1
|
Lou H, Pilz KS, Lorist MM. Effects of cue location and object orientation on object-based attention. Vision Res 2025; 226:108521. [PMID: 39566305 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2024.108521] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/30/2023] [Revised: 09/11/2024] [Accepted: 11/08/2024] [Indexed: 11/22/2024]
Abstract
Spatial cues have previously been found to facilitate information processing not only at cued locations but also within cued objects, so-called object-based attention. We used different variants of the classic two-rectangle paradigm to investigate the interaction of cue location and object orientation on object-based attentional effects. First, we re-analyzed data from a prior study using the classical two-rectangle paradigm. We expected faster attentional shifts along the horizontal compared to the vertical meridian. Results confirmed that cue location and rectangle orientation interactively influence object-based attention, with horizontal objects combined with upper left visual field cues eliciting faster responses than other conditions. In Experiment 2, we removed object contours to examine the benefits of shifting attention based purely on cue location. The results showed that these differences remained, indicating that attentional shifts are not solely guided by object contours. In Experiment 3, we added a third possible target location to the original two-rectangle experiment to examine whether attentional shifts followed a predictable pattern across the stimulus display. Despite faster responses to cued targets, no consistent and organized visual search pattern was observed when participants searched for targets at invalidly cued locations. Our findings suggest that object-based effects are influenced by both cue location and the orientation of attentional shifts. Shifts from left to right in the upper visual field consistently demonstrated significant benefits, whereas the benefits of vertical shifts were less consistent across experiments.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Hao Lou
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Groningen.
| | - Karin S Pilz
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Groningen; Cito Institute for Test Development, Arnhem, Netherlands
| | - Monicque M Lorist
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Groningen; Department of Biomedical Sciences of Cells and Systems, Cognitive Neuroscience Center, University of Groningen
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Pogorzelec P, Dybiec B. Role of long jumps in Lévy noise-induced multimodality. CHAOS (WOODBURY, N.Y.) 2024; 34:063136. [PMID: 38885068 DOI: 10.1063/5.0206970] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/05/2024] [Accepted: 05/24/2024] [Indexed: 06/20/2024]
Abstract
Lévy noise is a paradigmatic noise used to describe out-of-equilibrium systems. Typically, properties of Lévy noise driven systems are very different from their Gaussian white noise driven counterparts. In particular, under action of Lévy noise, stationary states in single-well, super-harmonic, potentials are no longer unimodal. Typically, they are bimodal; however, for fine-tuned potentials, the number of modes can be further increased. The multimodality arises as a consequence of the competition between long displacements induced by the non-equilibrium stochastic driving and action of the deterministic force. Here, we explore robustness of bimodality in the quartic potential under action of the Lévy noise. We explore various scenarios of bounding long jumps and assess their ability to weaken and destroy multimodality. In general, we demonstrate that despite its robustness it is possible to destroy the bimodality, however it requires drastic reduction in the length of noise-induced jumps.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Przemysław Pogorzelec
- Doctoral School of Exact and Natural Sciences, Faculty of Physics, Astronomy and Applied Computer Science, Jagiellonian University, Łojasiewicza 11, 30-348 Kraków, Poland
| | - Bartłomiej Dybiec
- Institute of Theoretical Physics and Mark Kac Center for Complex Systems Research, Faculty of Physics, Astronomy and Applied Computer Science, Jagiellonian University, Łojasiewicza 11, 30-348 Kraków, Poland
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Żbik B, Dybiec B. Lévy flights and Lévy walks under stochastic resetting. Phys Rev E 2024; 109:044147. [PMID: 38755837 DOI: 10.1103/physreve.109.044147] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/27/2023] [Accepted: 03/27/2024] [Indexed: 05/18/2024]
Abstract
Stochastic resetting is a protocol of starting anew, which can be used to facilitate the escape kinetics. We demonstrate that restarting can accelerate the escape kinetics from a finite interval restricted by two absorbing boundaries also in the presence of heavy-tailed, Lévy-type, α-stable noise. However, the width of the domain where resetting is beneficial depends on the value of the stability index α determining the power-law decay of the jump length distribution. For heavier (smaller α) distributions, the domain becomes narrower in comparison to lighter tails. Additionally, we explore connections between Lévy flights (LFs) and Lévy walks (LWs) in the presence of stochastic resetting. First of all, we show that for Lévy walks, the stochastic resetting can also be beneficial in the domain where the coefficient of variation is smaller than 1. Moreover, we demonstrate that in the domain where LWs are characterized by a finite mean jump duration (length), with the increasing width of the interval, the LWs start to share similarities with LFs under stochastic resetting.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Bartosz Żbik
- Faculty of Physics, Astronomy and Applied Computer Science, Jagiellonian University, Łojasiewicza 11, 30-348 Kraków, Poland
| | - Bartłomiej Dybiec
- Institute of Theoretical Physics and Mark Kac Center for Complex Systems Research, Faculty of Physics, Astronomy and Applied Computer Science, Jagiellonian University, Łojasiewicza 11, 30-348 Kraków, Poland
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Harris DJ, Wilson MR, Jones MI, de Burgh T, Mundy D, Arthur T, Olonilua M, Vine SJ. An investigation of feed-forward and feedback eye movement training in immersive virtual reality. J Eye Mov Res 2023; 15:10.16910/jemr.15.3.7. [PMID: 38978970 PMCID: PMC11229047 DOI: 10.16910/jemr.15.3.7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 07/10/2024] Open
Abstract
The control of eye gaze is critical to the execution of many skills. The observation that task experts in many domains exhibit more efficient control of eye gaze than novices has led to the development of gaze training interventions that teach these behaviours. We aimed to extend this literature by i) examining the relative benefits of feed-forward (observing an expert's eye movements) versus feed-back (observing your own eye movements) training, and ii) automating this training within virtual reality. Serving personnel from the British Army and Royal Navy were randomised to either feed-forward or feed-back training within a virtual reality simulation of a room search and clearance task. Eye movement metrics - including visual search, saccade direction, and entropy - were recorded to quantify the efficiency of visual search behaviours. Feed-forward and feed-back eye movement training produced distinct learning benefits, but both accelerated the development of efficient gaze behaviours. However, we found no evidence that these more efficient search behaviours transferred to better decision making in the room clearance task. Our results suggest integrating eye movement training principles within virtual reality training simulations may be effective, but further work is needed to understand the learning mechanisms.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- David J Harris
- School of Public Health and Sport Sciences, University of Exeter, UK
| | - Mark R Wilson
- School of Public Health and Sport Sciences, University of Exeter, UK
| | - Martin I Jones
- Defence Science and Technology Laboratory, Salisbury, UK
| | | | | | - Tom Arthur
- School of Public Health and Sport Sciences, University of Exeter, UK
| | | | - Samuel J Vine
- School of Public Health and Sport Sciences, University of Exeter, UK
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Capała K, Dybiec B. Drifted escape from the finite interval. Phys Rev E 2022; 105:064125. [PMID: 35854586 DOI: 10.1103/physreve.105.064125] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/05/2022] [Accepted: 06/09/2022] [Indexed: 06/15/2023]
Abstract
Properties of the noise-driven escape kinetics are mainly determined by the stochastic component of the system dynamics. Nevertheless, the escape dynamics is also sensitive to deterministic forces. Here, we are exploring properties of the overdamped drifted escape from finite intervals under the action of symmetric α-stable noises. We show that the properly rescaled mean first passage time follows the universal pattern as a function of the generalized Pécklet number, which can be used to efficiently discriminate between domains where drift or random force dominate. Stochastic driving of the α-stable type is capable of diminishing the significance of the drift in the regime when the drift prevails.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Karol Capała
- Institute of Theoretical Physics and Mark Kac Center for Complex Systems Research, Jagiellonian University, Łojasiewicza 11, 30-348 Kraków, Poland
| | - Bartłomiej Dybiec
- Institute of Theoretical Physics and Mark Kac Center for Complex Systems Research, Jagiellonian University, Łojasiewicza 11, 30-348 Kraków, Poland
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Stosic D, Stosic D, Vodenska I, Stanley HE, Stosic T. A New Look at Calendar Anomalies: Multifractality and Day-of-the-Week Effect. ENTROPY 2022; 24:e24040562. [PMID: 35455225 PMCID: PMC9031867 DOI: 10.3390/e24040562] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/06/2022] [Revised: 02/18/2022] [Accepted: 04/13/2022] [Indexed: 12/04/2022]
Abstract
Stock markets can become inefficient due to calendar anomalies known as the day-of-the-week effect. Calendar anomalies are well known in the financial literature, but the phenomena remain to be explored in econophysics. This paper uses multifractal analysis to evaluate if the temporal dynamics of market returns also exhibit calendar anomalies such as day-of-the-week effects. We apply multifractal detrended fluctuation analysis (MF-DFA) to the daily returns of market indices worldwide for each day of the week. Our results indicate that distinct multifractal properties characterize individual days of the week. Monday returns tend to exhibit more persistent behavior and richer multifractal structures than other day-resolved returns. Shuffling the series reveals that multifractality arises from a broad probability density function and long-term correlations. The time-dependent multifractal analysis shows that the Monday returns’ multifractal spectra are much wider than those of other days. This behavior is especially persistent during financial crises. The presence of day-of-the-week effects in multifractal dynamics of market returns motivates further research on calendar anomalies for distinct market regimes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Darko Stosic
- Centro de Informática, Universidade Federal de Pernambuco, Av. Luiz Freire s/n, Recife 50670-901, PE, Brazil; (D.S.); (D.S.)
| | - Dusan Stosic
- Centro de Informática, Universidade Federal de Pernambuco, Av. Luiz Freire s/n, Recife 50670-901, PE, Brazil; (D.S.); (D.S.)
| | - Irena Vodenska
- Department of Administrative Sciences, Metropolitan College, Boston University, 1010 Commonwealth Avenue, Boston, MA 02215, USA
- Correspondence:
| | - H. Eugene Stanley
- Center for Polymer Studies, Department of Physics, Boston University, 590 Commonwealth Avenue, Boston, MA 02215, USA;
| | - Tatijana Stosic
- Departamento de Estatística e Informática, Universidade Federal Rural de Pernambuco, Rua Dom Manoel de Medeiros s/n, Dois Irmãos, Recife 52171-900, PE, Brazil;
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Torres D, Sena WR, Carmona HA, Moreira AA, Makse HA, Andrade JS. Eye-tracking as a proxy for coherence and complexity of texts. PLoS One 2021; 16:e0260236. [PMID: 34898624 PMCID: PMC8668102 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0260236] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/01/2021] [Accepted: 11/04/2021] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Abstract
Reading is a complex cognitive process that involves primary oculomotor function and high-level activities like attention focus and language processing. When we read, our eyes move by primary physiological functions while responding to language-processing demands. In fact, the eyes perform discontinuous twofold movements, namely, successive long jumps (saccades) interposed by small steps (fixations) in which the gaze “scans” confined locations. It is only through the fixations that information is effectively captured for brain processing. Since individuals can express similar as well as entirely different opinions about a given text, it is therefore expected that the form, content and style of a text could induce different eye-movement patterns among people. A question that naturally arises is whether these individuals’ behaviours are correlated, so that eye-tracking while reading can be used as a proxy for text subjective properties. Here we perform a set of eye-tracking experiments with a group of individuals reading different types of texts, including children stories, random word generated texts and excerpts from literature work. In parallel, an extensive Internet survey was conducted for categorizing these texts in terms of their complexity and coherence, considering a large number of individuals selected according to different ages, gender and levels of education. The computational analysis of the fixation maps obtained from the gaze trajectories of the subjects for a given text reveals that the average “magnetization” of the fixation configurations correlates strongly with their complexity observed in the survey. Moreover, we perform a thermodynamic analysis using the Maximum-Entropy Model and find that coherent texts were closer to their corresponding “critical points” than non-coherent ones, as computed from the Pairwise Maximum-Entropy method, suggesting that different texts may induce distinct cohesive reading activities.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Débora Torres
- Departamento de Física, Universidade Federal do Ceará, Fortaleza, Ceará, Brazil
| | - Wagner R. Sena
- Departamento de Física, Universidade Federal do Ceará, Fortaleza, Ceará, Brazil
| | - Humberto A. Carmona
- Departamento de Física, Universidade Federal do Ceará, Fortaleza, Ceará, Brazil
| | - André A. Moreira
- Departamento de Física, Universidade Federal do Ceará, Fortaleza, Ceará, Brazil
| | - Hernán A. Makse
- Levich Institute and Physics Department, The City College of New York, New York City, New York, United States of America
| | - José S. Andrade
- Departamento de Física, Universidade Federal do Ceará, Fortaleza, Ceará, Brazil
- * E-mail:
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Suman AA, Russo C, Carrigan A, Nalepka P, Liquet-Weiland B, Newport RA, Kumari P, Di Ieva A. Spatial and time domain analysis of eye-tracking data during screening of brain magnetic resonance images. PLoS One 2021; 16:e0260717. [PMID: 34855867 PMCID: PMC8639086 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0260717] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/30/2021] [Accepted: 11/15/2021] [Indexed: 12/01/2022] Open
Abstract
INTRODUCTION Eye-tracking research has been widely used in radiology applications. Prior studies exclusively analysed either temporal or spatial eye-tracking features, both of which alone do not completely characterise the spatiotemporal dynamics of radiologists' gaze features. PURPOSE Our research aims to quantify human visual search dynamics in both domains during brain stimuli screening to explore the relationship between reader characteristics and stimuli complexity. The methodology can be used to discover strategies to aid trainee radiologists in identifying pathology, and to select regions of interest for machine vision applications. METHOD The study was performed using eye-tracking data 5 seconds in duration from 57 readers (15 Brain-experts, 11 Other-experts, 5 Registrars and 26 Naïves) for 40 neuroradiological images as stimuli (i.e., 20 normal and 20 pathological brain MRIs). The visual scanning patterns were analysed by calculating the fractal dimension (FD) and Hurst exponent (HE) using re-scaled range (R/S) and detrended fluctuation analysis (DFA) methods. The FD was used to measure the spatial geometrical complexity of the gaze patterns, and the HE analysis was used to measure participants' focusing skill. The focusing skill is referred to persistence/anti-persistence of the participants' gaze on the stimulus over time. Pathological and normal stimuli were analysed separately both at the "First Second" and full "Five Seconds" viewing duration. RESULTS All experts were more focused and a had higher visual search complexity compared to Registrars and Naïves. This was seen in both the pathological and normal stimuli in the first and five second analyses. The Brain-experts subgroup was shown to achieve better focusing skill than Other-experts due to their domain specific expertise. Indeed, the FDs found when viewing pathological stimuli were higher than those in normal ones. Viewing normal stimuli resulted in an increase of FD found in five second data, unlike pathological stimuli, which did not change. In contrast to the FDs, the scanpath HEs of pathological and normal stimuli were similar. However, participants' gaze was more focused for "Five Seconds" than "First Second" data. CONCLUSIONS The HE analysis of the scanpaths belonging to all experts showed that they have greater focus than Registrars and Naïves. This may be related to their higher visual search complexity than non-experts due to their training and expertise.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Abdulla Al Suman
- Computational NeuroSurgery (CNS) Lab, Faculty of Medicine, Health, and Human Sciences, Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia
| | - Carlo Russo
- Computational NeuroSurgery (CNS) Lab, Faculty of Medicine, Health, and Human Sciences, Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia
| | - Ann Carrigan
- School of Psychological Sciences, Faculty of Medicine, Health, and Human Sciences, Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia
- Centre for Elite Performance, Expertise and Training, Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia
| | - Patrick Nalepka
- School of Psychological Sciences, Faculty of Medicine, Health, and Human Sciences, Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia
- Centre for Elite Performance, Expertise and Training, Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia
| | - Benoit Liquet-Weiland
- Department of Mathematics and Statistics, Faculty of Science and Engineering, Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia
| | - Robert Ahadizad Newport
- Computational NeuroSurgery (CNS) Lab, Faculty of Medicine, Health, and Human Sciences, Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia
| | - Poonam Kumari
- Computational NeuroSurgery (CNS) Lab, Faculty of Medicine, Health, and Human Sciences, Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia
| | - Antonio Di Ieva
- Computational NeuroSurgery (CNS) Lab, Faculty of Medicine, Health, and Human Sciences, Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia
- Centre for Elite Performance, Expertise and Training, Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Structure of variability in scanning movement predicts braille reading performance in children. Sci Rep 2021; 11:7182. [PMID: 33785818 PMCID: PMC8009883 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-021-86674-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/16/2021] [Accepted: 03/19/2021] [Indexed: 02/01/2023] Open
Abstract
Among children learning to read braille, we asked whether the quantitative kinematics of scanning movements of the reading finger would be related to the proficiency of braille reading. Over a period of 12 months, we recorded the position and orientation of the reading fingers of eight congenitally or early blind children. We found that the strength of long-range power-law temporal correlations in the velocity fluctuations increased with performance in braille reading. In addition, we found that the variability of the angular orientation of the reading finger that affects the contact region on the fingerpad was negatively related to braille reading performance. These results confirm that the quantitative kinematics of finger scanning movements were related to functional performance in braille reading. The results add to the growing body of evidence that long-range temporal correlations in exploratory behavior can predict perceptual performance, and that scanning movements that center important tactile information on the small, high resolution area contribute to the pickup of information.
Collapse
|
10
|
Thornton IM. A Search Advantage for Horizontal Targets in Dynamic Displays. Iperception 2021; 12:20416695211004616. [PMID: 33912338 PMCID: PMC8047869 DOI: 10.1177/20416695211004616] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/15/2020] [Accepted: 03/02/2021] [Indexed: 11/16/2022] Open
Abstract
Several lines of evidence point to the existence of a visual processing advantage for horizontal over vertical orientations. We investigated whether such a horizontal advantage exists in the context of top-down visual search. Inspired by change detection studies, we created displays where a dynamic target -- a horizontal or a vertical group of five dots that changed contrast synchronously -- was embedded within a randomly flickering grid of dots. The display size (total dots) varied across trials, and the orientation of the target was constant within interleaved blocks. As expected, search was slow and inefficient. Importantly, participants were almost a second faster finding horizontal compared to vertical targets. They were also more efficient and more accurate during horizontal search. Such findings establish that the attentional templates thought to guide search for known targets can exhibit strong orientation anisotropies. We discuss possible underlying mechanisms and how these might be explored in future studies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ian M. Thornton
- Department of Cognitive Science, Faculty of Media and Knowledge Sciences, University of Malta, Msida, Malta
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Capała K, Dybiec B. Underdamped, anomalous kinetics in double-well potentials. Phys Rev E 2020; 102:052123. [PMID: 33327158 DOI: 10.1103/physreve.102.052123] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/23/2020] [Accepted: 11/04/2020] [Indexed: 06/12/2023]
Abstract
The noise-driven motion in a bistable potential acts as the archetypal model of various physical phenomena. Here, we contrast properties of the overdamped escape dynamics with the full (underdamped) dynamics. In the weak noise limit, for the overdamped particle driven by nonequilibrium, α-stable noise the ratio of forward to backward transition rates depends only on the width of a potential barrier separating both minima. Using analytical and numerical methods, we show that in the regime of full dynamics, contrary to the overdamped case, the ratio of transition rates depends on both the widths and the heights of the potential barrier separating minima of the double-well potential. The derived analytical formula for the ratio of transition rates is corroborated by extensive numerical simulations. Results of numerical simulations follow especially well the analytical predictions in the weak noise limit when the most probable escape scenario is via a single, strong, noise kick, which is sufficient to induce a quasideterministic transition over the potential barrier. Such an escape trajectory can be analyzed in terms of the instantaneous velocity, which is fully characterized by its density function, which is of the same type as the probability density underlying the noise distribution.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Karol Capała
- Institute of Theoretical Physics and Mark Kac Center for Complex Systems Research, Jagiellonian University, ul. St. Łojasiewicza 11, 30-348 Kraków, Poland
| | - Bartłomiej Dybiec
- Institute of Theoretical Physics and Mark Kac Center for Complex Systems Research, Jagiellonian University, ul. St. Łojasiewicza 11, 30-348 Kraków, Poland
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Zhang L, Yu F, Hu Q, Qiao Y, Xuan R, Ji G, Zhu C, Cai C, Wang K. Effects of SSRI Antidepressants on Attentional Bias toward Emotional Scenes in First-Episode Depressive Patients: Evidence from an Eye-Tracking Study. Psychiatry Investig 2020; 17:871-879. [PMID: 32933239 PMCID: PMC7538251 DOI: 10.30773/pi.2019.0345] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/02/2020] [Accepted: 06/07/2020] [Indexed: 12/31/2022] Open
Abstract
OBJECTIVE Attentional biases toward emotional scenes may represent vulnerability and maintenance factors in depression. Antidepressant therapy may improve cognitive function and reduce depression, and is considered as the mechanism of action of antidepressants. Therefore, we conducted an eye-tracking test to examine whether selective serotonin re-uptake inhibitor (SSRI) antidepressants can reduce negative attentional biases and elicit clinical responses in depression. METHODS Twenty first-episode depressive patients freely viewed three types of pictures that depicted different emotional scenes (i.e., positive-control, neutral-control, and negative-control) for 4,000 ms while their eye movements were monitored. The attentional bias to different emotional scenes was assessed before and after eight weeks of SSRI treatment using the eye-tracking method. The control group included a group of healthy individuals. RESULTS The results revealed that first-episode depressive patients oriented their gaze more frequently to negative images and less to happy images, compared to controls. Importantly, the attentional bias in depressive patients was regulated after eight weeks of SSRI treatment. Patients showed an increased tendency to fixate on positive images and a decreased tendency to focus on negative images. CONCLUSION This suggests that SSRI antidepressants decrease vulnerability to negative images, while having an effect on attention in respect to positive images.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lei Zhang
- Department of Medical Psychology, Chaohu Clinical Medical College, Anhui Medical University, Hefei, China.,Department of Neurology, the First Affiliated Hospital of Anhui Medical University, Hefei, China.,Anhui Province Key Laboratory of Cognition and Neuropsychiatric Disorders, Hefei, China.,Collaborative Innovation Center for Neuropsychiatric Disorders and Mental Health, Hefei, China
| | - Fengqiong Yu
- Department of Medical Psychology, Chaohu Clinical Medical College, Anhui Medical University, Hefei, China.,Department of Neurology, the First Affiliated Hospital of Anhui Medical University, Hefei, China.,Anhui Province Key Laboratory of Cognition and Neuropsychiatric Disorders, Hefei, China.,Collaborative Innovation Center for Neuropsychiatric Disorders and Mental Health, Hefei, China
| | - Qian Hu
- Department of Medical Psychology, Chaohu Clinical Medical College, Anhui Medical University, Hefei, China.,Department of Neurology, the First Affiliated Hospital of Anhui Medical University, Hefei, China.,Anhui Province Key Laboratory of Cognition and Neuropsychiatric Disorders, Hefei, China.,Collaborative Innovation Center for Neuropsychiatric Disorders and Mental Health, Hefei, China
| | - Yuxi Qiao
- Department of Medical Psychology, Chaohu Clinical Medical College, Anhui Medical University, Hefei, China.,Department of Neurology, the First Affiliated Hospital of Anhui Medical University, Hefei, China.,Anhui Province Key Laboratory of Cognition and Neuropsychiatric Disorders, Hefei, China.,Collaborative Innovation Center for Neuropsychiatric Disorders and Mental Health, Hefei, China
| | - Rongrong Xuan
- Department of Medical Psychology, Chaohu Clinical Medical College, Anhui Medical University, Hefei, China.,Department of Neurology, the First Affiliated Hospital of Anhui Medical University, Hefei, China.,Anhui Province Key Laboratory of Cognition and Neuropsychiatric Disorders, Hefei, China.,Collaborative Innovation Center for Neuropsychiatric Disorders and Mental Health, Hefei, China
| | - Gongjun Ji
- Department of Medical Psychology, Chaohu Clinical Medical College, Anhui Medical University, Hefei, China.,Department of Neurology, the First Affiliated Hospital of Anhui Medical University, Hefei, China.,Anhui Province Key Laboratory of Cognition and Neuropsychiatric Disorders, Hefei, China.,Collaborative Innovation Center for Neuropsychiatric Disorders and Mental Health, Hefei, China
| | - Chunyan Zhu
- Department of Medical Psychology, Chaohu Clinical Medical College, Anhui Medical University, Hefei, China.,Department of Neurology, the First Affiliated Hospital of Anhui Medical University, Hefei, China.,Anhui Province Key Laboratory of Cognition and Neuropsychiatric Disorders, Hefei, China.,Collaborative Innovation Center for Neuropsychiatric Disorders and Mental Health, Hefei, China
| | | | - Kai Wang
- Department of Neurology, the First Affiliated Hospital of Anhui Medical University, Hefei, China.,Anhui Province Key Laboratory of Cognition and Neuropsychiatric Disorders, Hefei, China.,Collaborative Innovation Center for Neuropsychiatric Disorders and Mental Health, Hefei, China
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
Capała K, Dybiec B, Gudowska-Nowak E. Nonlinear friction in underdamped anharmonic stochastic oscillators. CHAOS (WOODBURY, N.Y.) 2020; 30:073140. [PMID: 32752631 DOI: 10.1063/5.0007581] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/14/2020] [Accepted: 07/06/2020] [Indexed: 06/11/2023]
Abstract
Non-equilibrium stationary states of overdamped anharmonic stochastic oscillators driven by Lévy noise are typically multimodal. The very same situation is recorded for an underdamped Lévy noise-driven motion in single-well potentials with linear friction. Within the current article, we relax the assumption that the friction experienced by a particle is linear. Using computer simulations, we study underdamped motions in single-well potentials in the regime of nonlinear friction. We demonstrate that it is relatively easy to observe multimodality in the velocity distribution as it is determined by the friction itself and it is the same as the multimodality in the overdamped case with the analogous deterministic force. Contrary to the velocity marginal density, it is more difficult to induce multimodality in the position. Nevertheless, for a fine-tuned nonlinear friction, the spatial multimodality can be recorded.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Karol Capała
- Institute of Theoretical Physics and Mark Kac Center for Complex Systems Research, Jagiellonian University, ul. St. Łojasiewicza 11, 30-348 Kraków, Poland
| | - Bartłomiej Dybiec
- Institute of Theoretical Physics and Mark Kac Center for Complex Systems Research, Jagiellonian University, ul. St. Łojasiewicza 11, 30-348 Kraków, Poland
| | - Ewa Gudowska-Nowak
- Institute of Theoretical Physics and Mark Kac Center for Complex Systems Research, Jagiellonian University, ul. St. Łojasiewicza 11, 30-348 Kraków, Poland
| |
Collapse
|
14
|
Capała K, Dybiec B, Gudowska-Nowak E. Peculiarities of escape kinetics in the presence of athermal noises. CHAOS (WOODBURY, N.Y.) 2020; 30:013127. [PMID: 32013513 DOI: 10.1063/1.5126263] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/31/2019] [Accepted: 12/19/2019] [Indexed: 06/10/2023]
Abstract
Stochastic evolution of various dynamic systems and reaction networks is commonly described in terms of noise assisted escape of an overdamped particle from a potential well, as devised by the paradigmatic Langevin equation in which additive Gaussian stochastic force reproduces effects of thermal fluctuations from the reservoir. When implemented for systems close to equilibrium, the approach correctly explains the emergence of the Boltzmann distribution for the ensemble of trajectories generated by the Langevin equation and relates the intensity of the noise strength to the mobility. This scenario can be further generalized to include effects of non-Gaussian, burstlike forcing modeled by Lévy noise. In this case, however, the pulsatile additive noise cannot be treated as the internal (thermal) since the relation between the strength of the friction and variance of the noise is violated. Heavy tails of Lévy noise distributions not only facilitate escape kinetics, but also, more importantly, change the escape protocol by altering the final stationary state to a non-Boltzmann, nonequilibrium form. As a result, contrary to the kinetics induced by a Gaussian white noise, escape rates in environments with Lévy noise are determined not by the barrier height, but instead by the barrier width. We further discuss consequences of simultaneous action of thermal and Lévy noises on statistics of passage times and population of reactants in double-well potentials.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Karol Capała
- Marian Smoluchowski Institute of Physics and Mark Kac Center for Complex Systems Research, Jagiellonian University, ul. St. Łojasiewicza 11, 30-348 Kraków, Poland
| | - Bartłomiej Dybiec
- Marian Smoluchowski Institute of Physics and Mark Kac Center for Complex Systems Research, Jagiellonian University, ul. St. Łojasiewicza 11, 30-348 Kraków, Poland
| | - Ewa Gudowska-Nowak
- Marian Smoluchowski Institute of Physics and Mark Kac Center for Complex Systems Research, Jagiellonian University, ul. St. Łojasiewicza 11, 30-348 Kraków, Poland
| |
Collapse
|
15
|
|
16
|
Del Punta JA, Rodriguez KV, Gasaneo G, Bouzat S. Models for saccadic motion and postsaccadic oscillations. Phys Rev E 2019; 99:032422. [PMID: 30999401 DOI: 10.1103/physreve.99.032422] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/26/2018] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
Abstract
In a recent letter [S. Bouzat et al., Phys. Rev. Lett. 120, 178101 (2018)10.1103/PhysRevLett.120.178101], a mathematical model for eyeball and pupil motion was developed allowing for the understanding of the postsaccadic oscillations (PSO) as inertial effects. The model assumes that the inner part of the iris, which defines the pupil, moves driven by inertial forces induced by the eyeball rotation, in addition to viscous and elastic forces. Among other achievements, the model correctly reproduces eye-tracking experiments concerning PSO profiles and their dependence on the saccade size. In this paper we propose various extensions of the mentioned model, we provide analytical solutions, and we perform an exhaustive analysis of the dynamics. In particular, we consider a more general time dependence for the eyeball velocity enabling the description of saccades with vanishing initial acceleration. Moreover, we give the analytical solution in terms of hypergeometric functions for the constant parameter version of the model and we provide particular expressions for some cases of interest. We also introduce a new version of the model with inhomogeneous viscosity that can improve the fitting of the experimental results. Our analysis of the solutions explores the dependence of the PSO profiles on the system parameters for varying saccade sizes. We show that the PSO emerge in critical-like ways when parameters such as the elasticity of the iris, the global eyeball velocity, or the saccade size vary. Moreover, we find that the PSO profiles with the first overshoot smaller than the second one, which are usually observed in experiments, can be associated to parameter regions close to criticality.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- J A Del Punta
- Neufisur-Departamento de Física, Universidad Nacional del Sur-IFISUR, (8000) Bahía Blanca, Argentina.,Departamento de Matemática, Universidad Nacional del Sur, (8000) Bahía Blanca, Argentina
| | - K V Rodriguez
- Neufisur-Departamento de Física, Universidad Nacional del Sur-IFISUR, (8000) Bahía Blanca, Argentina.,CINA-Centro Integral de Neurociencias Aplicadas, (8000) Bahía Blanca, Argentina
| | - G Gasaneo
- Neufisur-Departamento de Física, Universidad Nacional del Sur-IFISUR, (8000) Bahía Blanca, Argentina.,CINA-Centro Integral de Neurociencias Aplicadas, (8000) Bahía Blanca, Argentina
| | - S Bouzat
- Centro Atómico Bariloche (CNEA), Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas, Av. E. Bustillo 9500 R8402AGP San Carlos de Bariloche Río Negro, Argentina
| |
Collapse
|
17
|
Asfaw DS, Jones PR, Mönter VM, Smith ND, Crabb DP. Does Glaucoma Alter Eye Movements When Viewing Images of Natural Scenes? A Between-Eye Study. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 2019; 59:3189-3198. [PMID: 29971443 DOI: 10.1167/iovs.18-23779] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
Purpose To investigate whether glaucoma produces measurable changes in eye movements. Methods Fifteen glaucoma patients with asymmetric vision loss (difference in mean deviation [MD] > 6 dB between eyes) were asked to monocularly view 120 images of natural scenes, presented sequentially on a computer monitor. Each image was viewed twice-once each with the better and worse eye. Patients' eye movements were recorded with an Eyelink 1000 eye-tracker. Eye-movement parameters were computed and compared within participants (better eye versus worse eye). These parameters included a novel measure: saccadic reversal rate (SRR), as well as more traditional metrics such as saccade amplitude, fixation counts, fixation duration, and spread of fixation locations (bivariate contour ellipse area [BCEA]). In addition, the associations of these parameters with clinical measures of vision were investigated. Results In the worse eye, saccade amplitude\(\def\upalpha{\unicode[Times]{x3B1}}\)\(\def\upbeta{\unicode[Times]{x3B2}}\)\(\def\upgamma{\unicode[Times]{x3B3}}\)\(\def\updelta{\unicode[Times]{x3B4}}\)\(\def\upvarepsilon{\unicode[Times]{x3B5}}\)\(\def\upzeta{\unicode[Times]{x3B6}}\)\(\def\upeta{\unicode[Times]{x3B7}}\)\(\def\uptheta{\unicode[Times]{x3B8}}\)\(\def\upiota{\unicode[Times]{x3B9}}\)\(\def\upkappa{\unicode[Times]{x3BA}}\)\(\def\uplambda{\unicode[Times]{x3BB}}\)\(\def\upmu{\unicode[Times]{x3BC}}\)\(\def\upnu{\unicode[Times]{x3BD}}\)\(\def\upxi{\unicode[Times]{x3BE}}\)\(\def\upomicron{\unicode[Times]{x3BF}}\)\(\def\uppi{\unicode[Times]{x3C0}}\)\(\def\uprho{\unicode[Times]{x3C1}}\)\(\def\upsigma{\unicode[Times]{x3C3}}\)\(\def\uptau{\unicode[Times]{x3C4}}\)\(\def\upupsilon{\unicode[Times]{x3C5}}\)\(\def\upphi{\unicode[Times]{x3C6}}\)\(\def\upchi{\unicode[Times]{x3C7}}\)\(\def\uppsy{\unicode[Times]{x3C8}}\)\(\def\upomega{\unicode[Times]{x3C9}}\)\(\def\bialpha{\boldsymbol{\alpha}}\)\(\def\bibeta{\boldsymbol{\beta}}\)\(\def\bigamma{\boldsymbol{\gamma}}\)\(\def\bidelta{\boldsymbol{\delta}}\)\(\def\bivarepsilon{\boldsymbol{\varepsilon}}\)\(\def\bizeta{\boldsymbol{\zeta}}\)\(\def\bieta{\boldsymbol{\eta}}\)\(\def\bitheta{\boldsymbol{\theta}}\)\(\def\biiota{\boldsymbol{\iota}}\)\(\def\bikappa{\boldsymbol{\kappa}}\)\(\def\bilambda{\boldsymbol{\lambda}}\)\(\def\bimu{\boldsymbol{\mu}}\)\(\def\binu{\boldsymbol{\nu}}\)\(\def\bixi{\boldsymbol{\xi}}\)\(\def\biomicron{\boldsymbol{\micron}}\)\(\def\bipi{\boldsymbol{\pi}}\)\(\def\birho{\boldsymbol{\rho}}\)\(\def\bisigma{\boldsymbol{\sigma}}\)\(\def\bitau{\boldsymbol{\tau}}\)\(\def\biupsilon{\boldsymbol{\upsilon}}\)\(\def\biphi{\boldsymbol{\phi}}\)\(\def\bichi{\boldsymbol{\chi}}\)\(\def\bipsy{\boldsymbol{\psy}}\)\(\def\biomega{\boldsymbol{\omega}}\)\(\def\bupalpha{\unicode[Times]{x1D6C2}}\)\(\def\bupbeta{\unicode[Times]{x1D6C3}}\)\(\def\bupgamma{\unicode[Times]{x1D6C4}}\)\(\def\bupdelta{\unicode[Times]{x1D6C5}}\)\(\def\bupepsilon{\unicode[Times]{x1D6C6}}\)\(\def\bupvarepsilon{\unicode[Times]{x1D6DC}}\)\(\def\bupzeta{\unicode[Times]{x1D6C7}}\)\(\def\bupeta{\unicode[Times]{x1D6C8}}\)\(\def\buptheta{\unicode[Times]{x1D6C9}}\)\(\def\bupiota{\unicode[Times]{x1D6CA}}\)\(\def\bupkappa{\unicode[Times]{x1D6CB}}\)\(\def\buplambda{\unicode[Times]{x1D6CC}}\)\(\def\bupmu{\unicode[Times]{x1D6CD}}\)\(\def\bupnu{\unicode[Times]{x1D6CE}}\)\(\def\bupxi{\unicode[Times]{x1D6CF}}\)\(\def\bupomicron{\unicode[Times]{x1D6D0}}\)\(\def\buppi{\unicode[Times]{x1D6D1}}\)\(\def\buprho{\unicode[Times]{x1D6D2}}\)\(\def\bupsigma{\unicode[Times]{x1D6D4}}\)\(\def\buptau{\unicode[Times]{x1D6D5}}\)\(\def\bupupsilon{\unicode[Times]{x1D6D6}}\)\(\def\bupphi{\unicode[Times]{x1D6D7}}\)\(\def\bupchi{\unicode[Times]{x1D6D8}}\)\(\def\buppsy{\unicode[Times]{x1D6D9}}\)\(\def\bupomega{\unicode[Times]{x1D6DA}}\)\(\def\bupvartheta{\unicode[Times]{x1D6DD}}\)\(\def\bGamma{\bf{\Gamma}}\)\(\def\bDelta{\bf{\Delta}}\)\(\def\bTheta{\bf{\Theta}}\)\(\def\bLambda{\bf{\Lambda}}\)\(\def\bXi{\bf{\Xi}}\)\(\def\bPi{\bf{\Pi}}\)\(\def\bSigma{\bf{\Sigma}}\)\(\def\bUpsilon{\bf{\Upsilon}}\)\(\def\bPhi{\bf{\Phi}}\)\(\def\bPsi{\bf{\Psi}}\)\(\def\bOmega{\bf{\Omega}}\)\(\def\iGamma{\unicode[Times]{x1D6E4}}\)\(\def\iDelta{\unicode[Times]{x1D6E5}}\)\(\def\iTheta{\unicode[Times]{x1D6E9}}\)\(\def\iLambda{\unicode[Times]{x1D6EC}}\)\(\def\iXi{\unicode[Times]{x1D6EF}}\)\(\def\iPi{\unicode[Times]{x1D6F1}}\)\(\def\iSigma{\unicode[Times]{x1D6F4}}\)\(\def\iUpsilon{\unicode[Times]{x1D6F6}}\)\(\def\iPhi{\unicode[Times]{x1D6F7}}\)\(\def\iPsi{\unicode[Times]{x1D6F9}}\)\(\def\iOmega{\unicode[Times]{x1D6FA}}\)\(\def\biGamma{\unicode[Times]{x1D71E}}\)\(\def\biDelta{\unicode[Times]{x1D71F}}\)\(\def\biTheta{\unicode[Times]{x1D723}}\)\(\def\biLambda{\unicode[Times]{x1D726}}\)\(\def\biXi{\unicode[Times]{x1D729}}\)\(\def\biPi{\unicode[Times]{x1D72B}}\)\(\def\biSigma{\unicode[Times]{x1D72E}}\)\(\def\biUpsilon{\unicode[Times]{x1D730}}\)\(\def\biPhi{\unicode[Times]{x1D731}}\)\(\def\biPsi{\unicode[Times]{x1D733}}\)\(\def\biOmega{\unicode[Times]{x1D734}}\)\((P = 0.012; - 13\% \)) and BCEA \((P = 0.005; - 16\% )\) were smaller, while SRR was greater (\(P = 0.018; + 16\% \)). There was a significant correlation between the intereye difference in BCEA, and differences in MD values (\({\rm{Spearman^{\prime} s}}\ r = 0.65;P = 0.01\)), while differences in SRR were associated with differences in visual acuity (\({\rm{Spearman^{\prime} s}}\ r = 0.64;P = 0.01\)). Furthermore, between-eye differences in BCEA were a significant predictor of between-eye differences in MD: for every 1-dB difference in MD, BCEA reduced by 6.2% (95% confidence interval, 1.6%-10.3%). Conclusions Eye movements are altered by visual field loss, and these changes are related to changes in clinical measures. Eye movements recorded while passively viewing images could potentially be used as biomarkers for visual field damage.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Daniel S Asfaw
- Division of Optometry and Visual Science, School of Health Science, City, University of London, London, United Kingdom
| | - Pete R Jones
- Division of Optometry and Visual Science, School of Health Science, City, University of London, London, United Kingdom
| | - Vera M Mönter
- Division of Optometry and Visual Science, School of Health Science, City, University of London, London, United Kingdom
| | - Nicholas D Smith
- Division of Optometry and Visual Science, School of Health Science, City, University of London, London, United Kingdom
| | - David P Crabb
- Division of Optometry and Visual Science, School of Health Science, City, University of London, London, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
18
|
Savage SW, Potter DD, Tatler BW. The effects of array structure and secondary cognitive task demand on processes of visual search. Vision Res 2018; 153:37-46. [PMID: 30248367 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2018.09.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/20/2018] [Revised: 08/30/2018] [Accepted: 09/05/2018] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
Many aspects of our everyday behaviour require that we search for objects. However, in real situations search is often conducted while internal and external factors compete for our attention resources. Cognitive distraction interferes with our ability to search for targets, increasing search times. Here we consider whether effects of cognitive distraction interfere differentially with three distinct phases of search: initiating search, overtly scanning through items in the display, and verifying that the object is indeed the target of search once it has been fixated. Furthermore, we consider whether strategic components of visual search that emerge when searching items organized into structured arrays are susceptible to cognitive distraction or not. We used Gilchrist & Harvey's (2006) structured and unstructured visual search paradigm with the addition of Savage, Potter, and Tatler's (2013) secondary puzzle task. Cognitive load influenced two phases of search: 1) scanning times and 2) verification times. Under high load, fixation durations were longer and re-fixations of distracters were more common. In terms of scanning strategy, we replicated Gilchrist and Harvey's (2006) findings of more systematic search for structured arrays than unstructured ones. We also found an effect of cognitive load on this aspect of search but only in structured arrays. Our findings suggest that our eyes, by default, produce an autonomous scanning pattern that is modulated but not completely eliminated by secondary cognitive load.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Steven William Savage
- Schepens Eye Research Institute, Massachusetts Eye and Ear, Harvard Medical School, 20 Staniford Street, 02114 Boston, MA, USA.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
19
|
Thermodynamics of Superdiffusion Generated by Lévy-Wiener Fluctuating Forces. ENTROPY 2018; 20:e20090658. [PMID: 33265747 PMCID: PMC7513181 DOI: 10.3390/e20090658] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/05/2018] [Revised: 08/16/2018] [Accepted: 08/29/2018] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
Scale free Lévy motion is a generalized analogue of the Wiener process. Its time derivative extends the notion of “white noise” to non-Gaussian noise sources, and as such, it has been widely used to model natural signal variations described by an overdamped Langevin stochastic differential equation. Here, we consider the dynamics of an archetypal model: a Brownian-like particle is driven by external forces, and noise is represented by uncorrelated Lévy fluctuations. An unperturbed system of that form eventually attains a steady state which is uniquely determined by the set of parameter values. We show that the analyzed Markov process with the stability index α<2 violates the detailed balance, i.e., its stationary state is quantified by a stationary probability density and nonvanishing current. We discuss consequences of the non-Gibbsian character of the stationary state of the system and its impact on the general form of the fluctuation–dissipation theorem derived for weak external forcing.
Collapse
|
20
|
Beltrán J, García-Vázquez MS, Benois-Pineau J, Gutierrez-Robledo LM, Dartigues JF. Computational Techniques for Eye Movements Analysis towards Supporting Early Diagnosis of Alzheimer's Disease: A Review. COMPUTATIONAL AND MATHEMATICAL METHODS IN MEDICINE 2018; 2018:2676409. [PMID: 29887912 PMCID: PMC5985110 DOI: 10.1155/2018/2676409] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/03/2017] [Accepted: 04/03/2018] [Indexed: 12/21/2022]
Abstract
An opportune early diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease (AD) would help to overcome symptoms and improve the quality of life for AD patients. Research studies have identified early manifestations of AD that occur years before the diagnosis. For instance, eye movements of people with AD in different tasks differ from eye movements of control subjects. In this review, we present a summary and evolution of research approaches that use eye tracking technology and computational analysis to measure and compare eye movements under different tasks and experiments. Furthermore, this review is targeted to the feasibility of pioneer work on developing computational tools and techniques to analyze eye movements under naturalistic scenarios. We describe the progress in technology that can enhance the analysis of eye movements everywhere while subjects perform their daily activities and give future research directions to develop tools to support early AD diagnosis through analysis of eye movements.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jessica Beltrán
- Instituto Politécnico Nacional-CITEDI, Tijuana, BC, Mexico
- CONACYT, Ciudad de México, Mexico
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
21
|
Herrmann CJJ, Metzler R, Engbert R. A self-avoiding walk with neural delays as a model of fixational eye movements. Sci Rep 2017; 7:12958. [PMID: 29021548 PMCID: PMC5636902 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-017-13489-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 20] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/20/2017] [Accepted: 09/26/2017] [Indexed: 11/09/2022] Open
Abstract
Fixational eye movements show scaling behaviour of the positional mean-squared displacement with a characteristic transition from persistence to antipersistence for increasing time-lag. These statistical patterns were found to be mainly shaped by microsaccades (fast, small-amplitude movements). However, our re-analysis of fixational eye-movement data provides evidence that the slow component (physiological drift) of the eyes exhibits scaling behaviour of the mean-squared displacement that varies across human participants. These results suggest that drift is a correlated movement that interacts with microsaccades. Moreover, on the long time scale, the mean-squared displacement of the drift shows oscillations, which is also present in the displacement auto-correlation function. This finding lends support to the presence of time-delayed feedback in the control of drift movements. Based on an earlier non-linear delayed feedback model of fixational eye movements, we propose and discuss different versions of a new model that combines a self-avoiding walk with time delay. As a result, we identify a model that reproduces oscillatory correlation functions, the transition from persistence to antipersistence, and microsaccades.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Carl J J Herrmann
- Institute of Physics and Astronomy, University of Potsdam, Potsdam, D-14476, Germany
| | - Ralf Metzler
- Institute of Physics and Astronomy, University of Potsdam, Potsdam, D-14476, Germany.
| | - Ralf Engbert
- Department of Psychology, University of Potsdam, Potsdam, D-14476, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
22
|
Amor TA, Luković M, Herrmann HJ, Andrade JS. Influence of scene structure and content on visual search strategies. J R Soc Interface 2017; 14:rsif.2017.0406. [PMID: 28747401 DOI: 10.1098/rsif.2017.0406] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/01/2017] [Accepted: 06/30/2017] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
When searching for a target within an image, our brain can adopt different strategies, but which one does it choose? This question can be answered by tracking the motion of the eye while it executes the task. Following many individuals performing various search tasks, we distinguish between two competing strategies. Motivated by these findings, we introduce a model that captures the interplay of the search strategies and allows us to create artificial eye-tracking trajectories, which could be compared with the experimental ones. Identifying the model parameters allows us to quantify the strategy employed in terms of ensemble averages, characterizing each experimental cohort. In this way, we can discern with high sensitivity the relation between the visual landscape and the average strategy, disclosing how small variations in the image induce changes in the strategy.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tatiana A Amor
- Computational Physics IfB, ETH Zurich, Stefano-Franscini-Platz 3, 8093, Zurich, Switzerland.,Departamento de Física, Universidade Federal do Ceará, 60451-970, Fortaleza, Ceará, Brazil
| | - Mirko Luković
- Computational Physics IfB, ETH Zurich, Stefano-Franscini-Platz 3, 8093, Zurich, Switzerland
| | - Hans J Herrmann
- Computational Physics IfB, ETH Zurich, Stefano-Franscini-Platz 3, 8093, Zurich, Switzerland.,Departamento de Física, Universidade Federal do Ceará, 60451-970, Fortaleza, Ceará, Brazil
| | - José S Andrade
- Departamento de Física, Universidade Federal do Ceará, 60451-970, Fortaleza, Ceará, Brazil
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Martínez-García R, Calabrese JM, López C. Online games: a novel approach to explore how partial information influences human random searches. Sci Rep 2017; 7:40029. [PMID: 28059115 PMCID: PMC5216393 DOI: 10.1038/srep40029] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/06/2016] [Accepted: 11/15/2016] [Indexed: 11/09/2022] Open
Abstract
Many natural processes rely on optimizing the success ratio of a search process. We use an experimental setup consisting of a simple online game in which players have to find a target hidden on a board, to investigate how the rounds are influenced by the detection of cues. We focus on the search duration and the statistics of the trajectories traced on the board. The experimental data are explained by a family of random-walk-based models and probabilistic analytical approximations. If no initial information is given to the players, the search is optimized for cues that cover an intermediate spatial scale. In addition, initial information about the extension of the cues results, in general, in faster searches. Finally, strategies used by informed players turn into non-stationary processes in which the length of e ach displacement evolves to show a well-defined characteristic scale that is not found in non-informed searches.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ricardo Martínez-García
- Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ 08544, USA
| | - Justin M. Calabrese
- Smithsonian Conservation Biology Institute, National Zoological Park, Front Royal, VA 22630, USA
- Department of Biology, University of Maryland, College Park, MD 20742, USA
| | - Cristóbal López
- IFISC, Instituto de Física Interdisciplinar y Sistemas Complejos (CSIC-UIB), E-07122 Palma de Mallorca, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
24
|
Kodera R, Tanahashi S, Iijima A, Maeda Y. Measurement of Eye Movement in Visual Search for a Target Symbol and Simulation to Construct a Small-World Network. ADVANCED BIOMEDICAL ENGINEERING 2017. [DOI: 10.14326/abe.6.129] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/04/2022] Open
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Atsuhiko Iijima
- Graduate School of Science and Technology, Niigata University
| | - Yoshinobu Maeda
- Graduate School of Science and Technology, Niigata University
| |
Collapse
|