1
|
Gantz L, Gispets J, Wilkins A, Gal E. Response to Optimizing the Wilkins Egg and Ball Test: Overcoming Limitations for Accurate Astigmatism Detection [Response To Letter]. Clin Ophthalmol 2024; 18:1049-1051. [PMID: 38616948 PMCID: PMC11012687 DOI: 10.2147/opth.s471148] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/28/2024] [Accepted: 03/29/2024] [Indexed: 04/16/2024] Open
Affiliation(s)
- Liat Gantz
- Department of Optometry and Vision Science, Hadassah Academic College, Jerusalem, Israel
| | - Joan Gispets
- University Vision Centre, Universitat Politècnica de Catalunya, Terrassa, Barcelona, Spain
| | - Arnold Wilkins
- Department of Psychology, University of Essex, Colchester, UK
| | - Eyal Gal
- Department of Optometry and Vision Science, Hadassah Academic College, Jerusalem, Israel
- University Vision Centre, Universitat Politècnica de Catalunya, Terrassa, Barcelona, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Clayton R, Siderov J. Differences in stereoacuity between crossed and uncrossed disparities reduce with practice. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt 2022; 42:1353-1362. [PMID: 35997266 PMCID: PMC9804356 DOI: 10.1111/opo.13040] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/16/2022] [Revised: 07/22/2022] [Accepted: 07/22/2022] [Indexed: 01/05/2023]
Abstract
INTRODUCTION Stereoacuity, like many forms of hyperacuity, improves with practice. We investigated the effects of repeated measurements over multiple visits on stereoacuity using two commonly utilised clinical stereotests, for both crossed and uncrossed disparity stimuli. METHODS Participants were adults with normal binocular vision (n = 17) aged between 18 and 50 years. Stereoacuity was measured using the Randot and TNO stereotests on five separate occasions over a six week period. We utilised both crossed and uncrossed stimuli to separately evaluate stereoacuity in both disparity directions. A subset of the subject group also completed a further five visits over an additional six week period. Threshold stereoacuity was determined by the lowest disparity level at which the subjects could correctly identify both the position and disparity direction (crossed or uncrossed) of the stimulus. Data were analysed by repeated measures analysis of variance. RESULTS Stereoacuity for crossed and uncrossed stimuli improved significantly across the first five visits (F1,21 = 4.24, p = 0.05). The main effect of disparity direction on stereoacuity was not significant (F1 = 0.02, p = 0.91). However, a significant interaction between disparity direction and stereotest was identified (F1 = 7.92, p = 0.01). CONCLUSIONS Stereoacuity measured with both the TNO and Randot stereotests improved significantly over the course of five repetitions. Although differences between crossed and uncrossed stereoacuity were evident, they depended on the stereotest used and reduced or disappeared after repeated measurements. A single measure of stereoacuity is inadequate for properly evaluating adult stereopsis clinically.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Robin Clayton
- Centre for Vision across the Life Span, Department of Optometry and Vision SciencesUniversity of HuddersfieldHuddersfieldUK
| | - John Siderov
- Centre for Vision across the Life Span, Department of Optometry and Vision SciencesUniversity of HuddersfieldHuddersfieldUK
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Morikawa S, Okamoto F, Murakami T, Sugiura Y, Hiraoka T, Okamoto Y, Oshika T. Relationship between stereopsis and vision-related quality of life in patients with branch retinal vein occlusion. BMJ Open Ophthalmol 2022; 7:e000925. [PMID: 35321212 PMCID: PMC8896036 DOI: 10.1136/bmjophth-2021-000925] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/13/2021] [Accepted: 02/22/2022] [Indexed: 11/30/2022] Open
Abstract
Background To investigate the relationship between stereopsis and vision-related quality of life (VR-QOL) in patients with branch retinal vein occlusion (BRVO) before and after treatment with intravitreal ranibizumab (IVR). Methods This prospective multicentred observational study included 37 patients undergoing IVR treatment for unilateral BRVO and 24 age-matched healthy controls. Stereopsis was evaluated using the TNO stereo test (TNO) and Titmus stereo test (TST) every month, and the 25-item National Eye Institute Visual Function Questionnaire (VFQ-25) was administered at baseline, then at 3, 6 and 12 months after treatment. Results Time course of the changes in stereopsis and VR-QOL. IVR treatment significantly reduced central fovea thickness and improved both the TNO and the TST from 2 to 12 months (both p<0.05). Stereopsis before and after IVR injection in the eyes with BRVO were significantly worse than those in control subjects (TNO, p<0.001; TST, p<0.001). The VFQ-25 composite score significantly improved from 3 to 12 months after IVR treatment (p<0.05). Univariate analysis showed that the TNO score at baseline was significantly correlated with the VFQ-25 composite score at baseline and after treatment (p<0.05, p<0.05, respectively). TST score was not associated with the VFQ-25 composite score at baseline or after treatment. Conclusions Treatment with IVR for BRVO improved cystoid macular oedema, which was correlated to improved stereopsis, although not to the control level. The TNO score at baseline was associated with VR-QOL in patients with BRVO.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Shohei Morikawa
- Department of Ophthalmology, Faculty of Medicine, University of Tsukuba, Tsukuba, Ibaraki, Japan
| | - Fumiki Okamoto
- Department of Ophthalmology, Faculty of Medicine, University of Tsukuba, Tsukuba, Ibaraki, Japan
| | - Tomoya Murakami
- Department of Ophthalmology, Faculty of Medicine, University of Tsukuba, Tsukuba, Ibaraki, Japan
| | - Yoshimi Sugiura
- Department of Ophthalmology, Faculty of Medicine, University of Tsukuba, Tsukuba, Ibaraki, Japan
| | - Takahiro Hiraoka
- Department of Ophthalmology, Faculty of Medicine, University of Tsukuba, Tsukuba, Ibaraki, Japan
| | | | - Tetsuro Oshika
- Department of Ophthalmology, Faculty of Medicine, University of Tsukuba, Tsukuba, Ibaraki, Japan
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Portela-Camino JA, Martín-González S, Ruiz-Alcocer J, Illarramendi-Mendicute I, Garrido-Mercado R. An Evaluation of the Agreement Between a Computerized Stereoscopic Game Test and the TNO Stereoacuity Test. CLINICAL OPTOMETRY 2021; 13:181-190. [PMID: 34267572 PMCID: PMC8275165 DOI: 10.2147/opto.s308445] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/04/2021] [Accepted: 06/15/2021] [Indexed: 06/13/2023]
Abstract
PURPOSE Stereo-anomaly is commonly associated with amblyopia. An investigation was conducted to determine whether the measurements of stereoacuity obtained with the stereoacuity reference test (TNO Test) show an agreement with a computer stereoscope video game. METHODS Thirty-two subjects (mean age 9.37±2.00 years) with an amblyopia history were selected for a blind and randomized study of stereoacuity improvement through a new random dot game. A masked examiner measured the stereoacuity three times per subject using the TNO test (at the beginning, at the end and after 6 months of the treatment). A second masked examiner measured stereoacuity using the new computerized game after the TNO masked evaluation. RESULTS The Pearson's correlation coefficient one test against the other was r2 = 0.767 and the Bland-Altman plot was r2= 0.069 (mean difference -0.03 log sec). Using three categories: poor (840-300 seconds of arc), coarse (480-210 seconds of arc) and moderate-fine stereoacuity (210-30 seconds of arc). Positive predictive values were 89.5% for moderate-fine; 72.7% for coarse; and 90.0% for poor stereoacuity. In addition, the agreement was evaluated using the Kappa coefficient (K= 0.743) with a 0.95 confidence interval and lower and upper Kappa limits were (0.628 and 0.858), respectively. Kappa coefficient and limits were still good when analyzing data before (K =0.663, 0.420 and 0.906) and after the treatment (K= 0.765, 0.632 and 0.899). CONCLUSION The Computerized Stereoscopic Game test allows the measure of stereoacuity. It can be used for both the purpose of detecting stereo vision deficits or tracking stereo vision development.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Santiago Martín-González
- Department of Construction and Manufacturing Engineering, University of Oviedo, Oviedo, Asturias, Spain
| | - Javier Ruiz-Alcocer
- Department of Optometry and Vision Science, Complutense University of Madrid, Madrid, Spain
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
5
|
Minkov V, Sawada T. Theoretical Treatment of Limitations Inherent in Simple 3D Stimuli: Triangles and the P3P Problem. Vision (Basel) 2021; 5:vision5010010. [PMID: 33671339 PMCID: PMC7931063 DOI: 10.3390/vision5010010] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/18/2020] [Revised: 02/05/2021] [Accepted: 02/09/2021] [Indexed: 11/10/2022] Open
Abstract
Understanding the visual stimulus in a psychophysical experiment, theoretically, is critical for controlling the experiment, for interpreting the empirical results of the experiment, and for discussing the mechanisms the visual system used to get these results. This fact encourages visual scientists to use “simple” visual stimuli in their experiments. A triangle is one of the simplest stimuli that has been used by psychophysicists to study 3D perception. It has also been used to compose the polygonal meshes that represent complex 3D surfaces in computer graphics. The relationship between the shape, orientation, and retinal image of a triangle has also been studied as the Perspective-3-Point problem (P3P). In this study, the statistical properties of this relationship between the 2D retinal image of a triangle and its recovered 3D orientation were tested in a simulation experiment whose results showed that a triangle is qualitatively different from more complex shapes that have been used to recover 3D information from their retinal images. This raises an important question, namely, how many, if any, inferences about our visual system can be generalized to our perceptions in everyday life when they are based on psychophysical experiments that used very simple visual stimuli such as triangles.
Collapse
|
6
|
Read JCA, Wong ZY, Yek X, Wong YX, Bachtoula O, Llamas-Cornejo I, Serrano-Pedraza I. ASTEROID stereotest v1.0: lower stereo thresholds using smaller, denser and faster dots. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt 2020; 40:815-827. [PMID: 32989799 DOI: 10.1111/opo.12737] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/03/2020] [Accepted: 08/21/2020] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
Abstract
PURPOSE In 2019, we described ASTEROID, a new stereotest run on a 3D tablet computer which involves a four-alternative disparity detection task on a dynamic random-dot stereogram. Stereo thresholds measured with ASTEROID were well correlated with, but systematically higher than (by a factor of around 1.5), thresholds measured with previous laboratory stereotests or the Randot Preschool clinical stereotest. We speculated that this might be due to the relatively large, sparse dots used in ASTEROID v0.9. Here, we introduce and test the stereo thresholds and test-repeatability of the new ASTEROID v1.0, which uses precomputed images to allow stereograms made up of much smaller, denser dots. METHODS Stereo thresholds and test/retest repeatability were tested and compared between the old and new versions of ASTEROID (n = 75) and the Randot Circles (n = 31) stereotest, in healthy young adults. RESULTS Thresholds on ASTEROID v1.0 are lower (better) than on ASTEROID v0.9 by a factor of 1.4, and do not differ significantly from thresholds on the Randot Circles. Thresholds were roughly log-normally distributed with a mean of 1.54 log10 arcsec (35 arcsec) on ASTEROID v1.0 compared to 1.70 log10 arcsec (50 arcsec) on ASTEROID v0.9. The standard deviation between observers was the same for both versions, 0.32 log10 arcsec, corresponding to a factor of 2 above and below the mean. There was no difference between the versions in their test/retest repeatability, with 95% coefficient of repeatability = 0.46 log10 arcsec (a factor of 2.9 or 1.5 octaves) and a Pearson correlation of 0.8 (comparable to other clinical stereotests). CONCLUSION The poorer stereo thresholds previously reported with ASTEROID v0.9 appear to have been due to the relatively large, coarse dots and low density used, rather than to some other aspect of the technology. Employing the small dots and high density used in ASTEROID v1.0, thresholds and test/retest repeatability are similar to other clinical stereotests.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jenny C A Read
- Biosciences Institute, Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK
| | - Zhen Yi Wong
- Biosciences Institute, Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK
| | - Xinye Yek
- Biosciences Institute, Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK
| | - Ying Xin Wong
- Biosciences Institute, Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK
| | - Omar Bachtoula
- Faculty of Psychology, Universidad Complutense de Madrid, Madrid, Spain
| | | | - Ignacio Serrano-Pedraza
- Biosciences Institute, Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK.,Faculty of Psychology, Universidad Complutense de Madrid, Madrid, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Martín S, Portela JA, Ding J, Ibarrondo O, Levi DM. Evaluation of a Virtual Reality implementation of a binocular imbalance test. PLoS One 2020; 15:e0238047. [PMID: 32822405 PMCID: PMC7446887 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0238047] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/26/2020] [Accepted: 08/07/2020] [Indexed: 01/12/2023] Open
Abstract
The purpose of this study was (1) to implement a test for binocular imbalance in a Virtual Reality headset, (2) to assess its testability, reliability and outcomes in a population of clinical patients and (3) to evaluate the relationships of interocular acuity difference, stereoacuity and binocular imbalance to amblyogenic risk factors. 100 volunteers (6 to 70 years old, mean 21.2 ± 16.2), 21 with no amblyogenic risk factors and 79 with amblyopia or a history of amblyopia participated. Participants were classified by amblyogenic risk factor (24 anisometropic, 25 strabismic and 30 mixed) and, for those with strabismus, also by refractive response (16 accommodative and 39 non-accommodative). We characterized our sample using three variables, called the ‘triplet’ henceforth: interocular acuity difference, stereoacuity and imbalance factor. Binocular imbalance showed high test-retest reliability (no significant difference between test and retest in a subgroup, n = 20, p = 0.831); was correlated with Worth 4 dots test (r = 0.538, p<0.0001); and correlated with both interocular acuity difference (r = 0.575, p<0.0001) and stereoacuity (r = 0.675, p<0.0001). The mean values of each variable of the triplet differed depending on group classification. Mixed and non-accommodative groups showed the worst mean values compared with the other groups. Among participants with strabismus, strabismic vs mixed subgroups did not show significant differences in any variable of the triplet, whereas the accommodative vs non-accommodative subgroups showed significant differences in all of them. According to a univariate logistic model, any variable of the triplet provides a good metric for differentiating patients from controls, except for binocular imbalance for anisometropic subgroup. The proposed binocular imbalance test is feasible and reliable. We recommend monitoring amblyopia clinically not only considering visual acuity, but also stereoacuity and interocular imbalance. Stereoacuity on its own fails because of the high percentage of patients with no measurable stereoacuity. Binocular imbalance may help to fill that gap.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Jian Ding
- School of Optometry, University of California, Berkeley, CA, United States of America
| | | | - Dennis M. Levi
- School of Optometry, University of California, Berkeley, CA, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Zhao L, Wu H. The effect of dot size in random-dot stereograms on the results of stereoacuity measurements. BMC Ophthalmol 2020; 20:253. [PMID: 32580763 PMCID: PMC7313195 DOI: 10.1186/s12886-020-01526-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/23/2019] [Accepted: 06/18/2020] [Indexed: 12/02/2022] Open
Abstract
Background This study aimed to evaluate the effect of the size of the dots in random-dot stereograms on the results of stereoacuity measurements. Methods A stereopsis measurement system was created using a phoropter and two 4 K smartphones. Three dot sizes, including 1 × 1 pixel, 6 × 6 pixels, and 10 × 10 pixels (equivalent to 0.17 min arc, 1 min arc, and 1.68 min arc, respectively), were used to form random-dot arrays, and each test pattern had one Lea symbol hidden within it. The resulting stereograms were tested on 30 subjects with normal acuity and stereoacuity. Results Stereoacuity measured with the 1-pixel dots was significantly worse than that measured with the 6-pixel dots (Wilcoxon signed-rank test, Z = -4.903, P < 0.001) and the 10-pixel dots (Z = -4.941, P < 0.001). No significant difference was found between 6-pixel dot and 10-pixel dot stereograms (Z = -1.000, P = 0.317). Conclusion The size of the dots in random-dot stereograms affects the test results significantly when the dots are too small for the eye to resolve.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lingzhi Zhao
- Department of Medical Equipment, the Second Hospital of Jilin University, Changchun, China
| | - Huang Wu
- Department of Optometry, The Second Hospital of Jilin University, No. 218, Ziqiang Street, Nanguan District, Changchun, 130041, China.
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Ocansey S, Osuobeni E, Siderov J. Lateral interference, effects of flankers and reference bar configuration on foveal depth discrimination thresholds. Vision Res 2019; 156:96-104. [PMID: 30738788 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2018.08.008] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/24/2017] [Revised: 08/20/2018] [Accepted: 08/27/2018] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Stephen Ocansey
- Anglia Vision Research, Department of Vision and Hearing Sciences, Anglia Ruskin University, Cambridge, United Kingdom.
| | - Ebi Osuobeni
- Anglia Vision Research, Department of Vision and Hearing Sciences, Anglia Ruskin University, Cambridge, United Kingdom
| | - John Siderov
- Anglia Vision Research, Department of Vision and Hearing Sciences, Anglia Ruskin University, Cambridge, United Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Abstract
PURPOSE Reports of dissimilar stereothresholds for contour and random-dot (RD) targets may reflect differences in stimulus properties or differences between local and global stereoscopic processing mechanisms. In this study, we evaluated whether the stereothresholds obtained using low- and high-density RD stimuli are consistent with a distinction between local and global disparity processing. METHODS Stereothresholds were measured in eight normal subjects for a small disparate line segment superimposed on RD surrounds with densities that ranged between 0.07 and 28.3%. RESULTS Stereothresholds averaged 0.23 arc min for an RD density of 0.39% and approximately doubled for lower and higher densities. The increase in stereothresholds at low densities is likely because of the increased spacing between elements, which reduces their usefulness as a reference for relative disparity judgments. The increase in stereothresholds at high densities is attributed to a crowding effect. CONCLUSIONS Because the stereothresholds measured with RD stimuli of low and high density are limited by different constraints, they can be considered to be different types of stereotargets. However, because the stereothresholds measured for RD targets of varying densities are similar to those determined previously for a local, two-rod stereotarget, it is likely that all these stimuli are processed by a single disparity-processing mechanism.
Collapse
|
11
|
Gantz L, Bedell HE. Transfer of perceptual learning of depth discrimination between local and global stereograms. Vision Res 2010; 50:1891-9. [PMID: 20600234 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2010.06.011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/19/2009] [Revised: 06/21/2010] [Accepted: 06/21/2010] [Indexed: 10/19/2022]
Abstract
Several previous studies reported differences when stereothresholds are assessed with local-contour stereograms vs. complex random-dot stereograms (RDSs). Dissimilar thresholds may be due to differences in the properties of the stereograms (e.g. spatial frequency content, contrast, inter-element separation, area) or to different underlying processing mechanisms. This study examined the transfer of perceptual learning of depth discrimination between local and global RDSs with similar properties, and vice versa. If global and local stereograms are processed by separate neural mechanisms, then the magnitude and rate of training for the two types of stimuli are likely to differ, and the transfer of training from one stimulus type to the other should be minimal. Based on previous results, we chose RDSs with element densities of 0.17% and 28.3% to serve as the local and global stereograms, respectively. Fourteen inexperienced subjects with normal binocular vision were randomly assigned to either a local- or global- RDS training group. Stereothresholds for both stimulus types were measured before and after 7700 training trials distributed over 10 sessions. Stereothresholds for the trained condition improve for approximately 3000 trials, by an average of 0.36+/-0.08 for local and 0.29+/-0.10 for global RDSs, and level off thereafter. Neither the rate nor the magnitude of improvement differ statistically between the local- and global-training groups. Further, no significant difference exists in the amount of improvement on the trained vs. the untrained targets for either training group. These results are consistent with the operation of a single mechanism to process both local and global stereograms.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Liat Gantz
- College of Optometry, University of Houston, 505 J. Davis Armistead Building, Houston, TX 77204-2020, USA.
| | | |
Collapse
|