Cognitive Neurology Laboratory, Hertie Institute for Clinical Brain Research, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
| | - P Thier
- Cognitive Neurology Laboratory, Hertie Institute for Clinical Brain Research, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
- Werner Reichardt Centre for Integrative Neuroscience, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
32
|
Clark IR, Lee KC, Poux T, Langergraber KE, Mitani JC, Watts D, Reed J, Sandel AA. White sclera is present in chimpanzees and other mammals. J Hum Evol 2023; 176:103322. [PMID: 36706647 PMCID: PMC9998187 DOI: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2022.103322] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/16/2022] [Revised: 12/22/2022] [Accepted: 12/23/2022] [Indexed: 01/27/2023]
Affiliation(s)
- Isabelle R Clark
- Department of Anthropology, University of Texas at Austin, 2201 Speedway, Austin, TX 78712, USA
| | - Kevin C Lee
- School of Human Evolution and Social Change and Institute of Human Origins, Arizona State University, 900 S. Cady Mall, Tempe, AZ 85281, USA
| | - Tucker Poux
- Tufts University, 419 Boston Ave, Medford, MA 02155, USA
| | - Kevin E Langergraber
- School of Human Evolution and Social Change and Institute of Human Origins, Arizona State University, 900 S. Cady Mall, Tempe, AZ 85281, USA
| | - John C Mitani
- Department of Anthropology, University of Michigan, 1085 S. University Ave., Ann Arbor, MI 48109, USA
| | - David Watts
- Department of Anthropology, Yale University, P.O. Box 208277, New Haven, CT 06520, USA
| | - James Reed
- Underdog Films Ltd, 2 St. Paul's Road, Bristol, BS8 1LT, UK
| | - Aaron A Sandel
- Department of Anthropology, University of Texas at Austin, 2201 Speedway, Austin, TX 78712, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
33
|
Wolf W, Thielhelm J, Tomasello M. Five-year-old children show cooperative preferences for faces with white sclera. J Exp Child Psychol 2023; 225:105532. [PMID: 35988359 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2022.105532] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/22/2021] [Revised: 07/29/2022] [Accepted: 07/29/2022] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
The cooperative eye hypothesis posits that human eye morphology evolved to facilitate cooperation. Although it is known that young children prefer stimuli with eyes that contain white sclera, it is unknown whether white sclera influences children's perception of a partner's cooperativeness specifically. In the current studies, we used an online methodology to present 5-year-old children with moving three-dimensional face models in which facial morphology was manipulated. Children found "alien" faces with human eyes more cooperative than faces with dark sclera (Study 2) but not faces with enlarged irises (Study 1). For more human-like faces (Study 3), children found human eyes more cooperative than either enlarged irises or dark sclera and found faces with enlarged irises cuter (but not more cooperative) than eyes with dark sclera. Together, these results provide strong support for the cooperative eye hypothesis.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Wouter Wolf
- Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, NC 27708, USA; Department of Developmental Psychology, Utrecht University, 3584 CS Utrecht, the Netherlands.
| | - Julia Thielhelm
- Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, NC 27708, USA
| | - Michael Tomasello
- Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, NC 27708, USA; Department of Developmental and Comparative Psychology, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, 04103 Leipzig, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
34
|
Are there quantitative differences between eye-gaze and arrow cues? A meta-analytic answer to the debate and a call for qualitative differences. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2023; 144:104993. [PMID: 36496190 DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2022.104993] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/20/2022] [Revised: 12/03/2022] [Accepted: 12/05/2022] [Indexed: 12/12/2022]
Abstract
Gaze acts from an early age as a cue to orient attention and, thereafter, to infer our social partners' intentions, thoughts, and emotions. Variants of the attentional orienting paradigm have been used to study the orienting capabilities associated to eye gaze. However, to date, it is still unclear whether this methodology truly assesses "social-specific" processes exclusively involved in attention to eye-gaze or the operation of domain-general attentional processes. The present study provides a comprehensive meta-analysis indicating that eye-gaze and non-social directional stimuli, such as arrows, produce equivalent attentional effects. This result casts doubt on the potential utility of the classic cueing task in revealing social-specific processes. On the other hand, we review behavioral evidence suggesting that eye-gaze stimuli may induce higher-order social processes when more specific experimental procedures that analyze qualitative rather than quantitative differences are used. These findings point to an integrated view in which domain-general and social specific processes both contribute to the attentional mechanisms induced by eye-gaze direction. Finally, some proposals about the social components specifically triggered by eye-gaze stimuli are discussed.
Collapse
|
35
|
Aday JS, Fang L, Carlson JM. Eye-size effects in the dot-probe task: Greater sclera exposure predicts delayed disengagement from fearful faces. PLoS One 2023; 18:e0285839. [PMID: 37195990 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0285839] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/16/2022] [Accepted: 05/03/2023] [Indexed: 05/19/2023] Open
Abstract
Fearful facial expressions are nonverbal and biologically salient signals of potential threat that automatically hold, capture, and direct observers' attention. They are characterized by enlarged eye whites and dilated pupils, and fearful eyes alone are sufficient to capture attention. The morphological properties of the eye region, such as sclera exposure, are thought to play an important role in nonverbal communication. Specifically, increased sclera exposure associated with fearful expressions has been shown to moderate how observers' shift their attention toward the direction of another's gaze. Yet, the extent to which variability in sclera exposure possibly impacts the capture and hold of attention by fearful faces is untested. To address this, a sample of 249 adults completed a dot-probe task of selective attention with fearful and neutral faces. The results suggested that (1) fearful faces were prioritized over neutral faces (i.e., they captured and held attention), (2) greater sclera exposure at target locations facilitated reaction times, and (3) attention was held by greater sclera exposure of fearful faces at task irrelevant locations resulting in delayed disengagement. Collectively, the results indicate that fearful facial expressions and sclera exposure modulate spatial attention through independent and interactive mechanisms. Sclera exposure appears to be an important facilitator of nonverbal communication and perhaps represents an understudied variable in social cognition more broadly.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jacob S Aday
- Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of California, San Francisco, California, United States of America
| | - Lin Fang
- Department of Psychological Science, Northern Michigan University, Marquette, Michigan, United States of America
| | - Joshua M Carlson
- Department of Psychological Science, Northern Michigan University, Marquette, Michigan, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
36
|
Hattori M, Saito A, Nagasawa M, Kikusui T, Yamamoto S. Changes in Cat Facial Morphology Are Related to Interaction with Humans. Animals (Basel) 2022; 12:ani12243493. [PMID: 36552413 PMCID: PMC9774281 DOI: 10.3390/ani12243493] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/27/2022] [Revised: 12/03/2022] [Accepted: 12/05/2022] [Indexed: 12/14/2022] Open
Abstract
We aimed to clarify the changes in facial morphology of cats in relation to their interactions with humans. In Study 1, we compared the facial morphology of cats (feral mixed breed, owned domestic mixed breed, and owned domestic purebreds) with that of African wildcats. After collecting 3295 photos, we found that owned domestic cats' noses were significantly shorter than those of African wildcats and feral mixed breed, and there were no significant differences between the latter two. The eye angles were significantly more gradual in owned domestic purebreds than in the other groups. In Study 2, we examined the correlation between facial morphology and years with the owner, and found that the former is not affected by the latter. This suggests that changes in facial morphology are possibly transgenerational changes. The difference in facial morphology between wildcats and owned cats might be caused by domestication, and that between feral cats and owned cats might be due to feralization. In Study 3, we investigated whether cats' facial features affect cuteness ratings. We asked human participants to evaluate the cuteness of cats' face images and found that faces with shorter nose lengths were considered cuter. This suggests that owned domestic cats' facial morphology is preferred by humans.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Madoka Hattori
- Wildlife Research Center, Kyoto University, 2-24 Tanaka-Sekiden-cho, Sakyo-ku, Kyoto 606-8203, Japan
- Department of Animal Science and Biotechnology, Azabu University, 1-17-71 Fuchinobe, Chuo-ku, Sagamihara-shi 252-5201, Japan
| | - Atsuko Saito
- Department of Psychology, Faculty of Human Sciences, Sophia University, 7-1 Kioicho, Chiyoda-ku, Tokyo 102-8554, Japan
| | - Miho Nagasawa
- Department of Animal Science and Biotechnology, Azabu University, 1-17-71 Fuchinobe, Chuo-ku, Sagamihara-shi 252-5201, Japan
| | - Takefumi Kikusui
- Department of Animal Science and Biotechnology, Azabu University, 1-17-71 Fuchinobe, Chuo-ku, Sagamihara-shi 252-5201, Japan
| | - Shinya Yamamoto
- Wildlife Research Center, Kyoto University, 2-24 Tanaka-Sekiden-cho, Sakyo-ku, Kyoto 606-8203, Japan
- Institute for Advanced Study, Kyoto University, Yoshida Ushinomiya-cho, Sakyo-ku, Kyoto 606-8501, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
37
|
Horstmann G, Linke L. Perception of direct gaze in a video-conference setting: the effects of position and size. Cogn Res Princ Implic 2022; 7:67. [PMID: 35867185 PMCID: PMC9307695 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-022-00418-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/04/2022] [Accepted: 06/28/2022] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
A common problem in video conferences is gaze direction. In face-to-face communication, it is common that speaker and listener intermittently look at each other. In a video-conference setting, where multiple participants are on the screen, things are complicated and not necessarily optimal. If the listener feels looked at when the speaker looks into the camera, how tolerant is the listener for slight deviations? And does this depend on the position of the speaker’s tile on the screen, or the size of the tile? In a first experiment, participants from a student population judged whether they are looked at, while vertical gaze direction of the looker was varied. Furthermore, the position of the tile on the screen varied. The results showed that a slightly upward directed gaze was optimal for the direct gaze judgment, with a width of ± 4 degrees. Optimal gaze direction was somewhat higher for tiles at the bottom of the screen. A second experiment tested the effect of size on the perception of horizontal gaze directions. Size was found to increase the gaze cone. The paper concludes with some recommendations for a setup of video conference systems, optimized for perceived gaze contact.
Collapse
|
38
|
Wacewicz S, Perea-García JO, Lewandowski Z, Danel DP. The adaptive significance of human scleral brightness: an experimental study. Sci Rep 2022; 12:20261. [PMID: 36424405 PMCID: PMC9691750 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-022-24403-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/24/2022] [Accepted: 11/15/2022] [Indexed: 11/27/2022] Open
Abstract
Homogeneously depigmented sclerae have long been proposed to be uniquely human-an adaptation to enable cooperative behaviour by facilitating interpersonal coordination through gaze following. However, recent evidence has shown that deeply pigmented sclerae also afford gaze following if surrounding a bright iris. Furthermore, while current scleral depigmentation is clearly adaptive in modern humans, it is less clear how the evolutionarily intermediate stages of scleral pigmentation may have been adaptive. In sum, it is unclear why scleral depigmentation became the norm in humans, while not so in sister species like chimpanzees, or why some extant species display intermediate degrees of pigmentation (as our ancestors presumably did at some point). We created realistic facial images of 20 individually distinct hominins with diverse facial morphologies, each face in the (i) humanlike bright sclera and (ii) generalised apelike dark sclera version. Participants in two online studies rated the bright-sclera hominins as younger, healthier, more attractive and trustworthy, but less aggressive than the dark-sclera hominins. Our results support the idea that the appearance of more depigmented sclerae promoted perceived traits that fostered trust, increasing fitness for those individuals and resulting in depigmentation as a fixed trait in extant humans.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Slawomir Wacewicz
- Center for Language Evolution Studies, Nicolaus Copernicus University, Toruń, Poland.
| | - Juan Olvido Perea-García
- The Cognitive Psychology Unit, Faculty of Social Sciences, Leiden University, Leiden, The Netherlands
| | - Zdzisław Lewandowski
- Department of Human Biology, Wroclaw University of Health and Sport Sciences, Wroclaw, Poland
| | - Dariusz P Danel
- Department of Anthropology, Hirszfeld Institute of Immunology and Experimental Therapy, Polish Academy of Sciences, Wroclaw, Poland
| |
Collapse
|
39
|
Hietanen JO, Syrjämäki AH, Hietanen JK. Perception of eye contact, self-referential thinking and age. Conscious Cogn 2022; 106:103435. [PMID: 36399921 DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2022.103435] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/21/2021] [Revised: 10/28/2022] [Accepted: 10/31/2022] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
Increased thinking about one's self has been proposed to widen the gaze cone, that is, the range of gaze deviations that an observer judges as looking directly at them (eye contact). This study investigated the effects of a self-referential thinking manipulation and demographic factors on the gaze cone. In a preregistered experiment (N = 200), the self-referential thinking manipulation, as compared to a control manipulation, did not influence the gaze cone, or the use of first-person pronouns in a manipulation check measuring self-referential processing. This may indicate a failure of the manipulation and participants' lack of effort. However, participants' age was significantly correlated with both measures: older people had wider gaze cones and used more self-referring pronouns. A second experiment (N = 300) further examined the effect of the manipulation and demographic factors on self-referential processing, and the results were replicated. These findings may reflect age-related self-reference and positivity effects.
Collapse
|
40
|
Infants infer third-party social dominance relationships based on visual access to intergroup conflict. Sci Rep 2022; 12:18250. [PMID: 36309546 PMCID: PMC9617854 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-022-22640-z] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/22/2021] [Accepted: 10/18/2022] [Indexed: 12/31/2022] Open
Abstract
During a conflict, having a greater number of allies than the opposition can improve one's success in a conflict. However, allies must be aware that has a conflict has occurred, and this is often influenced by what they are able to see. Here, we explored whether infants' assessment of social dominance is influenced by whether or not social allies have visual access to an episode of intergroup conflict. In Experiment 1, 9-12-month-olds only expected an agent to be socially dominant if their allies were able to witness the conflict. Experiment 2 provided further support for this finding, as infants did not expect an agent from a numerically larger group to be socially dominant when allies were unable to witness the conflict. Together, these results suggest that infants do not simply use a heuristic in which "numerically larger groups are always more dominant". Importantly, infants are able to incorporate social allies' ability to witness a conflict when predicting social dominance between groups.
Collapse
|
41
|
Vaitonytė J, Alimardani M, Louwerse MM. Corneal reflections and skin contrast yield better memory of human and virtual faces. Cogn Res Princ Implic 2022; 7:94. [PMID: 36258062 PMCID: PMC9579222 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-022-00445-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/21/2022] [Accepted: 10/07/2022] [Indexed: 11/10/2022] Open
Abstract
Virtual faces have been found to be rated less human-like and remembered worse than photographic images of humans. What it is in virtual faces that yields reduced memory has so far remained unclear. The current study investigated face memory in the context of virtual agent faces and human faces, real and manipulated, considering two factors of predicted influence, i.e., corneal reflections and skin contrast. Corneal reflections referred to the bright points in each eye that occur when the ambient light reflects from the surface of the cornea. Skin contrast referred to the degree to which skin surface is rough versus smooth. We conducted two memory experiments, one with high-quality virtual agent faces (Experiment 1) and the other with the photographs of human faces that were manipulated (Experiment 2). Experiment 1 showed better memory for virtual faces with increased corneal reflections and skin contrast (rougher rather than smoother skin). Experiment 2 replicated these findings, showing that removing the corneal reflections and smoothening the skin reduced memory recognition of manipulated faces, with a stronger effect exerted by the eyes than the skin. This study highlights specific features of the eyes and skin that can help explain memory discrepancies between real and virtual faces and in turn elucidates the factors that play a role in the cognitive processing of faces.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Julija Vaitonytė
- grid.12295.3d0000 0001 0943 3265Department of Cognitive Science and Artificial Intelligence, Tilburg University, Dante Building D 134, Warandelaan 2, 5037 AB Tilburg, The Netherlands
| | - Maryam Alimardani
- grid.12295.3d0000 0001 0943 3265Department of Cognitive Science and Artificial Intelligence, Tilburg University, Dante Building D 134, Warandelaan 2, 5037 AB Tilburg, The Netherlands
| | - Max M. Louwerse
- grid.12295.3d0000 0001 0943 3265Department of Cognitive Science and Artificial Intelligence, Tilburg University, Dante Building D 134, Warandelaan 2, 5037 AB Tilburg, The Netherlands
| |
Collapse
|
42
|
Perea-García JO, Ramarajan K, Kret ME, Hobaiter C, Monteiro A. Ecological factors are likely drivers of eye shape and colour pattern variations across anthropoid primates. Sci Rep 2022; 12:17240. [PMID: 36243745 PMCID: PMC9569326 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-022-20900-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/29/2022] [Accepted: 09/20/2022] [Indexed: 01/06/2023] Open
Abstract
External eye appearance across primate species is diverse in shape and colouration, yet we still lack an explanation for the drivers of such diversity. Here we quantify substantial interspecific variation in eye shape and colouration across 77 primate species representing all extant genera of anthropoid primates. We reassess a series of hypotheses aiming to explain ocular variation in horizontal elongation and in colouration across species. Heavier body weight and terrestrial locomotion are associated with elongated eye outlines. Species living closer to the equator present more pigmented conjunctivae, suggesting photoprotective functions. Irises become bluer in species living further away from the equator, adding to existing literature supporting a circadian clock function for bluer irises. These results shift the current focus from communicative, to ecological factors in driving variation in external eye appearance in anthropoid primates. They also highlight the possibility that similar ecological factors contributed to selection for blue eyes in ancestral human populations living in northern latitudes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Juan Olvido Perea-García
- Department of Biological Sciences, National University of Singapore, Singapore, 117557, Singapore.
- Institute of Psychology, Cognitive Psychology Unit, Leiden University, Wassenaarseweg 52, 2333 AK, Leiden, The Netherlands.
| | | | - Mariska E Kret
- Institute of Psychology, Cognitive Psychology Unit, Leiden University, Wassenaarseweg 52, 2333 AK, Leiden, The Netherlands
| | - Catherine Hobaiter
- Centre for Social Learning and Cognitive Evolution and Scottish Primate Research Group, School of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of St Andrews, Fife, Scotland, UK
| | - Antónia Monteiro
- Department of Biological Sciences, National University of Singapore, Singapore, 117557, Singapore
- Science Division, Yale-NUS College, Singapore, Singapore
| |
Collapse
|
43
|
Veitch E, Dybvik H, Steinert M, Alsos OA. Collaborative Work with Highly Automated Marine Navigation Systems. Comput Support Coop Work 2022:1-32. [PMID: 36250043 PMCID: PMC9547091 DOI: 10.1007/s10606-022-09450-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 09/08/2022] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
In navigation applications, Artificial Intelligence (AI) can improve efficiency and decision making. It is not clear, however, how designers should account for human cooperation when integrating AI systems in navigation work. In a novel empirical study, we examine the transition in the maritime domain towards higher levels of machine autonomy. Our method involved interviewing technology designers (n = 9) and navigators aboard two partially automated ferries (n = 5), as well as collecting field observations aboard one of the ferries. The results indicated a discrepancy between how designers construed human-AI collaboration compared to navigators' own accounts in the field. Navigators reflected upon their role as one of 'backup,' defined by ad-hoc control takeovers from the automation. Designers positioned navigators 'in the loop' of a larger control system but discounted the role of in-situ skills and heuristic decision making in all but the most controlled takeover actions. The discrepancy shed light on how integration of AI systems may be better aligned to human cooperation in navigation. This included designing AI systems that render computational activities more visible and that incorporate social cues that articulate human work in its natural setting. Positioned within the field of AI alignment research, the main contribution is a formulation of human-AI interaction design insights for future navigation and control room work.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Erik Veitch
- Department of Design, Faculty of Architecture and Design, Norwegian University of Science and Technology (NTNU), Produktdesign, 341, Gløshaugen, Kolbjørn Hejes Vei 2 B, 7491 Trondheim, Norway
| | - Henrikke Dybvik
- Department of Mechanical and Industrial Engineering, Faculty of Engineering, Norwegian University of Science and Technology (NTNU), Verkstedteknisk, P317, Gløshaugen, Richard Birkelandsvei 2B, 7034 Trondheim, Norway
| | - Martin Steinert
- Department of Mechanical and Industrial Engineering, Faculty of Engineering, Norwegian University of Science and Technology (NTNU), Verkstedteknisk, P317, Gløshaugen, Richard Birkelandsvei 2B, 7034 Trondheim, Norway
| | - Ole Andreas Alsos
- Department of Design, Faculty of Architecture and Design, Norwegian University of Science and Technology (NTNU), Produktdesign, 341, Gløshaugen, Kolbjørn Hejes Vei 2 B, 7491 Trondheim, Norway
| |
Collapse
|
44
|
Viola M. Seeing through the shades of situated affectivity. Sunglasses as a socio-affective artifact. PHILOSOPHICAL PSYCHOLOGY 2022. [DOI: 10.1080/09515089.2022.2118574] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/14/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Marco Viola
- Department of Philosophy, Communication, and Performing Arts, Rome 3 University, Rome, Italy
| |
Collapse
|
45
|
Jantzen KJ, McNamara N, Harris A, Schubert A, Brooks M, Seifert M, Symons LA. Contrast reversal of the iris and sclera increases the face sensitive N170. Front Hum Neurosci 2022; 16:987217. [PMID: 36158625 PMCID: PMC9491205 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2022.987217] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/05/2022] [Accepted: 08/12/2022] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
Previous research has demonstrated that reversing the contrast of the eye region, which includes the eyebrows, affects the N170 ERP. To selectively assess the impact of just the eyes, the present study evaluated the N170 in response to reversing contrast polarity of just the iris and sclera in upright and inverted face stimuli. Contrast reversal of the eyes increased the amplitude of the N170 for upright faces, but not for inverted faces, suggesting that the contrast of eyes is an important contributor to the N170 ERP.
Collapse
|
46
|
Linke L, Horstmann G. How vergence influences the perception of being looked at. Perception 2022; 51:789-803. [PMID: 36062732 DOI: 10.1177/03010066221122359] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022]
Abstract
Perceiving other people's direct gaze is important for many areas of everyday activity. For horizontal and vertical eye movements, the area of being looked at, known as the cone of gaze, has been well explored. Previous research has shown a range of eye rotations (up to eccentricities of 4°-9°) that people accept as direct gaze. Vergence is an important cue for perceiving the depth of fixation. This study examines the range of vergence angles that support the perception of being looked at. In two experiments, observers adjusted the degree of vergence of the lookers' eyes until they felt just (not) looked at. The first experiment also asked to adjust the point of being exactly looked at, which was 0° (parallel eyes). The thresholds of being just (not) looked at were around 4.5° of convergence and 2.5° divergence, which results in a depth of 7° of vergence. This depth was replicated in Experiment 2, while the thresholds of convergence (3.5°) and divergence (3.5°) slightly differ from Experiment 1. The results indicate a consistent area of vergences being accepted as direct gaze, yielding first-time evidence for a third dimension-the depth dimension-of direct gaze.
Collapse
|
47
|
Abstract
Eye contact is essential for human interactions. We investigated whether humans are able to avoid eye contact while navigating crowds. At a science festival, we fitted 62 participants with a wearable eye tracker and instructed them to walk a route. Half of the participants were further instructed to avoid eye contact. We report that humans can flexibly allocate their gaze while navigating crowds and avoid eye contact primarily by orienting their head and eyes towards the floor. We discuss implications for crowd navigation and gaze behavior. In addition, we address a number of issues encountered in such field studies with regard to data quality, control of the environment, and participant adherence to instructions. We stress that methodological innovation and scientific progress are strongly interrelated.
Collapse
|
48
|
Mearing AS, Burkart JM, Dunn J, Street SE, Koops K. The evolutionary drivers of primate scleral coloration. Sci Rep 2022; 12:14119. [PMID: 35982191 PMCID: PMC9388658 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-022-18275-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/22/2022] [Accepted: 08/08/2022] [Indexed: 11/09/2022] Open
Abstract
The drivers of divergent scleral morphologies in primates are currently unclear, though white sclerae are often assumed to underlie human hyper-cooperative behaviours. Humans are unusual in possessing depigmented sclerae whereas many other extant primates, including the closely-related chimpanzee, possess dark scleral pigment. Here, we use phylogenetic generalized least squares (PGLS) analyses with previously generated species-level scores of proactive prosociality, social tolerance (both n = 15 primate species), and conspecific lethal aggression (n = 108 primate species) to provide the first quantitative, comparative test of three existing hypotheses. The 'self-domestication' and 'cooperative eye' explanations predict white sclerae to be associated with cooperative, rather than competitive, environments. The 'gaze camouflage' hypothesis predicts that dark scleral pigment functions as gaze direction camouflage in competitive social environments. Notably, the experimental evidence that non-human primates draw social information from conspecific eye movements is unclear, with the latter two hypotheses having recently been challenged. Here, we show that white sclerae in primates are associated with increased cooperative behaviours whereas dark sclerae are associated with reduced cooperative behaviours and increased conspecific lethal violence. These results are consistent with all three hypotheses of scleral evolution, suggesting that primate scleral morphologies evolve in relation to variation in social environment.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alex S Mearing
- Department of Archaeology, Fitzwilliam Street, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, CB2 1QH, UK.
| | - Judith M Burkart
- Department of Anthropology, University of Zurich, 8057, Zurich, Switzerland
| | - Jacob Dunn
- Department of Archaeology, Fitzwilliam Street, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, CB2 1QH, UK.,School of Life Sciences, Anglia Ruskin University, Cambridge, CB1 1PT, UK.,Department of Cognitive Biology, University of Vienna, 1090, Vienna, Austria
| | - Sally E Street
- Department of Anthropology, University of Durham, Durham, DH1 3LE, UK
| | - Kathelijne Koops
- Department of Archaeology, Fitzwilliam Street, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, CB2 1QH, UK.,Department of Anthropology, University of Zurich, 8057, Zurich, Switzerland
| |
Collapse
|
49
|
Kramer SS, Russell R. A Novel Human Sex Difference: Male Sclera Are Redder and Yellower than Female Sclera. ARCHIVES OF SEXUAL BEHAVIOR 2022; 51:2733-2740. [PMID: 35507123 DOI: 10.1007/s10508-022-02304-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/11/2020] [Revised: 01/28/2022] [Accepted: 01/29/2022] [Indexed: 06/14/2023]
Abstract
In a seminal study, Kobayashi and Kohshima (1997) found that the human sclera-the white of the eye-is unique among primates for its whitish color, and subsequent work has supported the notion that this coloration underlies the human ability to gaze follow. Kobayashi and Kohshima also claimed that there is no significant sex difference in sclera color, though no data were presented to support the claim. We investigated sclera color in a standardized sample of faces varying in age and sex, presenting the first data comparing male and female sclera color. Our data support the claim that indeed there is a sex difference in sclera color, with male sclera being yellower and redder than female sclera. We also replicated earlier findings that female sclera vary in color across the adult lifespan, with older sclera appearing yellower, redder, and slightly darker than younger sclera, and we extended these findings to male sclera. Finally, in two experiments we found evidence that people use sclera color as a cue for making judgements of facial femininity or masculinity. When sclera were manipulated to appear redder and yellower, faces were perceived as more masculine, but were perceived as more feminine when sclera were manipulated to appear less red and yellow. Though people are typically unaware of the sex difference in sclera color, these findings suggest that people nevertheless use the difference as a visual cue when perceiving sex-related traits from the face.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sarah S Kramer
- Department of Psychology, Gettysburg College, Gettysburg, PA, 17325, USA
| | - Richard Russell
- Department of Psychology, Gettysburg College, Gettysburg, PA, 17325, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
50
|
Luft CDB, Zioga I, Giannopoulos A, Di Bona G, Binetti N, Civilini A, Latora V, Mareschal I. Social synchronization of brain activity increases during eye-contact. Commun Biol 2022; 5:412. [PMID: 35508588 PMCID: PMC9068716 DOI: 10.1038/s42003-022-03352-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/16/2021] [Accepted: 04/11/2022] [Indexed: 11/23/2022] Open
Abstract
Humans make eye-contact to extract information about other people’s mental states, recruiting dedicated brain networks that process information about the self and others. Recent studies show that eye-contact increases the synchronization between two brains but do not consider its effects on activity within single brains. Here we investigate how eye-contact affects the frequency and direction of the synchronization within and between two brains and the corresponding network characteristics. We also evaluate the functional relevance of eye-contact networks by comparing inter- and intra-brain networks of friends vs. strangers and the direction of synchronization between leaders and followers. We show that eye-contact increases higher inter- and intra-brain synchronization in the gamma frequency band. Network analysis reveals that some brain areas serve as hubs linking within- and between-brain networks. During eye-contact, friends show higher inter-brain synchronization than strangers. Dyads with clear leader/follower roles demonstrate higher synchronization from leader to follower in the alpha frequency band. Importantly, eye-contact affects synchronization between brains more than within brains, demonstrating that eye-contact is an inherently social signal. Future work should elucidate the causal mechanisms behind eye-contact induced synchronization. Friends making eye-contact have higher inter-brain synchronization than strangers. Eye-contact affects neural synchronization between brains more than within a brain, highlighting that eye-contact is an inherently social signal.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Caroline Di Bernardi Luft
- School of Biological and Behavioural Sciences, Queen Mary, University of London, London, E1 4NS, United Kingdom.
| | - Ioanna Zioga
- School of Biological and Behavioural Sciences, Queen Mary, University of London, London, E1 4NS, United Kingdom.,Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
| | - Anastasios Giannopoulos
- School of Electrical and Computer Engineering, National Technical University of Athens (NTUA), Athens, Greece
| | - Gabriele Di Bona
- School of Mathematical Sciences, Queen Mary University of London, London, E1 4NS, United Kingdom
| | - Nicola Binetti
- School of Biological and Behavioural Sciences, Queen Mary, University of London, London, E1 4NS, United Kingdom
| | - Andrea Civilini
- School of Mathematical Sciences, Queen Mary University of London, London, E1 4NS, United Kingdom
| | - Vito Latora
- School of Mathematical Sciences, Queen Mary University of London, London, E1 4NS, United Kingdom.,Dipartimento di Fisica ed Astronomia, Università di Catania and INFN, I-95123, Catania, Italy.,The Alan Turing Institute, The British Library, London, NW1 2DB, United Kingdom.,Complexity Science Hub, Josefstäadter Strasse 39, A 1080, Vienna, Austria
| | - Isabelle Mareschal
- School of Biological and Behavioural Sciences, Queen Mary, University of London, London, E1 4NS, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|