1
|
Fernández-López M, Perea M. A Letter is a Letter and its Co-Occurrences: Cracking the Emergence of Position-Invariance Processing. Psychon Bull Rev 2023; 30:2328-2337. [PMID: 37145389 PMCID: PMC10728230 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-023-02265-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 02/27/2023] [Indexed: 05/06/2023]
Abstract
Visual word recognition requires encoding letter identities and positions (orthographic processing). The present study focuses on the emergence of the mechanism responsible for encoding letter order in a word: position invariance. Reading experience leads to developing a flexible mechanism that encodes the information of the position of letters, explaining why jugde and judge are easily confused. Critically, orthographic regularities (e.g., frequent letter co-occurrences) modulate letter position encoding: the pseudoword mohter is extremely similar to mother because, in middle positions, the bigram TH is much more frequent than HT. Here, we tested whether position invariance emerges rapidly after the exposition to orthographic regularities-bigrams-in a novel script. To that end, we designed a study with two phases. In Phase 1, following Chetail (2017; Experiment 1b, Cognition, 163, 103-120), individuals were first exposed to a flow of artificial words for a few minutes, with four bigrams occurring frequently. Afterward, participants judged the strings with trained bigrams as more wordlike (i.e., readers quickly picked up subtle new orthographic regularities) than the strings with untrained bigrams, replicating Chetail (2017). In Phase 2, participants performed a same-different matching task in which they had to decide whether pairs of five-letter strings were the same or not. The critical comparison was between pairs with a transposition of letters in a frequent (trained) versus infrequent (untrained) bigram. Results showed that participants were more prone to make errors with frequent bigrams than with infrequent bigrams with a letter transposition. These findings reveal that position invariance emerges rapidly, after continuous exposure to orthographic regularities.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Maria Fernández-López
- Department of Methodology of Behavioral Sciences and ERI-Lectura, Univesity of València, Facultat de Psicologia i Logopèdia, Blasco Ibáñez Avenue 21, 46010, València, Spain.
| | - Manuel Perea
- Department of Methodology of Behavioral Sciences and ERI-Lectura, Univesity of València, Facultat de Psicologia i Logopèdia, Blasco Ibáñez Avenue 21, 46010, València, Spain
- Nebrija University, Madrid, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Perea M, Marcet A, Baciero A, Gómez P. Reading about a RELO-VUTION. PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2022; 87:1306-1321. [PMID: 35948686 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-022-01720-9] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/26/2022] [Accepted: 07/23/2022] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
Pseudowords created by transposing two letters of words (e.g., MOHTER; CHOLOCATE) are highly confusable with their base word; this is known as the transposed-letter similarity effect. In this work, we examined whether transposed-letter effects occur when words span more than one line (e.g., CHOLO- in one line and CATE in another line; note that the transposed letters L and C are in different lines). While this type of presentation is not the canonical format for reading in alphabetic languages, it is widely used in advertising, billboards, and street signs. Transposed-letter pseudowords and their replacement-letter controls were written in the standard one-line format versus a two-line format (Experiments 1-2) or a syllable-per-line format (Experiment 3). While results showed some decrease in the transposed-letter effect in the two-line and syllabic formats, the transposed-letter effect was still substantial in the accuracy of responses. These findings demonstrate that even when the letters being transposed are relatively far apart in space, the transposed-letter effect is still robust. Thus, a major component of letter position coding occurs at an abstract level.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Manuel Perea
- Departamento de Metodología and ERI-Lectura, Universitat de València, Valencia, Spain
- Centro de Investigación en Cognición, Universidad Antonio de Nebrija, Madrid, Spain
| | - Ana Marcet
- Grupo de Investigación en Enseñanza de Lenguas, Departamento de Didáctica de la Lengua y la Literatura, Universitat de València, Valencia, Spain
| | - Ana Baciero
- Department of Psychology, Bournemouth University, Bournemouth, UK
| | - Pablo Gómez
- Psychology Department, California State University San Bernardino, Palm Desert Campus, 37500 Cook Street, Palm Desert, CA, 92211, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Fernandes T, Araújo S. From Hand to Eye With the Devil In-Between: Which Cognitive Mechanisms Underpin the Benefit From Handwriting Training When Learning Visual Graphs? Front Psychol 2021; 12:736507. [PMID: 34777123 PMCID: PMC8578702 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.736507] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/05/2021] [Accepted: 10/06/2021] [Indexed: 11/28/2022] Open
Abstract
Cognitive science has recently shown a renewed interest on the benefit from training in handwriting (HW) when learning visual graphs, given that this learning experience improves more subsequent visual graph recognition than other forms of training. However, the underlying cognitive mechanism of this HW benefit has been elusive. Building on the 50 years of research on this topic, the present work outlines a theoretical approach to study this mechanism, specifying testable hypotheses that will allow distinguishing between confronting perspectives, i.e., symbolic accounts that hold that perceptual learning and visual analysis underpin the benefit from HW training vs. embodied sensorimotor accounts that argue for motoric representations as inner part of orthographic representations acquired via HW training. From the evidence critically revisited, we concluded that symbolic accounts are parsimonious and could better explain the benefit from HW training when learning visual graphs. The future challenge will be to put at test the detailed predictions presented here, so that the devil has no longer room in this equation.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Tânia Fernandes
- Faculdade de Psicologia, Universidade de Lisboa, Lisbon, Portugal
| | - Susana Araújo
- Faculdade de Psicologia, Universidade de Lisboa, Lisbon, Portugal
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Fernández-López M, Gómez P, Perea M. Which Factors Modulate Letter Position Coding in Pre-literate Children? Front Psychol 2021; 12:708274. [PMID: 34421758 PMCID: PMC8375292 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.708274] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/11/2021] [Accepted: 07/13/2021] [Indexed: 12/03/2022] Open
Abstract
One of the central landmarks of learning to read is the emergence of orthographic processing (i.e., the encoding of letter identity and letter order): it constitutes the necessary link between the low-level stages of visual processing and the higher-level processing of words. Regarding the processing of letter position, many experiments have shown worse performance in various tasks for the transposed-letter pair judge-JUDGE than for the orthographic control jupte-JUDGE. Importantly, 4-y.o. pre-literate children also show letter transposition effects in a same-different task: TZ-ZT is more error-prone than TZ-PH. Here, we examined whether this effect with pre-literate children is related to the cognitive and linguistic skills required to learn to read. Specifically, we examined the relation of the transposed-letter in a same-different task with the scores of these children in phonological, alphabetic and metalinguistic awareness, linguistic skills, and basic cognitive processes. To that end, we used a standardized battery to assess the abilities related with early reading acquisition. Results showed that the size of the transposed-letter effect in pre-literate children was strongly associated with the sub-test on basic cognitive processes (i.e., memory and perception) but not with the other sub-tests. Importantly, identifying children who may need a pre-literacy intervention is crucial to minimize eventual reading difficulties. We discuss how this marker can be used as a tool to anticipate reading difficulties in beginning readers.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- María Fernández-López
- Department of Methodology of Behavioral Sciences and ERI-Lectura, Universitat de València, València, Spain
| | - Pablo Gómez
- Department of Psychology, California State University, San Bernardino, Palm Desert, CA, United States
| | - Manuel Perea
- Department of Methodology of Behavioral Sciences and ERI-Lectura, Universitat de València, València, Spain.,Center of Research in Cognition, Universidad Antonio de Nebrija, Madrid, Spain
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Kim J, Jung J, Nam K. Neural correlates of confusability in recognition of morphologically complex Korean words. PLoS One 2021; 16:e0249111. [PMID: 33857191 PMCID: PMC8049294 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0249111] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/15/2019] [Accepted: 03/11/2021] [Indexed: 11/30/2022] Open
Abstract
When people confuse and reject a non-word that is created by switching two adjacent letters from an actual word, is called the transposition confusability effect (TCE). The TCE is known to occur at the very early stages of visual word recognition with such unit exchange as letters or syllables, but little is known about the brain mechanisms of TCE. In this study, we examined the neural correlates of TCE and the effect of a morpheme boundary placement on TCE. We manipulated the placement of a morpheme boundary by exchanging places of two syllables embedded in Korean morphologically complex words made up of lexical morpheme and grammatical morpheme. In the two experimental conditions, the transposition syllable within-boundary condition (TSW) involved exchanging two syllables within the same morpheme, whereas the across-boundary condition (TSA) involved the exchange of syllables across the stem and grammatical morpheme boundary. During fMRI, participants performed the lexical decision task. Behavioral results revealed that the TCE was found in TSW condition, and the morpheme boundary, which is manipulated in TSA, modulated the TCE. In the fMRI results, TCE induced activation in the left inferior parietal lobe (IPL) and intraparietal sulcus (IPS). The IPS activation was specific to a TCE and its strength of activation was associated with task performance. Furthermore, two functional networks were involved in the TCE: the central executive network and the dorsal attention network. Morpheme boundary modulation suppressed the TCE by recruiting the prefrontal and temporal regions, which are the key regions involved in semantic processing. Our findings propose the role of the dorsal visual pathway in syllable position processing and that its interaction with other higher cognitive systems is modulated by the morphological boundary in the early phases of visual word recognition.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jeahong Kim
- Department of Psychology, Korea University, Seoul, Republic of Korea
| | - JeYoung Jung
- School of Psychology, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, United Kingdom
| | - Kichun Nam
- Department of Psychology, Korea University, Seoul, Republic of Korea
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Letter and word identification in the fovea and parafovea. Atten Percept Psychophys 2021; 83:2071-2082. [PMID: 33748904 PMCID: PMC8213579 DOI: 10.3758/s13414-021-02273-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 02/02/2021] [Indexed: 12/02/2022]
Abstract
We investigated the extent to which accuracy in word identification in foveal and parafoveal vision is determined by variations in the visibility of the component letters of words. To do so we measured word identification accuracy in displays of three three-letter words, one on fixation and the others to the left and right of the central word. We also measured accuracy in identifying the component letters of these words when presented at the same location in a context of three three-letter nonword sequences. In the word identification block, accuracy was highest for central targets and significantly greater for words to the right compared with words to the left. In the letter identification block, we found an extended W-shaped function across all nine letters, with greatest accuracy for the three central letters and for the first and last letter in the complete sequence. Further analyses revealed significant correlations between average letter identification per nonword position and word identification at the corresponding position. We conclude that letters are processed in parallel across a sequence of three three-letter words, hence enabling parallel word identification when letter identification accuracy is high enough.
Collapse
|
7
|
Perea M, Baciero A, Marcet A, Fernández-López M, Gómez P. Do Grading Gray Stimuli Help to Encode Letter Position? Vision (Basel) 2021; 5:12. [PMID: 33806403 PMCID: PMC8005957 DOI: 10.3390/vision5010012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/27/2020] [Revised: 02/04/2021] [Accepted: 03/01/2021] [Indexed: 11/16/2022] Open
Abstract
Numerous experiments in the past decades recurrently showed that a transposed-letter pseudoword (e.g., JUGDE) is much more wordlike than a replacement-letter control (e.g., JUPTE). Critically, there is an ongoing debate as to whether this effect arises at a perceptual level (e.g., perceptual uncertainty at assigning letter position of an array of visual objects) or at an abstract language-specific level (e.g., via a level of "open bigrams" between the letter and word levels). Here, we designed an experiment to test the limits of perceptual accounts of letter position coding. The stimuli in a lexical decision task were presented either with a homogeneous letter intensity or with a graded gray intensity, which indicated an unambiguous letter order. The pseudowords were either transposed-letter pseudowords or replaced-letter pseudowords (e.g., jugde vs. jupte). The results showed much longer response times and substantially more errors in the transposed-letter pseudowords than in the replacement-letter pseudowords, regardless of visual format. These findings favor the idea that language-specific orthographic element factors play an essential role when encoding letter position during word recognition.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Manuel Perea
- Departamento de Metodología and ERI-Lectura, Universitat de València, 46010 Valencia, Spain;
- Centro de Ciencia Cognitiva, Universidad Antonio de Nebrija, 28015 Madrid, Spain;
| | - Ana Baciero
- Centro de Ciencia Cognitiva, Universidad Antonio de Nebrija, 28015 Madrid, Spain;
| | - Ana Marcet
- Departamento de Didáctica de la Lengua y la Literatura, Universitat de València, 46022 Valencia, Spain;
| | - María Fernández-López
- Departamento de Metodología and ERI-Lectura, Universitat de València, 46010 Valencia, Spain;
| | - Pablo Gómez
- Department of Psychology, Palm Desert Campus, California State University, San Bernardino, CA 92407, USA;
| |
Collapse
|