1
|
Cantor AR. " Yo trato de no llorar": Rethinking Obstetric Violence in Costa Rica. Med Anthropol 2023; 42:163-176. [PMID: 36692941 DOI: 10.1080/01459740.2023.2166410] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/25/2023]
Abstract
Obstetric violence is an emergent paradigm that uses gender-based violence to frame traumatic childbirth. Despite its growing popularity in the literature, it may not adequately address the nuanced ways that all actors experience these interactions. While Costa Rica adopted a nationally endorsed humane birthing policy, the semi-structured interviews on which I draw in this article show that health care personnel continue to dehumanize and objectify women; experiences considered characteristic of obstetric violence. However, women's own interpretations of their experiences are not aligned with definitions of obstetric violence. This lacuna in praxis highlights the need to critically reevaluate how birth trauma is conceptualized within a contemporary context.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Allison R Cantor
- Department of Anthropology, New Mexico State University, Las Cruces, New Mexico, USA
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Barnett SA, Griffiths TL, Hawkins RD. A Pragmatic Account of the Weak Evidence Effect. OPEN MIND 2022; 6:169-182. [DOI: 10.1162/opmi_a_00061] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/08/2021] [Accepted: 07/18/2022] [Indexed: 11/04/2022] Open
Abstract
Abstract
Language is not only used for neutral information; we often seek to persuade by arguing in favor of a particular view. Persuasion raises a number of challenges for classical accounts of belief updating, as information cannot be taken at face value. How should listeners account for a speaker’s “hidden agenda” when incorporating new information? Here, we extend recent probabilistic models of recursive social reasoning to allow for persuasive goals and show that our model provides a pragmatic account for why weakly favorable arguments may backfire, a phenomenon known as the weak evidence effect. Critically, this model predicts a systematic relationship between belief updates and expectations about the information source: weak evidence should only backfire when speakers are expected to act under persuasive goals and prefer the strongest evidence. We introduce a simple experimental paradigm called the Stick Contest to measure the extent to which the weak evidence effect depends on speaker expectations, and show that a pragmatic listener model accounts for the empirical data better than alternative models. Our findings suggest further avenues for rational models of social reasoning to illuminate classical decision-making phenomena.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Samuel A. Barnett
- Department of Computer Science, Princeton University, Princeton, New Jersey
| | - Thomas L. Griffiths
- Department of Computer Science, Princeton University, Princeton, New Jersey
- Department of Psychology, Princeton University, Princeton, New Jersey
| | - Robert D. Hawkins
- Department of Psychology, Princeton University, Princeton, New Jersey
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Ambridge B, Doherty L, Maitreyee R, Tatsumi T, Zicherman S, Mateo Pedro P, Kawakami A, Bidgood A, Pye C, Narasimhan B, Arnon I, Bekman D, Efrati A, Fabiola Can Pixabaj S, Marroquín Pelíz M, Julajuj Mendoza M, Samanta S, Campbell S, McCauley S, Berman R, Misra Sharma D, Bhaya Nair R, Fukumura K. Testing a computational model of causative overgeneralizations: Child judgment and production data from English, Hebrew, Hindi, Japanese and K'iche'. OPEN RESEARCH EUROPE 2022; 1:1. [PMID: 37645154 PMCID: PMC10446094 DOI: 10.12688/openreseurope.13008.2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 12/21/2021] [Indexed: 08/31/2023]
Abstract
How do language learners avoid the production of verb argument structure overgeneralization errors ( *The clown laughed the man c.f. The clown made the man laugh), while retaining the ability to apply such generalizations productively when appropriate? This question has long been seen as one that is both particularly central to acquisition research and particularly challenging. Focussing on causative overgeneralization errors of this type, a previous study reported a computational model that learns, on the basis of corpus data and human-derived verb-semantic-feature ratings, to predict adults' by-verb preferences for less- versus more-transparent causative forms (e.g., * The clown laughed the man vs The clown made the man laugh) across English, Hebrew, Hindi, Japanese and K'iche Mayan. Here, we tested the ability of this model (and an expanded version with multiple hidden layers) to explain binary grammaticality judgment data from children aged 4;0-5;0, and elicited-production data from children aged 4;0-5;0 and 5;6-6;6 ( N=48 per language). In general, the model successfully simulated both children's judgment and production data, with correlations of r=0.5-0.6 and r=0.75-0.85, respectively, and also generalized to unseen verbs. Importantly, learners of all five languages showed some evidence of making the types of overgeneralization errors - in both judgments and production - previously observed in naturalistic studies of English (e.g., *I'm dancing it). Together with previous findings, the present study demonstrates that a simple learning model can explain (a) adults' continuous judgment data, (b) children's binary judgment data and (c) children's production data (with no training of these datasets), and therefore constitutes a plausible mechanistic account of the acquisition of verbs' argument structure restrictions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ben Ambridge
- University of Liverpool, Liverpool, UK
- ESRC International Centre for Language and Communicative Development (LuCiD), Liverpool, UK
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | - Inbal Arnon
- Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Jerusalem, Israel
| | - Dani Bekman
- Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Jerusalem, Israel
| | - Amir Efrati
- Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Jerusalem, Israel
| | | | | | | | - Soumitra Samanta
- University of Liverpool, Liverpool, UK
- ESRC International Centre for Language and Communicative Development (LuCiD), Liverpool, UK
| | | | | | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
4
|
Ambridge B, Maitreyee R, Tatsumi T, Doherty L, Zicherman S, Pedro PM, Bannard C, Samanta S, McCauley S, Arnon I, Bekman D, Efrati A, Berman R, Narasimhan B, Sharma DM, Nair RB, Fukumura K, Campbell S, Pye C, Pixabaj SFC, Pelíz MM, Mendoza MJ. The crosslinguistic acquisition of sentence structure: Computational modeling and grammaticality judgments from adult and child speakers of English, Japanese, Hindi, Hebrew and K'iche'. Cognition 2020; 202:104310. [PMID: 32623135 PMCID: PMC7397526 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104310] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/17/2019] [Revised: 03/31/2020] [Accepted: 04/13/2020] [Indexed: 11/16/2022]
Abstract
This preregistered study tested three theoretical proposals for how children form productive yet restricted linguistic generalizations, avoiding errors such as *The clown laughed the man, across three age groups (5-6 years, 9-10 years, adults) and five languages (English, Japanese, Hindi, Hebrew and K'iche'). Participants rated, on a five-point scale, correct and ungrammatical sentences describing events of causation (e.g., *Someone laughed the man; Someone made the man laugh; Someone broke the truck; ?Someone made the truck break). The verb-semantics hypothesis predicts that, for all languages, by-verb differences in acceptability ratings will be predicted by the extent to which the causing and caused event (e.g., amusing and laughing) merge conceptually into a single event (as rated by separate groups of adult participants). The entrenchment and preemption hypotheses predict, for all languages, that by-verb differences in acceptability ratings will be predicted by, respectively, the verb's relative overall frequency, and frequency in nearly-synonymous constructions (e.g., X made Y laugh for *Someone laughed the man). Analysis using mixed effects models revealed that entrenchment/preemption effects (which could not be distinguished due to collinearity) were observed for all age groups and all languages except K'iche', which suffered from a thin corpus and showed only preemption sporadically. All languages showed effects of event-merge semantics, except K'iche' which showed only effects of supplementary semantic predictors. We end by presenting a computational model which successfully simulates this pattern of results in a single discriminative-learning mechanism, achieving by-verb correlations of around r = 0.75 with human judgment data.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ben Ambridge
- University of Liverpool, United Kingdom of Great Britain and Northern Ireland; ESRC International Centre for Language and Communicative Development (LuCiD).
| | - Ramya Maitreyee
- University of Liverpool, United Kingdom of Great Britain and Northern Ireland
| | | | - Laura Doherty
- University of Liverpool, United Kingdom of Great Britain and Northern Ireland
| | | | | | - Colin Bannard
- University of Liverpool, United Kingdom of Great Britain and Northern Ireland
| | - Soumitra Samanta
- University of Liverpool, United Kingdom of Great Britain and Northern Ireland; ESRC International Centre for Language and Communicative Development (LuCiD)
| | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | | - Kumiko Fukumura
- University of Stirling, United Kingdom of Great Britain and Northern Ireland
| | | | - Clifton Pye
- University of Kansas, United States of America
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
5
|
Hahn U. Argument Quality in Real World Argumentation. Trends Cogn Sci 2020; 24:363-374. [PMID: 32298622 DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2020.01.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/28/2019] [Revised: 01/13/2020] [Accepted: 01/17/2020] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
Abstract
The idea of resolving dispute through the exchange of arguments and reasons has been central to society for millennia. We exchange arguments as a way of getting at the truth in contexts as diverse as science, the court room, and our everyday lives. In democracies, political decisions should be negotiated through argument, not deception, or even worse, brute force. If argument is to lead to the truth or to good decisions, then some arguments must be better than others and 'argument strength' must have some meaningful connection with truth. Can argument strength be measured in a way that tracks an objective relationship with truth and not just mere persuasiveness? This article describes recent developments in providing such measures.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ulrike Hahn
- Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck College, University of London, London, UK.
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Hayes BK, Banner S, Forrester S, Navarro DJ. Selective sampling and inductive inference: Drawing inferences based on observed and missing evidence. Cogn Psychol 2019; 113:101221. [PMID: 31200210 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2019.05.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/14/2018] [Revised: 04/11/2019] [Accepted: 05/14/2019] [Indexed: 11/26/2022]
Abstract
We propose and test a Bayesian model of property induction with evidence that has been selectively sampled leading to "censoring" or exclusion of potentially relevant data. A core model prediction is that identical evidence samples can lead to different patterns of inductive inference depending on the censoring mechanisms that cause some instances to be excluded. This prediction was confirmed in four experiments examining property induction following exposure to identical samples that were subject to different sampling frames. Each experiment found narrower generalization of a novel property when the sample instances were selected because they shared a common property (property sampling) than when they were selected because they belonged to the same category (category sampling). In line with model predictions, sampling frame effects were moderated by the addition of explicit negative evidence (Experiment 1), sample size (Experiment 2) and category base rates (Experiments 3-4). These data show that reasoners are sensitive to constraints on the sampling process when making property inferences; they consider both the observed evidence and the reasons why certain types of evidence has not been observed.
Collapse
|
7
|
Hertlein KM, Dulley C, Cloud R, Leon D, Chang J. Does absence of evidence mean evidence of absence? Managing the issue of partner surveillance in infidelity treatment. SEXUAL AND RELATIONSHIP THERAPY 2017. [DOI: 10.1080/14681994.2017.1397952] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Submit a Manuscript] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/18/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Katherine M. Hertlein
- Couple and Family Therapy Program, School of Medicine, University of Nevada, Las Vegas, NV, USA
| | - Christy Dulley
- Couple and Family Therapy Program, School of Medicine, University of Nevada, Las Vegas, NV, USA
| | - Raven Cloud
- Couple and Family Therapy Program, School of Medicine, University of Nevada, Las Vegas, NV, USA
| | - Daniela Leon
- Couple and Family Therapy Program, School of Medicine, University of Nevada, Las Vegas, NV, USA
| | - Jenna Chang
- Couple and Family Therapy Program, School of Medicine, University of Nevada, Las Vegas, NV, USA
| |
Collapse
|