1
|
Marchant R, Carter J, Fairhurst C. Opening doors: suggested practice for medical professionals for when a child might be close to telling about abuse. Arch Dis Child 2021; 106:108-110. [PMID: 33234531 DOI: 10.1136/archdischild-2020-320093] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 10/22/2020] [Indexed: 11/04/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Ruth Marchant
- Triangle Services for Children, Brighton, East Sussex, UK
| | - Jamie Carter
- Community Paediatric Department, Seaside View CDC, Brighton General Hospital, Sussex Community NHS Foundation Trust, Brighton, UK
| | - Charlie Fairhurst
- Department of Paediatric Neurosciences, Evelina Children's hospital, London, UK
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Johnson JL, Hobbs SD, Chae Y, Goodman GS, Shestowsky D, Block SD. "I Didn't Do That!" Event Valence and Child Age Influence Adults' Discernment of Preschoolers' True and False Statements. JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE 2021; 36:NP753-NP771. [PMID: 29294958 DOI: 10.1177/0886260517736276] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 06/07/2023]
Abstract
Justice can hinge on adults' abilities to distinguish accurate from inaccurate child testimony. Yet relatively little is known about factors that affect adults' abilities to determine the accuracy of children's eyewitness reports. In this study, adults (N = 108) viewed videoclips of 3- and 5-year-olds answering open-ended and leading questions about positive and negative actually experienced ("true") events or never experienced ("false") events that the children either affirmed or denied. Analyses revealed that adults were more accurate at determining the veracity of negative compared with positive incidents, particularly when children said that they had experienced the event. Moreover, adults' accuracy was at chance for older children's false denials. Psycholegal implications are discussed.
Collapse
|
3
|
An Investigation of the Question-Types Teachers Use to Elicit Information From Children. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2020. [DOI: 10.1017/edp.2014.5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/27/2022]
|
4
|
Cleveland KC, Quas JA, Lyon TD. The effects of implicit encouragement and the putative confession on children's memory reports. CHILD ABUSE & NEGLECT 2018; 80:113-122. [PMID: 29604502 PMCID: PMC5953828 DOI: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2018.03.024] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/08/2017] [Revised: 03/10/2018] [Accepted: 03/14/2018] [Indexed: 05/11/2023]
Abstract
The current study tested the effects of two interview techniques on children's report productivity and accuracy following exposure to suggestion: implicit encouragement (backchanneling, use of children's names) and the putative confession (telling children that a suspect "told me everything that happened and wants you to tell the truth"). One hundred and forty-three, 3-8-year-old children participated in a classroom event. One week later, they took part in a highly suggestive conversation about the event and then a mock forensic interview in which the two techniques were experimentally manipulated. Greater use of implicit encouragement led to increases, with age, in children's narrative productivity. Neither technique improved or reduced children's accuracy. No increases in errors about previously suggested information were evident when children received either technique. Implications for the use of these techniques in child forensic interviews are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kyndra C Cleveland
- Vanderbilt University, Department of Psychology and Human Development, Peabody College, 230 Appleton Place #552, Jesup Building Room 105, Nashville, TN 37203-5721, United States.
| | - Jodi A Quas
- 4328 Social & Behavioral Sciences Gateway, University of California, Irvine, CA 92697-7085, United States.
| | - Thomas D Lyon
- University of Southern California, 699 Exposition Boulevard, Los Angeles, CA 90089, United States.
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Talwar V, Hubbard K, Saykaly C, Lee K, Lindsay RCL, Bala N. Does parental coaching affect children's false reports? Comparing verbal markers of deception. BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES & THE LAW 2018; 36:84-97. [PMID: 29460438 DOI: 10.1002/bsl.2331] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/17/2016] [Revised: 07/29/2017] [Accepted: 01/13/2018] [Indexed: 06/08/2023]
Abstract
The present study examined differences in children's true and false narratives as a function of parental coaching by comparing the verbal markers associated with deception. Children (N = 65, 4-7 years old) played the same game with an adult stranger over three consecutive days. Parents coached their children to falsely allege that they had played a second game and to generate details for the fabricated event. One week after the last play session, children were interviewed about their experiences. For children with the least amount of parental coaching, true and false reports could be distinguished by multiple verbal markers of deception (e.g., cognitive processes, temporal information, self-references). The fabricated reports of children who spent more time being coaching by a parent resembled their truthful reports. These findings have implications for real-world forensic contexts when children have been coached to make false allegations and fabricate information at the behest of a parent.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Victoria Talwar
- Department of Educational and Counselling Psychology, McGill University, Montreal, Canada
| | - Kyle Hubbard
- Department of Educational and Counselling Psychology, McGill University, Montreal, Canada
| | - Christine Saykaly
- Department of Educational and Counselling Psychology, McGill University, Montreal, Canada
| | - Kang Lee
- Dr Eric Jackman Institute of Child Study and Applied Psychology and Human Development Department, University of Toronto, Toronto, Canada
| | - R C L Lindsay
- Department of Psychology, Queen's University, Kingston, Canada
| | - Nicholas Bala
- Faculty of Law, Queen's University, Kingston, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Marchant R, Turner L. ‘Opening Doors’: best practice when a young child might be showing or telling you that they are at risk. ACTA ACUST UNITED AC 2017. [DOI: 10.12968/eyed.2017.19.6.54] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/11/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Ruth Marchant
- Forensic interviewer and witness intermediary, providing communication support to very young children in their involvement with the police and the courts
| | - Lucy Turner
- Works with Triangle as an intermediary, interviewer, advocate, and trainer. She is qualified as an Early Years Teacher and SENCO
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Talwar V, Yachison S, Leduc K, Nagar PM. Practice makes perfect? The impact of coaching and moral stories on children’s lie-telling. INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT 2017. [DOI: 10.1177/0165025417728583] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
Children ( n = 202; 4 to 7 years old) witnessed a confederate break a toy and were asked to keep the transgression a secret. Children were randomly assigned to a Coaching condition (i.e., No Coaching, Light Coaching, or Heavy Coaching) and a Moral Story condition (i.e., Positive or Neutral). Overall, 89.7% of children lied about the broken toy when asked open-ended questions about the event. During direct questions, children in the Heavy Coaching condition lied more than children in the No Coaching and Light Coaching conditions. Older children were influenced by both Heavy Coaching and Light Coaching, whereas younger children were influenced only by Heavy Coaching. Children in the Positive Story condition were less likely to maintain their lies than those in the Neutral Story condition. An interaction between Coaching and Moral Story conditions influenced lie-maintenance.
Collapse
|
8
|
Sharman SJ, Powell MB, Skouteris H. A qualitative evaluation of the challenges faced by dieticians when interviewing children. Appetite 2016; 100:80-5. [PMID: 26879223 DOI: 10.1016/j.appet.2016.02.014] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/29/2015] [Revised: 01/28/2016] [Accepted: 02/09/2016] [Indexed: 11/18/2022]
Abstract
BACKGROUND Investigative interviewing is a critical and challenging skill involved in the assessment and design of appropriate interventions for children's dietary problems. The current study provided an evaluation of the challenges faced by professional dieticians when conducting child investigative interviews, in the hope that this would provide a framework for the development of further guidance and resources in this important area. METHODS Fourteen professional dieticians were interviewed; they were asked about the information that they needed to elicit from children in particular situations and the questions that they would ask to do so. They were also asked to describe the strengths and limitations of the techniques that they used. RESULTS The results revealed that professionals faced three main challenges. The first challenge was eliciting information from children who did not want to answer questions. The second challenge was determining the level of accuracy in children's (and caregivers') responses. The third challenge was eliciting very specific information in particular situations, such as determining the cause of an allergic reaction. CONCLUSIONS Overall, professionals had difficulty articulating the questions that they would use to elicit the information that they required; indeed, their responses focused more on the content that they wanted to elicit (such as specific details) rather than the overall process that they would use to do so. Professionals may benefit from the development of guidelines to assist them in their interviews with children, based on what is currently known about interviewing children generally.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Stefanie J Sharman
- School of Psychology, Deakin University, 221 Burwood Highway, Burwood, VIC, 3125, Australia.
| | - Martine B Powell
- School of Psychology, Deakin University, 221 Burwood Highway, Burwood, VIC, 3125, Australia.
| | - Helen Skouteris
- School of Psychology, Deakin University, 221 Burwood Highway, Burwood, VIC, 3125, Australia.
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Brubacher SP, Powell M, Skouteris H, Guadagno B. The effects of e-simulation interview training on teachers' use of open-ended questions. CHILD ABUSE & NEGLECT 2015; 43:95-103. [PMID: 25703802 DOI: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2015.02.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/11/2014] [Revised: 01/27/2015] [Accepted: 02/02/2015] [Indexed: 06/04/2023]
Abstract
Teachers in many parts of the world are mandated reporters of child abuse and maltreatment but very little is known concerning how they question children in suspicious circumstances. Teachers (n=36), who had previously participated in a mock interview scenario designed to characterize their baseline use of various question-types when attempting to elicit sensitive information from children, were given online training in choosing effective questions. They engaged in simulated interviews with a virtual avatar several times in one week and then participated in a mock interview scenario. The amount and proportion of open-ended questions they used increased dramatically after training. The overall number of questions, and amount and proportions of specific and leading questions decreased. In particular, large decreases were observed in more risky yes-no and other forced-choice questions. Given that most teachers may feel the need to ask a child about an ambiguous situation at some point during their careers it is worthwhile to incorporate practice asking effective questions into their training, and the present research suggests that an e-learning format is effective. Additionally, effective questions encourage the development of narrative competence, and we discuss how teachers might include open-ended questions during regular classroom learning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sonja P Brubacher
- Centre for Investigative Interviewing, Deakin University, Australia; School of Psychology, Deakin University, Australia
| | - Martine Powell
- Centre for Investigative Interviewing, Deakin University, Australia; School of Psychology, Deakin University, Australia
| | - Helen Skouteris
- Centre for Investigative Interviewing, Deakin University, Australia; School of Psychology, Deakin University, Australia
| | | |
Collapse
|