1
|
Kurupınar M, Serbest O, Yılmaz D, Soley G. Children's expectations about the stability of others' knowledge and preference states. J Exp Child Psychol 2024; 240:105834. [PMID: 38183878 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2023.105834] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/19/2023] [Revised: 11/06/2023] [Accepted: 12/04/2023] [Indexed: 01/08/2024]
Abstract
It is a crucial ability to predict others' psychological states across time and contexts. Focusing on cultural inventions such as songs and stories, we contrasted children's attributions of stability with others' knowledge and preference states across time and space and whether these attributions change as a function of children's familiarity with the known/liked items. Children (91 4-year-olds and 97 6-year-olds) were introduced to characters who knew or liked a song, a story, a game and a dance that were either novel or familiar. Children were asked whether the characters would still know/like these when they move to another city or when they grow up to be an adult. Both age groups expected these attributes to be more durable in the moving scenario compared with the growing-up scenario, but this trend became more robust with age. Whereas overall children did not judge knowledge as more durable than preferences, children found knowledge to be more enduring with age. The 6-year-olds' stability attributions also increased when known/liked items were familiar. These results suggest that, across the preschool years, children become more nuanced in their predictions about the future forms of knowledge and preference states.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mahmut Kurupınar
- Department of Psychology, Boğaziçi University, Bebek, 34342 Istanbul, Turkey
| | - Oya Serbest
- Department of Psychology, Boğaziçi University, Bebek, 34342 Istanbul, Turkey; Department of Psychology, University of California, Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, CA 93106, USA
| | - Duygu Yılmaz
- Department of Psychology, Boğaziçi University, Bebek, 34342 Istanbul, Turkey; Department of Psychology, New York University
| | - Gaye Soley
- Department of Psychology, Boğaziçi University, Bebek, 34342 Istanbul, Turkey; Department of Cognition, Development and Educational Psychology, University of Barcelona.
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Bridgers S, De Simone C, Gweon H, Ruggeri A. Children seek help based on how others learn. Child Dev 2023; 94:1259-1280. [PMID: 37185813 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.13926] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/01/2020] [Revised: 01/02/2023] [Accepted: 01/24/2023] [Indexed: 05/17/2023]
Abstract
Do children consider how others learned when seeking help? Across three experiments, German children (N = 536 3-to-8 year olds, 49% female, majority White, tested 2017-2019) preferred to learn from successful active learners selectively by context: They sought help solving a problem from a learner who had independently discovered the solution to a previous problem over those who had learned through instruction or observation, but only when the current problem was novel, yet related, to the learners' problem (Experiment 1). Older, but not younger, children preferred the active learner even when she was offered help (Experiment 2), though only when her discovery was deliberate (Experiment 3). Although a preference to learn from successful active learners emerges early, a genuine appreciation for process beyond outcome increases across childhood.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sophie Bridgers
- Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, California, USA
| | - Costanza De Simone
- Max Planck Research Group iSearch, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany
| | - Hyowon Gweon
- Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, California, USA
| | - Azzurra Ruggeri
- Max Planck Research Group iSearch, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Aboody R, Huey H, Jara-Ettinger J. Preschoolers decide who is knowledgeable, who to inform, and who to trust via a causal understanding of how knowledge relates to action. Cognition 2022; 228:105212. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2022.105212] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/11/2021] [Revised: 05/12/2022] [Accepted: 06/23/2022] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
|
4
|
Lockhart KL, Goddu MK, Keil FC. How much can you learn in one year? How content, pedagogical resources, and learner’s age influence beliefs about knowledge acquisition. COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT 2021. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2021.101115] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
|
5
|
Ballantyne N. Recent work on intellectual humility: A philosopher’s perspective. THE JOURNAL OF POSITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2021. [DOI: 10.1080/17439760.2021.1940252] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/20/2022]
|
6
|
Soley G, Köseler B. The social meaning of common knowledge across development. Cognition 2021; 215:104811. [PMID: 34153925 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104811] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/19/2020] [Revised: 06/06/2021] [Accepted: 06/10/2021] [Indexed: 10/21/2022]
Abstract
Common knowledge can be a potent sign of shared social attributes among people, but not all knowledge is socially meaningful to the same extent. For instance, compared to shared knowledge of cultural practices, knowledge of self-evident facts might be a poorer indicator of shared group membership among individuals. Two studies explored adults' and 6-to-9 years old children's social inferences based on what others know as well as their sensitivity to the distinctions in the diagnostic potential of different kinds of knowledge. Participants were presented with targets who were knowledgeable about familiar things that are either culture-specific (e.g., a traditional dance) or general (e.g., a self-evident fact), and asked to make inferences about their language and where they live. Adults and 8-year-olds privileged culture-specific knowledge over general knowledge when making both kinds of inferences about the targets, whereas 6-year-olds did not distinguish between the two knowledge types. Thus, what others know is socially meaningful from early in life, and across development, children become increasingly aware of the diagnostic potential of culture-specific knowledge when making social inferences about others. These findings suggest novel social implications of knowledge assessment.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gaye Soley
- Department of Psychology, Boğaziçi University, Istanbul, Turkey.
| | - Begüm Köseler
- Department of Psychology, Boğaziçi University, Istanbul, Turkey
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Richardson E, Sheskin M, Keil FC. An Illusion of Self-Sufficiency for Learning About Artifacts in Scaffolded Learners, But Not Observers. Child Dev 2021; 92:1523-1538. [PMID: 33458814 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.13506] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
Two studies ask whether scaffolded children (n = 243, 5-6 years and 9-10 years) recognize that assistance is needed to learn to use complex artifacts. In Study 1, children were asked to learn to use a toy pantograph. While children recognized the need for assistance for indirect knowledge, 70% of scaffolded children claimed that they would have learned to use the artifact without assistance, even though 0% of children actually succeeded without assistance. In Study 2, this illusion of self-sufficiency was significantly attenuated when observing another learner being scaffolded. Learners may fail to appreciate artifacts' opacity because self-directed exploration can be partially informative, such that learning to use artifacts is typically scaffolded instead of taught explicitly.
Collapse
|
8
|
Liberman Z, Gerdin E, Kinzler KD, Shaw A. (Un)common knowledge: Children use social relationships to determine who knows what. Dev Sci 2020; 23:e12962. [PMID: 32159917 DOI: 10.1111/desc.12962] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/08/2019] [Revised: 12/16/2019] [Accepted: 02/27/2020] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
Socially savvy individuals track what they know and what other people likely know, and they use this information to navigate the social world. We examine whether children expect people to have shared knowledge based on their social relationships (e.g., expecting friends to know each other's secrets, expecting members of the same cultural group to share cultural knowledge) and we compare children's reasoning about shared knowledge to their reasoning about common knowledge (e.g., the wrongness of moral violations). In three studies, we told 4- to 9-year-olds (N = 227) about what a child knew and asked who else knew the information: The child's friend (Studies 1-3), the child's schoolmate (Study 1), another child from the same national group (Study 2), or the child's sibling (Study 3). In all three studies, older children reliably used relationships to infer what other people knew. Moreover, with age, children increasingly considered both the type of knowledge and an individual's social relationships when reporting who knew what. The results provide support for a 'Selective Inferences' hypothesis and suggest that children's early attention to social relationships facilitates an understanding of how knowledge transfers - an otherwise challenging cognitive process.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Zoe Liberman
- Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of California, Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, CA, USA
| | - Emily Gerdin
- Department of Psychology, Yale University, New Haven, CT, USA
| | | | - Alex Shaw
- Department of Psychology, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Soley G. What Do Group Members Share? The Privileged Status of Cultural Knowledge for Children. Cogn Sci 2019. [DOI: 10.1111/cogs.12786] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Gaye Soley
- Department of Psychology Bogazici University
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Lockhart KL, Chuey A, Kerr S, Keil FC. The Privileged Status of Knowing Mechanistic Information: An Early Epistemic Bias. Child Dev 2019; 90:1772-1788. [PMID: 31106424 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.13246] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
Four studies with 180 5-7 year olds, 165 8-11 year olds and 199 adults show that young children appreciate the distinctive role played by mechanistic explanations in tracking causal patterns. Young children attributed greater knowledge to individuals offering mechanistic reasons for a claim than others who provide equally detailed nonmechanistic reasons. In Study 1, 5-7 year olds attributed greater knowledge to those offering mechanistic reasons. In Studies 2 and 3, all ages (5-7 and adults for Study 2; 5-7, 8-11 and adults for Study 3) assigned greater knowledge to those offering mechanistic reasons about causally central features than those offering nonmechanistic reasons. In Study 4, all ages (5-7, 8-11, adults) modulated the epistemic bias as a function of embedding goals.
Collapse
|
11
|
Marble KE, Boseovski JJ. Children’s Judgments of Cultural Expertise: The Influence of Cultural Status and Learning Method. The Journal of Genetic Psychology 2019; 180:17-30. [DOI: 10.1080/00221325.2018.1562418] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Kimberly E. Marble
- Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina at Greensboro, Greensboro, North Carolina, USA
| | - Janet J. Boseovski
- Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina at Greensboro, Greensboro, North Carolina, USA
| |
Collapse
|
12
|
Children's Developing Ideas About Knowledge and Its Acquisition. ADVANCES IN CHILD DEVELOPMENT AND BEHAVIOR 2018; 54:123-151. [PMID: 29455861 DOI: 10.1016/bs.acdb.2017.10.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
We review key aspects of young children's concept of knowledge. First, we discuss children's early insights into the way that information can be communicated from informant to recipient as well as their active search for information via questions. We then analyze the way that preschool children talk explicitly and cogently about knowledge and the presuppositions they make in doing so. We argue that all children, irrespective of culture and language, eventually arrive at the same fundamental conception of knowledge in the preschool years. Nevertheless, despite the universality of this basic conception, young children are likely to show considerable variation in their pattern of information seeking, depending on the conversational practices of their family and culture.
Collapse
|