1
|
Ellis DA, Towse J, Brown O, Cork A, Davidson BI, Devereux S, Hinds J, Ivory M, Nightingale S, Parry DA, Piwek L, Shaw H, Towse AS. Assessing computational reproducibility in Behavior Research Methods. Behav Res Methods 2024; 56:8745-8760. [PMID: 39322919 PMCID: PMC11525395 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-024-02501-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 08/15/2024] [Indexed: 09/27/2024]
Abstract
Psychological science has thrived thanks to new methods and innovative practices. Journals, including Behavior Research Methods (BRM), continue to support the dissemination and evaluation of research assets including data, software/hardware, statistical code, and databases of stimuli. However, such research assets rarely allow for computational reproducibility, meaning they are difficult to reuse. Therefore, in this preregistered report, we explore how BRM's authors and BRM structures shape the landscape of functional research assets. Our broad research questions concern: (1) How quickly methods and analytical techniques reported in BRM can be used and developed further by other scientists; (2) Whether functionality has improved following changes to BRM journal policy in support of computational reproducibility; (3) Whether we can disentangle such policy changes from changes in reproducibility over time. We randomly sampled equal numbers of papers (N = 204) published in BRM before and after the implementation of policy changes. Pairs of researchers recorded how long it took to ensure assets (data, software/hardware, statistical code, and materials) were fully operational. They also coded the completeness and reusability of the assets. While improvements were observed in all measures, only changes to completeness were altered significantly following the policy changes (d = .37). The effects varied between different types of research assets, with data sets from surveys/experiments showing the largest improvements in completeness and reusability. Perhaps more importantly, changes to policy do appear to have improved the life span of research products by reducing natural decline. We conclude with a discussion of how, in the future, research and policy might better support computational reproducibility within and beyond psychological science.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - John Towse
- Department of Psychology, Lancaster University, Lancaster, UK
| | - Olivia Brown
- School of Management, University of Bath, Bath, UK
| | - Alicia Cork
- School of Management, University of Bath, Bath, UK
| | | | - Sophie Devereux
- Department of Psychology, Lancaster University, Lancaster, UK
| | - Joanne Hinds
- School of Management, University of Bath, Bath, UK
| | - Matthew Ivory
- Department of Psychology, Lancaster University, Lancaster, UK
| | | | - Douglas A Parry
- Department of Information Science, Stellenbosch University, Stellenbosch, South Africa
| | - Lukasz Piwek
- School of Management, University of Bath, Bath, UK
| | - Heather Shaw
- Department of Psychology, Lancaster University, Lancaster, UK
| | - Andrea S Towse
- Department of Psychology, Lancaster University, Lancaster, UK
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Prosser AMB, Bagnall R, Higson-Sweeney N. Reflection over compliance: Critiquing mandatory data sharing policies for qualitative research. J Health Psychol 2024; 29:653-658. [PMID: 38282356 PMCID: PMC11141091 DOI: 10.1177/13591053231225903] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/30/2024] Open
Abstract
Many journals are moving towards a 'Mandatory Inclusion of Raw Data' (MIRD) model of data sharing, where it is expected that raw data be publicly accessible at article submission. While open data sharing is beneficial for some research topics and methodologies within health psychology, in other cases it may be ethically and epistemologically questionable. Here, we outline several questions that qualitative researchers might consider surrounding the ethics of open data sharing. Overall, we argue that universal open raw data mandates cannot adequately represent the diversity of qualitative research, and that MIRD may harm rigorous and ethical research practice within health psychology and beyond. Researchers should instead find ways to demonstrate rigour thorough engagement with questions surrounding data sharing. We propose that all researchers utilise the increasingly common 'data availability statement' to demonstrate reflexive engagement with issues of ethics, epistemology and participant protection when considering whether to open data.
Collapse
|
3
|
Kalandadze T, Hart SA. Open Developmental Science: An Overview and Annotated Reading List. INFANT AND CHILD DEVELOPMENT 2024; 33:e2334. [PMID: 39308897 PMCID: PMC11415275 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2334] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/10/2021] [Accepted: 04/21/2022] [Indexed: 02/06/2023]
Abstract
The increasing adoption of open science practices in the last decade has been changing the scientific landscape across fields. However, developmental science has been relatively slow in adopting open science practices. To address this issue, we followed the format of Crüwell et al., (2019) and created summaries and an annotated list of informative and actionable resources discussing ten topics in developmental science: Open science; Reproducibility and replication; Open data, materials and code; Open access; Preregistration; Registered reports; Replication; Incentives; Collaborative developmental science. This article offers researchers and students in developmental science a starting point for understanding how open science intersects with developmental science. After getting familiarized with this article, the developmental scientist should understand the core tenets of open and reproducible developmental science, and feel motivated to start applying open science practices in their workflow.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Sara A. Hart
- Department of Psychology, Florida State University
- Florida Center for Reading Research, Florida State University
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Elmer T. Computational social science is growing up: why puberty consists of embracing measurement validation, theory development, and open science practices. EPJ DATA SCIENCE 2023; 12:58. [PMID: 38098785 PMCID: PMC10716103 DOI: 10.1140/epjds/s13688-023-00434-1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/24/2023] [Accepted: 11/30/2023] [Indexed: 12/17/2023]
Abstract
Puberty is a phase in which individuals often test the boundaries of themselves and surrounding others and further define their identity - and thus their uniqueness compared to other individuals. Similarly, as Computational Social Science (CSS) grows up, it must strike a balance between its own practices and those of neighboring disciplines to achieve scientific rigor and refine its identity. However, there are certain areas within CSS that are reluctant to adopt rigorous scientific practices from other fields, which can be observed through an overreliance on passively collected data (e.g., through digital traces, wearables) without questioning the validity of such data. This paper argues that CSS should embrace the potential of combining both passive and active measurement practices to capitalize on the strengths of each approach, including objectivity and psychological quality. Additionally, the paper suggests that CSS would benefit from integrating practices and knowledge from other established disciplines, such as measurement validation, theoretical embedding, and open science practices. Based on this argument, the paper provides ten recommendations for CSS to mature as an interdisciplinary field of research.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Timon Elmer
- Department of Psychology, Applied Social and Health Psychology, University of Zurich, Binzmühlestrasse 14/14, 8050 Zurich, Switzerland
| |
Collapse
|