1
|
Coppinger BA, Carlson NV, Freeberg TM, Sieving KE. Mixed-species groups and the question of dominance in the social ecosystem. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2023; 378:20220097. [PMID: 37066641 PMCID: PMC10107276 DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2022.0097] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/16/2022] [Accepted: 02/10/2023] [Indexed: 04/18/2023] Open
Abstract
Dominance interactions and hierarchies are of long-standing interest in the field of animal behaviour. Currently, dominance hierarchies are viewed as complex social structures formed by repeated interactions between individuals. Most studies on this phenomenon come from single-species groups. However, animals are constantly surrounded by and interact with individuals of other species. Behaviour and social interactions of individuals can be shaped by the presence or behaviour of other species in their social ecosystem, which has important implications for social behaviour in groups. Given how ubiquitous mixed-species animal groups are, deeper study of the relationships between mixed-species group (MSG) structure and dominance will be key to understanding constraints on individual behaviour and decision making. Here we call for more research into dominance interactions among individuals in MSGs. Greater understanding of the dynamics of dominance relationships among individuals in MSGs, whose size and composition can change considerably over shorter and longer term time frames, will be crucial to understanding their structure and functioning. This article is part of the theme issue 'Mixed-species groups and aggregations: shaping ecological and behavioural patterns and processes'.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- B. A. Coppinger
- Department of Biology, Villanova University, Villanova, PA 19085, USA
| | - N. V. Carlson
- Graduate School of Science, Faculty of Science, Kyoto University, Kyoto, Japan
- Department of Biology, University of Victoria, Victoria, Canada V8W 2Y2
| | - T. M. Freeberg
- Department of Psychology, University of Tennessee, Knoxville, PA 37996, USA
| | - K. E. Sieving
- Wildlife Ecology and Conservation, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL 32611, USA
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Mangini GG, Rutt CL, Sridhar H, Buitron G, Muñoz J, Robinson SK, Montaño-Centellas F, Zarco A, Fanjul ME, Fernández-Arellano G, Xing S, Camerlenghi E. A classification scheme for mixed-species bird flocks. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2023; 378:20220100. [PMID: 37066650 PMCID: PMC10107246 DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2022.0100] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/16/2022] [Accepted: 01/17/2023] [Indexed: 04/18/2023] Open
Abstract
The literature on mixed-species flocks references a wide variety of bird associations. These studies, however, have used an array of unstructured characteristics to describe flocks, ranging from the temporal occurrence of flocking to the identity and behavioural features of constituent members, with little consensus on which key traits define and characterize a mixed-species flock. Moreover, although most studies report species-specific roles, there is no clear consensus about what these roles signify nor how to define them. This lack of consistency limits our ability to compare flocks from different habitats, regions and species pools. To unify this sizable body of literature, we reviewed and synthesized 538 studies on mixed-species flocks. We propose 13 categories to classify mixed-species flocks using behavioural and physical traits at the flock and participant level, as well as the habitat where the flock occurs. Lastly, we discuss the historical terminology for different species roles and propose definitions to clarify and distinguish among nuclear, leader, sentinel, and flock-following species. We envision that these guidelines will provide a universal language for mixed-species flock research, paving the way for future comparisons and new insight between different regions and systems. This article is part of the theme issue 'Mixed-species groups and aggregations: shaping ecological and behavioural patterns and processes'.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- G. Giselle Mangini
- Instituto de Ecologia Regional (IER) CONICET-UNT, 4107 Yerba Buena, Argentina
| | | | - Hari Sridhar
- Independent Researcher, Bengaluru, Karnataka 560003, India
- Konrad Lorenz Institute for Evolution and Cognition Research, 3400 Klosterneuburg, Austria
| | - Galo Buitron
- Universidad Estatal Amazónica-Sede Académica El Pangui, Zamora Chinchipe, 190401, Ecuador
| | - Jenny Muñoz
- University of British Columbia Biodiversity Research Center, Vancouver, Canada V6T 1Z4
| | - Scott K. Robinson
- Florida Museum of Natural History, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL 32611, USA
| | | | - Agustin Zarco
- Instituto Argentino de Investigación en las Zonas Áridas (IADIZA) CONICET, 5500 Mendoza, Argentina
- Fundación de Historia Natural Félix de Azara, 1405 Buenos Aires, Argentina
| | - M. Elisa Fanjul
- Instituto de Vertebrados, Zoología, Fundación Miguel Lillo, 4000 Tucumán, Argentina
- Facultad de Ciencias Naturales e IML – Universidad Nacional de Tucumán, 4000 Tucumán, Argentina
| | - Gilberto Fernández-Arellano
- Programa de Pós-Graduação em Ecologia e Conservação da Biodiversidade, Universidade Federal de Mato Grosso – UFMT, 78060-900 Cuiabá, Brazil
| | - Shuang Xing
- School of Ecology, Sun Yat-sen University, 518107 Guangdong, People's Republic of China
| | - Ettore Camerlenghi
- School of Biological Sciences, Monash University, Wellington Road, Clayton, Victoria 3800, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Lakin R, Hendrie C. Effects of alarm call playback on the behaviour of wild European herring gulls. LEARNING AND MOTIVATION 2021. [DOI: 10.1016/j.lmot.2020.101691] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/22/2022]
|
4
|
Goodale E, Sridhar H, Sieving KE, Bangal P, Colorado Z GJ, Farine DR, Heymann EW, Jones HH, Krams I, Martínez AE, Montaño-Centellas F, Muñoz J, Srinivasan U, Theo A, Shanker K. Mixed company: a framework for understanding the composition and organization of mixed-species animal groups. Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc 2020; 95:889-910. [PMID: 32097520 PMCID: PMC7383667 DOI: 10.1111/brv.12591] [Citation(s) in RCA: 44] [Impact Index Per Article: 11.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/23/2019] [Revised: 02/02/2020] [Accepted: 02/06/2020] [Indexed: 12/19/2022]
Abstract
Mixed-species animal groups (MSGs) are widely acknowledged to increase predator avoidance and foraging efficiency, among other benefits, and thereby increase participants' fitness. Diversity in MSG composition ranges from two to 70 species of very similar or completely different phenotypes. Yet consistency in organization is also observable in that one or a few species usually have disproportionate importance for MSG formation and/or maintenance. We propose a two-dimensional framework for understanding this diversity and consistency, concentrating on the types of interactions possible between two individuals, usually of different species. One axis represents the similarity of benefit types traded between the individuals, while the second axis expresses asymmetry in the relative amount of benefits/costs accrued. Considering benefit types, one extreme represents the case of single-species groups wherein all individuals obtain the same supplementary, group-size-related benefits, and the other extreme comprises associations of very different, but complementary species (e.g. one partner creates access to food while the other provides vigilance). The relevance of social information and the matching of activities (e.g. speed of movement) are highest for relationships on the supplementary side of this axis, but so is competition; relationships between species will occur at points along this gradient where the benefits outweigh the costs. Considering benefit amounts given or received, extreme asymmetry occurs when one species is exclusively a benefit provider and the other a benefit user. Within this parameter space, some MSG systems are constrained to one kind of interaction, such as shoals of fish of similar species or leader-follower interactions in fish and other taxa. Other MSGs, such as terrestrial bird flocks, can simultaneously include a variety of supplementary and complementary interactions. We review the benefits that species obtain across the diversity of MSG types, and argue that the degree and nature of asymmetry between benefit providers and users should be measured and not just assumed. We then discuss evolutionary shifts in MSG types, focusing on drivers towards similarity in group composition, and selection on benefit providers to enhance the benefits they can receive from other species. Finally, we conclude by considering how individual and collective behaviour in MSGs may influence both the structure and processes of communities.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Eben Goodale
- Guangxi Key Laboratory for Forest Ecology and Conservation, College of Forestry, Guangxi University, Nanning, Guangxi, 530004, China
| | - Hari Sridhar
- Centre for Ecological Sciences, Indian Institute of Science, Bengaluru, Karnataka, 560012, India.,National Centre for Biological Sciences, Tata Institute of Fundamental Research, Bangalore, Karnataka, 560012, India
| | - Kathryn E Sieving
- Department of Wildlife Ecology and Conservation, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, 32611, U.S.A
| | - Priti Bangal
- Centre for Ecological Sciences, Indian Institute of Science, Bengaluru, Karnataka, 560012, India
| | - Gabriel J Colorado Z
- Departamento de Ciencias Forestales, Universidad Nacional de Colombia, Medellín, 050034, Colombia
| | - Damien R Farine
- Department of Collective Behavior, Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior, Universitätsstrasse 10, D-78464, Konstanz, Germany.,Centre for the Advanced Study of Collective Behaviour, University of Konstanz, D-78464, Konstanz, Germany.,Department of Biology, University of Konstanz, D-78464, Konstanz, Germany
| | - Eckhard W Heymann
- Deutsches Primatenzentrum, Leibniz-Institut für Primatenforschung, D-37077, Göttingen, Germany
| | - Harrison H Jones
- Department of Biology, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, 32611, U.S.A.,Florida Museum of Natural History, Gainesville, FL, 32611, U.S.A
| | - Indrikis Krams
- Institute of Ecology and Earth Sciences, University of Tartu, Tartu, 51410, Estonia.,Department of Zoology and Animal Ecology, Faculty of Biology, University of Latvia, Rīga, 1004, Latvia
| | - Ari E Martínez
- Department of Biological Sciences, California State University, Long Beach, CA, 90840, U.S.A
| | - Flavia Montaño-Centellas
- Department of Wildlife Ecology and Conservation, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, 32611, U.S.A.,Instituto de Ecologia, Universidad Mayor de San Andres, La Paz, 10077, Bolivia
| | - Jenny Muñoz
- Zoology Department and Biodiversity Research Center, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, V6T 1ZA, Canada
| | - Umesh Srinivasan
- Woodrow Wilson School of Public and International Affairs, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ, 08540, U.S.A
| | - Anne Theo
- Centre for Ecological Sciences, Indian Institute of Science, Bengaluru, Karnataka, 560012, India
| | - Kartik Shanker
- Centre for Ecological Sciences, Indian Institute of Science, Bengaluru, Karnataka, 560012, India.,Dakshin Foundation, Bengaluru, 560092, India
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Goller M, Shizuka D. Evolutionary origins of vocal mimicry in songbirds. Evol Lett 2018; 2:417-426. [PMID: 30283692 PMCID: PMC6121844 DOI: 10.1002/evl3.62] [Citation(s) in RCA: 28] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/17/2018] [Revised: 05/15/2018] [Accepted: 05/22/2018] [Indexed: 11/24/2022] Open
Abstract
Vocal learning is an important behavior in oscines (songbirds). Some songbird species learn heterospecific sounds as well as conspecific vocalizations. The emergence of vocal mimicry is necessarily tied to the evolution of vocal learning, as mimicry requires the ability to acquire sounds through learning. As such, tracking the evolutionary origins of vocal mimicry may provide insights into the causes of variation in song learning programs among songbirds. We compiled a database of known vocal mimics that comprised 339 species from 43 families. We then traced the evolutionary history of vocal mimicry across the avian phylogeny using ancestral trait reconstruction on a dataset of oscine passerines for which vocalizations have been described. We found that the common ancestor to oscines was unlikely to mimic sounds, suggesting that song learning evolved with mechanisms to constrain learning to conspecific models. Mimicry then evolved repeatedly within the songbird clade, either through relaxation of constraints on conspecific learning or through selection for active vocal mimicry. Vocal mimicry is likely ancestral in only a handful of clades, and we detect many instances of independent origins of mimicry. Our analysis underscores the liability of vocal mimicry in songbirds, and highlights the evolutionary flexibility of song learning mechanisms.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Maria Goller
- School of Biological Sciences University of Nebraska-Lincoln Lincoln Nebraska 68588-0118
| | - Daizaburo Shizuka
- School of Biological Sciences University of Nebraska-Lincoln Lincoln Nebraska 68588-0118
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Nácarová J, Veselý P, Bugnyar T. Ravens adjust their antipredatory responses to con- and hetero-specific alarms to the perceived threat. Ethology 2018. [DOI: 10.1111/eth.12764] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Jana Nácarová
- Faculty of Science; University of South Bohemia; České Budějovice Czech Republic
| | - Petr Veselý
- Faculty of Science; University of South Bohemia; České Budějovice Czech Republic
| | - Thomas Bugnyar
- Department of Cognitive Biology; University of Vienna; Vienna Austria
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Igic B, McLachlan J, Lehtinen I, Magrath RD. Crying wolf to a predator: deceptive vocal mimicry by a bird protecting young. Proc Biol Sci 2016; 282:20150798. [PMID: 26041353 DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2015.0798] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Animals often mimic dangerous or toxic species to deter predators; however, mimicry of such species may not always be possible and mimicry of benign species seems unlikely to confer anti-predator benefits. We reveal a system in which a bird mimics the alarm calls of harmless species to fool a predator 40 times its size and protect its offspring against attack. Our experiments revealed that brown thornbills (Acanthiza pusilla) mimic a chorus of other species' aerial alarm calls, a cue of an Accipiter hawk in flight, when predators attack their nest. The absence of any flying predators in this context implies that these alarms convey deceptive information about the type of danger present. Experiments on the primary nest predators of thornbills, pied currawongs (Strepera graculina), revealed that the predators treat these alarms as if they themselves are threatened by flying hawks, either by scanning the sky for danger or fleeing, confirming a deceptive function. In turn, these distractions delay attack and provide thornbill nestlings with an opportunity to escape. This sophisticated defence strategy exploits the complex web of interactions among multiple species across several trophic levels, and in particular exploits a predator's ability to eavesdrop on and respond appropriately to heterospecific alarm calls. Our findings demonstrate that prey can fool predators by deceptively mimicking alarm calls of harmless species, suggesting that defensive mimicry could be more widespread because of indirect effects on predators within a web of eavesdropping.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Branislav Igic
- Research School of Biology, The Australian National University, Canberra, Australian Capital Territory, Australia
| | - Jessica McLachlan
- Research School of Biology, The Australian National University, Canberra, Australian Capital Territory, Australia Department of Zoology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
| | - Inkeri Lehtinen
- Research School of Biology, The Australian National University, Canberra, Australian Capital Territory, Australia Department of Environmental Sciences, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland
| | - Robert D Magrath
- Research School of Biology, The Australian National University, Canberra, Australian Capital Territory, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Westrip JRS, Bell MBV. Breaking down the Species Boundaries: Selective Pressures behind Interspecific Communication in Vertebrates. Ethology 2015. [DOI: 10.1111/eth.12379] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/31/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- James R. S. Westrip
- Institute of Evolutionary Biology; School of Biological Sciences; University of Edinburgh; Ashworth Laboratories; Edinburgh UK
| | - Matthew B. V. Bell
- Institute of Evolutionary Biology; School of Biological Sciences; University of Edinburgh; Ashworth Laboratories; Edinburgh UK
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Kaplan G. Animal communication. WILEY INTERDISCIPLINARY REVIEWS. COGNITIVE SCIENCE 2014; 5:661-677. [PMID: 26308872 DOI: 10.1002/wcs.1321] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/12/2014] [Revised: 07/16/2014] [Accepted: 08/04/2014] [Indexed: 02/03/2023]
Abstract
UNLABELLED Animal communication is first and foremost about signal transmission and aims to understand how communication occurs. It is a field that has contributed to and been inspired by other fields, from information technology to neuroscience, in finding ever better methods to eavesdrop on the actual 'message' that forms the basis of communication. Much of this review deals with vocal communication as an example of the questions that research on communication has tried to answer and it provides an historical overview of the theoretical arguments proposed. Topics covered include signal transmission in different environments and different species, referential signaling, and intentionality. The contention is that animal communication may reveal significant thought processes that enable some individuals in a small number of species so far investigated to anticipate what conspecifics might do, although some researchers think of such behavior as adaptive or worth dismissing as anthropomorphizing. The review further points out that some species are more likely than others to develop more complex communication patterns. It is a matter of asking how animals categorize their world and which concepts require cognitive processes and which are adaptive. The review concludes with questions of life history, social learning, and decision making, all criteria that have remained relatively unexplored in communication research. Long-lived, cooperative social animals have so far offered especially exciting prospects for investigation. There are ample opportunities and now very advanced technologies as well to tap further into expressions of memory of signals, be they vocal or expressed in other modalities. WIREs Cogn Sci 2014, 5:661-677. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1321 For further resources related to this article, please visit the WIREs website. CONFLICT OF INTEREST The author has declared no conflicts of interest for this article.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gisela Kaplan
- Centre for Neuroscience and Animal Behaviour, School of Science and Technology, University of New England, Armidale, Australia
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Dalziell AH, Welbergen JA, Igic B, Magrath RD. Avian vocal mimicry: a unified conceptual framework. Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc 2014; 90:643-68. [PMID: 25079896 DOI: 10.1111/brv.12129] [Citation(s) in RCA: 34] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/25/2013] [Revised: 06/10/2014] [Accepted: 06/16/2014] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
Mimicry is a classical example of adaptive signal design. Here, we review the current state of research into vocal mimicry in birds. Avian vocal mimicry is a conspicuous and often spectacular form of animal communication, occurring in many distantly related species. However, the proximate and ultimate causes of vocal mimicry are poorly understood. In the first part of this review, we argue that progress has been impeded by conceptual confusion over what constitutes vocal mimicry. We propose a modified version of Vane-Wright's (1980) widely used definition of mimicry. According to our definition, a vocalisation is mimetic if the behaviour of the receiver changes after perceiving the acoustic resemblance between the mimic and the model, and the behavioural change confers a selective advantage on the mimic. Mimicry is therefore specifically a functional concept where the resemblance between heterospecific sounds is a target of selection. It is distinct from other forms of vocal resemblance including those that are the result of chance or common ancestry, and those that have emerged as a by-product of other processes such as ecological convergence and selection for large song-type repertoires. Thus, our definition provides a general and functionally coherent framework for determining what constitutes vocal mimicry, and takes account of the diversity of vocalisations that incorporate heterospecific sounds. In the second part we assess and revise hypotheses for the evolution of avian vocal mimicry in the light of our new definition. Most of the current evidence is anecdotal, but the diverse contexts and acoustic structures of putative vocal mimicry suggest that mimicry has multiple functions across and within species. There is strong experimental evidence that vocal mimicry can be deceptive, and can facilitate parasitic interactions. There is also increasing support for the use of vocal mimicry in predator defence, although the mechanisms are unclear. Less progress has been made in explaining why many birds incorporate heterospecific sounds into their sexual displays, and in determining whether these vocalisations are functionally mimetic or by-products of sexual selection for other traits such as repertoire size. Overall, this discussion reveals a more central role for vocal mimicry in the behavioural ecology of birds than has previously been appreciated. The final part of this review identifies important areas for future research. Detailed empirical data are needed on individual species, including on the structure of mimetic signals, the contexts in which mimicry is produced, how mimicry is acquired, and the ecological relationships between mimic, model and receiver. At present, there is little information and no consensus about the various costs of vocal mimicry for the protagonists in the mimicry complex. The diversity and complexity of vocal mimicry in birds raises important questions for the study of animal communication and challenges our view of the nature of mimicry itself. Therefore, a better understanding of avian vocal mimicry is essential if we are to account fully for the diversity of animal signals.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Anastasia H Dalziell
- Division of Evolution, Ecology and Genetics, Research School of Biology, Australian National University, Canberra 0200, Australia
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
11
|
Child MF, Flower TP, Ridley AR. Investigating a link between bill morphology, foraging ecology and kleptoparasitic behaviour in the fork-tailed drongo. Anim Behav 2012. [DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.07.027] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/28/2022]
|
12
|
Flower TP, Gribble M. Kleptoparasitism by attacks versus false alarm calls in fork-tailed drongos. Anim Behav 2012. [DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.11.009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
|
13
|
Barrera JP, Chong L, Judy KN, Blumstein DT. Reliability of public information: predators provide more information about risk than conspecifics. Anim Behav 2011. [DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.01.010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 25] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
|
14
|
Radford AN, Bell MBV, Hollén LI, Ridley AR. Singing for your supper: sentinel calling by kleptoparasites can mitigate the cost to victims. Evolution 2010; 65:900-6. [PMID: 21361918 DOI: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2010.01180.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 35] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
Parasitism generally imposes costs on victims, yet many victims appear to tolerate their parasites. We suggest that in some cases this may be because parasites provide victims with mitigating benefits, paradoxically giving rise to selection for advertisement rather than concealment by parasites. We investigate this possibility using the interaction between an avian kleptoparasite, the fork-tailed drongo (Dicrurus adsimilis), and one of its victims, the pied babbler (Turdoides bicolor). Combining field observations and a playback experiment, we demonstrate that a conspicuous vocal signal broadcast by drongos perched waiting to steal food from foraging babblers allows the latter to improve their own foraging efficiency, although not to the same extent as that experienced in response to conspecific sentinel calling. We argue that "sentinel" calling by drongos may originally have arisen as a means of manipulating babblers: because babblers find more food items and venture into the open more in response to these vocalizations, drongos are presented with more kleptoparasitism opportunities. However, the resulting benefit to babblers could be sufficient to reduce selection for the evolution of defenses against drongos, and the current situation may represent a rare example of an interspecific relationship in transition from a parasitism to a mutualism.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Andrew N Radford
- School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Woodland Road, Bristol, BS8 1UG, United Kingdom.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
15
|
Flower T. Fork-tailed drongos use deceptive mimicked alarm calls to steal food. Proc Biol Sci 2010; 278:1548-55. [PMID: 21047861 DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2010.1932] [Citation(s) in RCA: 87] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Despite the prevalence of vocal mimicry in animals, few functions for this behaviour have been shown. I propose a novel hypothesis that false mimicked alarm calls could be used deceptively to scare other species and steal their food. Studies have previously suggested that animals use their own species-specific alarm calls to steal food. However none have shown conclusively that these false alarms are deceptive, or that mimicked alarm calls are used in this manner. Here, I show that wild fork-tailed drongos (Dicrurus adsimilis) make both drongo-specific and mimicked false alarm calls when watching target species handling food, in response to which targets flee to cover abandoning their food. The drongo-specific and mimicked calls made in false alarms were structurally indistinguishable from calls made during true alarms at predators by drongos and other species. Furthermore, I demonstrate by playback experiments that two of these species, meerkats (Suricata suricatta) and pied babblers (Turdoides bicolor), are deceived by both drongo-specific and mimicked false alarm calls. These results provide the first conclusive evidence that false alarm calls are deceptive and demonstrate a novel function for vocal mimicry. This work also provides valuable insight into the benefits of deploying variable mimetic signals in deceptive communication.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tom Flower
- Department of Zoology, University of Cambridge, Downing Street, Cambridge CB2 3EJ, UK.
| |
Collapse
|
16
|
Interspecific information transfer influences animal community structure. Trends Ecol Evol 2010; 25:354-61. [PMID: 20153073 DOI: 10.1016/j.tree.2010.01.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 210] [Impact Index Per Article: 15.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/10/2009] [Revised: 12/28/2009] [Accepted: 01/11/2010] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
Abstract
Acquiring information from the cues and signals of other species of the same trophic level is widespread among animals, and can help individuals exploit resources and avoid predators. But can such interspecific information transfer also influence the spatial structure of species within communities? Whereas some species use heterospecific information without changing their position, we review research that indicates that heterospecific information is a driving factor in the formation or maintenance of temporary or stable mixed-species groups. Heterospecific information can also influence the organization of such groups, including leadership. Further, animals sometimes select habitats using heterospecific information. We survey interspecific information transfer, and evaluate the morphological, ecological and behavioral factors that make some species information sources and others information seekers.
Collapse
|