1
|
Raja V. The motifs of radical embodied neuroscience. Eur J Neurosci 2024; 60:4738-4755. [PMID: 38816952 DOI: 10.1111/ejn.16434] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/26/2024] [Revised: 04/05/2024] [Accepted: 05/20/2024] [Indexed: 06/01/2024]
Abstract
In this paper, I analyse how the emerging scientific framework of radical embodied neuroscience is different from contemporary mainstream cognitive neuroscience. To do so, I propose the notion of motif to enrich the philosophical toolkit of cognitive neuroscience. This notion can be used to characterize the guiding ideas of any given scientific framework in psychology and neuroscience. Motifs are highly unconstrained, open-ended concepts that support equally open-ended families of explanations. Different scientific frameworks-e.g., psychophysics or cognitive neuroscience-provide these motifs to answer the overarching themes of these disciplines, such as the relationship between stimuli and sensations or the proper methods of the sciences of the mind. Some motifs of mainstream cognitive neuroscience are the motif of encoding, the motif of input-output systems, and the motif of algorithms. The two first ones answer the question about the relationship between stimuli, sensations and experience (e.g., stimuli are input and are encoded by brain structures). The latter one answers the question regarding the mechanism of cognition and experience. The three of them are equally unconstrained and open-ended, and they serve as an umbrella for different kinds of explanation-i.e., different positions regarding what counts as a code or as an input. Along with the articulation of the notion of motif, the main aim of this article is to present three motifs for radical embodied neuroscience: the motif of complex stimulation, the motif of organic behaviour and the motif of resonance.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Vicente Raja
- Department of Philosophy, Universidad de Murcia, Murcia, Spain
- Rotman Institute of Philosophy, Western University, London, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Guo C, Maruya A, Zaidi Q. Complexity of mental geometry for 3D pose perception. Vision Res 2024; 222:108438. [PMID: 38851047 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2024.108438] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/19/2022] [Revised: 05/24/2024] [Accepted: 05/25/2024] [Indexed: 06/10/2024]
Abstract
Biological visual systems rely on pose estimation of 3D objects to navigate and interact with their environment, but the neural mechanisms and computations for inferring 3D poses from 2D retinal images are only partially understood, especially where stereo information is missing. We previously presented evidence that humans infer the poses of 3D objects lying centered on the ground by using the geometrical back-transform from retinal images to viewer-centered world coordinates. This model explained the almost veridical estimation of poses in real scenes and the illusory rotation of poses in obliquely viewed pictures, which includes the "pointing out of the picture" phenomenon. Here we test this model for more varied configurations and find that it needs to be augmented. Five observers estimated poses of sloped, elevated, or off-center 3D sticks in each of 16 different poses displayed on a monitor in frontal and oblique views. Pose estimates in scenes and pictures showed remarkable accuracy and agreement between observers, but with a systematic fronto-parallel bias for oblique poses similar to the ground condition. The retinal projection of the pose of an object sloped wrt the ground depends on the slope. We show that observers' estimates can be explained by the back-transform derived for close to the correct slope. The back-transform explanation also applies to obliquely viewed pictures and to off-center objects and elevated objects, making it more likely that observers use internalized perspective geometry to make 3D pose inferences while actively incorporating inferences about other aspects of object placement.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Crystal Guo
- Graduate Center for Vision Research, State University of New York, 33 West 42(nd) St, New York, NY 10036, United States
| | - Akihito Maruya
- Graduate Center for Vision Research, State University of New York, 33 West 42(nd) St, New York, NY 10036, United States
| | - Qasim Zaidi
- Graduate Center for Vision Research, State University of New York, 33 West 42(nd) St, New York, NY 10036, United States.
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Volotsky S, Segev R. Figure-ground segmentation based on motion in the archerfish. Anim Cogn 2024; 27:33. [PMID: 38616235 PMCID: PMC11016505 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01873-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/01/2023] [Revised: 04/01/2024] [Accepted: 04/01/2024] [Indexed: 04/16/2024]
Abstract
Figure-ground segmentation is a fundamental process in visual perception that involves separating visual stimuli into distinct meaningful objects and their surrounding context, thus allowing the brain to interpret and understand complex visual scenes. Mammals exhibit varying figure-ground segmentation capabilities, ranging from primates that can perform well on figure-ground segmentation tasks to rodents that perform poorly. To explore figure-ground segmentation capabilities in teleost fish, we studied how the archerfish, an expert visual hunter, performs figure-ground segmentation. We trained archerfish to discriminate foreground objects from the background, where the figures were defined by motion as well as by discontinuities in intensity and texture. Specifically, the figures were defined by grating, naturalistic texture, and random noise moving in counterphase with the background. The archerfish performed the task well and could distinguish between all three types of figures and grounds. Their performance was comparable to that of primates and outperformed rodents. These findings suggest the existence of a complex visual process in the archerfish visual system that enables the delineation of figures as distinct from backgrounds, and provide insights into object recognition in this animal.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Svetlana Volotsky
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beersheba, Israel
- School of Brain Sciences and Cognition, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beersheba, Israel
- Department of Life Sciences, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beersheba, Israel
| | - Ronen Segev
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beersheba, Israel.
- School of Brain Sciences and Cognition, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beersheba, Israel.
- Department of Life Sciences, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beersheba, Israel.
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Luongo FJ, Liu L, Ho CLA, Hesse JK, Wekselblatt JB, Lanfranchi FF, Huber D, Tsao DY. Mice and primates use distinct strategies for visual segmentation. eLife 2023; 12:74394. [PMID: 36790170 PMCID: PMC9981152 DOI: 10.7554/elife.74394] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/02/2021] [Accepted: 01/22/2023] [Indexed: 02/16/2023] Open
Abstract
The rodent visual system has attracted great interest in recent years due to its experimental tractability, but the fundamental mechanisms used by the mouse to represent the visual world remain unclear. In the primate, researchers have argued from both behavioral and neural evidence that a key step in visual representation is 'figure-ground segmentation', the delineation of figures as distinct from backgrounds. To determine if mice also show behavioral and neural signatures of figure-ground segmentation, we trained mice on a figure-ground segmentation task where figures were defined by gratings and naturalistic textures moving counterphase to the background. Unlike primates, mice were severely limited in their ability to segment figure from ground using the opponent motion cue, with segmentation behavior strongly dependent on the specific carrier pattern. Remarkably, when mice were forced to localize naturalistic patterns defined by opponent motion, they adopted a strategy of brute force memorization of texture patterns. In contrast, primates, including humans, macaques, and mouse lemurs, could readily segment figures independent of carrier pattern using the opponent motion cue. Consistent with mouse behavior, neural responses to the same stimuli recorded in mouse visual areas V1, RL, and LM also did not support texture-invariant segmentation of figures using opponent motion. Modeling revealed that the texture dependence of both the mouse's behavior and neural responses could be explained by a feedforward neural network lacking explicit segmentation capabilities. These findings reveal a fundamental limitation in the ability of mice to segment visual objects compared to primates.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Francisco J Luongo
- Division of Biology and Biological Engineering, California Institute of TechnologyPasadenaUnited States
| | - Lu Liu
- Division of Biology and Biological Engineering, California Institute of TechnologyPasadenaUnited States
| | - Chun Lum Andy Ho
- Department of Basic Neurosciences, University of GenevaGenevaSwitzerland
| | - Janis K Hesse
- Division of Biology and Biological Engineering, California Institute of TechnologyPasadenaUnited States
- Computation and Neural Systems, California Institute of TechnologyPasadenaUnited States
- University of California, BerkeleyBerkeleyUnited States
| | - Joseph B Wekselblatt
- Division of Biology and Biological Engineering, California Institute of TechnologyPasadenaUnited States
| | - Frank F Lanfranchi
- Division of Biology and Biological Engineering, California Institute of TechnologyPasadenaUnited States
- Computation and Neural Systems, California Institute of TechnologyPasadenaUnited States
- University of California, BerkeleyBerkeleyUnited States
| | - Daniel Huber
- Department of Basic Neurosciences, University of GenevaGenevaSwitzerland
| | - Doris Y Tsao
- University of California, BerkeleyBerkeleyUnited States
- Howard Hughes Medical InstituteBerkeleyUnited States
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Linton P, Morgan MJ, Read JCA, Vishwanath D, Creem-Regehr SH, Domini F. New Approaches to 3D Vision. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2023; 378:20210443. [PMID: 36511413 PMCID: PMC9745878 DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2021.0443] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/30/2022] [Accepted: 10/25/2022] [Indexed: 12/15/2022] Open
Abstract
New approaches to 3D vision are enabling new advances in artificial intelligence and autonomous vehicles, a better understanding of how animals navigate the 3D world, and new insights into human perception in virtual and augmented reality. Whilst traditional approaches to 3D vision in computer vision (SLAM: simultaneous localization and mapping), animal navigation (cognitive maps), and human vision (optimal cue integration) start from the assumption that the aim of 3D vision is to provide an accurate 3D model of the world, the new approaches to 3D vision explored in this issue challenge this assumption. Instead, they investigate the possibility that computer vision, animal navigation, and human vision can rely on partial or distorted models or no model at all. This issue also highlights the implications for artificial intelligence, autonomous vehicles, human perception in virtual and augmented reality, and the treatment of visual disorders, all of which are explored by individual articles. This article is part of a discussion meeting issue 'New approaches to 3D vision'.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Paul Linton
- Presidential Scholars in Society and Neuroscience, Center for Science and Society, Columbia University, New York, NY 10027, USA
- Italian Academy for Advanced Studies in America, Columbia University, New York, NY 10027, USA
- Visual Inference Lab, Zuckerman Mind Brain Behavior Institute, Columbia University, New York, NY 10027, USA
| | - Michael J. Morgan
- Department of Optometry and Visual Sciences, City, University of London, Northampton Square, London EC1V 0HB, UK
| | - Jenny C. A. Read
- Biosciences Institute, Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne, Tyne & Wear NE2 4HH, UK
| | - Dhanraj Vishwanath
- School of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of St Andrews, St Andrews, Fife KY16 9JP, UK
| | | | - Fulvio Domini
- Department of Cognitive, Linguistic, and Psychological Sciences, Brown University, Providence, RI 02912-9067, USA
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Zaidi Q. The reasonable effectiveness of contours in vision. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2022; 119:e2215097119. [PMID: 36264820 PMCID: PMC9636940 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.2215097119] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/18/2022] Open
Affiliation(s)
- Qasim Zaidi
- Graduate Center for Vision Research, State University of New York, New York, NY 10036
| |
Collapse
|