1
|
Ferguson E, Mills R, Dawe‐Lane E, Khan Z, Reynolds C, Davison K, Edge D, Smith R, O'Hagan N, Desai R, Croucher M, Eaton N, Brailsford SR. Questions on travel and sexual behaviours negatively impact ethnic minority donor recruitment: Effect of negative word-of-mouth and avoidance. Vox Sang 2024; 119:1245-1256. [PMID: 39505359 PMCID: PMC11634444 DOI: 10.1111/vox.13748] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/09/2024] [Revised: 09/20/2024] [Accepted: 09/21/2024] [Indexed: 11/08/2024]
Abstract
BACKGROUND AND OBJECTIVES Donor selection questions differentially impacting ethnic minorities can discourage donation directly or via negative word-of-mouth. We explore the differential impact of two blood safety questions relating to (i) sexual contacts linked to areas where human immunodeficiency virus (HIV) rates are high and (ii) travelling to areas where malaria is endemic. Epidemiological data are used to assess infection risk and the need for these questions. MATERIALS AND METHODS We report two studies. Study 1 is a behavioural study on negative word-of-mouth and avoiding donation among ethnic minorities (n = 981 people from National Health Service Blood and Transplant (NHSBT) and the general population: 761 were current donors). Study 2 is an epidemiology study (utilizing NHSBT/UK Health Security Agency (UKHSA) surveillance data on HIV-positive donations across the UK blood services between1996 and 2019) to assess whether the sexual risk question contributes to reducing HIV risk and whether travel deferral was more prevalent among ethnic minorities (2015-2019). Studies 1 and 2 provide complementary evidence on the behavioural impact to support policy implications. RESULTS A high proportion of people from ethnic minorities were discouraged from donating and expressed negative word-of-mouth. This was mediated by perceived racial discrimination within the UK National Health Service. The number of donors with HIV who the sexual contact question could have deferred was low, with between 8% and 9.3% of people from ethnic minorities deferred on travel compared with 1.7% of White people. CONCLUSION Blood services need to consider ways to minimize negative word-of-mouth, remove questions that are no longer justified on evidence and provide justification for those that remain.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Eamonn Ferguson
- School of PsychologyUniversity of NottinghamNottinghamUnited Kingdom
- National Institute for Health and Care Research Blood and Transplant Research Unit in Donor Health and BehaviourUniversity of CambridgeCambridgeUnited Kingdom
| | - Richard Mills
- School of PsychologyUniversity of NottinghamNottinghamUnited Kingdom
- National Institute for Health and Care Research Blood and Transplant Research Unit in Donor Health and BehaviourUniversity of CambridgeCambridgeUnited Kingdom
| | - Erin Dawe‐Lane
- School of PsychologyUniversity of NottinghamNottinghamUnited Kingdom
- Clinical, Educational and Health Psychology, Psychology and Language SciencesUniversity College LondonLondonUnited Kingdom
| | - Zaynah Khan
- School of PsychologyUniversity of NottinghamNottinghamUnited Kingdom
- Research Support Officer, Institute of Mental Health, Nottinghamshire Healthcare NHS Foundation TrustUnited Kingdom
| | - Claire Reynolds
- NHS Blood and Transplant/UK Health Security Agency Epidemiology UnitNHSBTLondonUnited Kingdom
| | - Katy Davison
- NHS Blood and Transplant/UK Health Security Agency Epidemiology UnitUKHSALondonUnited Kingdom
| | - Dawn Edge
- Equality, Diversity & Inclusion Research Unit (EDI‐RU), Greater Manchester Mental Health (GMMH) NHS Trust. Biomedical Research CentreManchester Academic Health Science CentreManchesterUnited Kingdom
| | - Robert Smith
- School of PsychologyUniversity of NottinghamNottinghamUnited Kingdom
- National Institute for Health and Care Research Blood and Transplant Research Unit in Donor Health and BehaviourUniversity of CambridgeCambridgeUnited Kingdom
| | - Niall O'Hagan
- School of PsychologyUniversity of NottinghamNottinghamUnited Kingdom
| | - Roshan Desai
- School of PsychologyUniversity of NottinghamNottinghamUnited Kingdom
| | - Mark Croucher
- NHS Blood and Transplant, Donor Experience ServicesLondonUnited Kingdom
| | - Nadine Eaton
- NHS Blood and Transplant, Marketing, TransfusionsBristolUnited Kingdom
| | - Susan R. Brailsford
- NHS Blood and Transplant/UK Health Security Agency Epidemiology UnitNHSBTLondonUnited Kingdom
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Andrews JL, Grunewald K, Schweizer S. A human working memory advantage for social network information. Proc Biol Sci 2024; 291:20241930. [PMID: 39657809 PMCID: PMC11631495 DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2024.1930] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/03/2023] [Revised: 10/30/2024] [Accepted: 10/30/2024] [Indexed: 12/12/2024] Open
Abstract
As a social species, humans live in complexly bounded social groups. In order to navigate these networks, humans rely on a set of social-cognitive processes, including social working memory. Here, we designed a novel network memory task to study working memory for social versus non-social network information across 241 participants (18-65 years) in a tightly controlled, preregistered study. We show that humans demonstrate a working memory advantage for social, relative to non-social, network information. We also observed a self-relevant positivity bias, but an 'other' negativity bias. These findings are interpreted in the context of an evolutionary need to belong to one's social group, to identify risks to one's social safety and to appropriately track one's social status within a complex network of social relationships.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Jack L. Andrews
- School of Psychology, University of New South Wales, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia
- Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
| | - Karina Grunewald
- School of Psychology, University of New South Wales, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia
| | - Susanne Schweizer
- School of Psychology, University of New South Wales, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia
- Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Ágústsdóttir DH, Öberg S, Christophersen C, Oggesen BT, Rosenberg J. The Frequency of Urination Dysfunction in Patients Operated on for Rectal Cancer: A Systematic Review with Meta-Analyses. Curr Oncol 2024; 31:5929-5942. [PMID: 39451746 PMCID: PMC11505854 DOI: 10.3390/curroncol31100442] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/20/2024] [Revised: 09/19/2024] [Accepted: 09/26/2024] [Indexed: 10/26/2024] Open
Abstract
The frequency of long-term urination dysfunction after surgery for rectal cancer remains unclear, yet it is essential to establish this to improve treatment strategies. Randomized controlled trials (RCTs), non-RCTs, and cohort studies were included with patients having undergone sphincter-preserving total (TME) or partial mesorectal excision (PME) for the treatment of primary rectal cancer in this review. The outcome was urination dysfunction reported at least three months postoperatively, both overall urination dysfunction and subdivided into specific symptoms. The online databases PubMed, Embase, and Cochrane CENTRAL were searched, bias was assessed using the Newcastle-Ottawa scale, and results were synthesized using one-group frequency meta-analyses. A total of 55 studies with 15,072 adults were included. The median follow-up was 29 months (range 3-180). The pooled overall urination dysfunction was 21% (95% confidence interval (CI) 12%-30%) 3-11 months postoperatively and 25% (95% CI 19%-32%) ≥12 months postoperatively. Retention and incontinence were common 3-11 months postoperatively, with pooled frequencies of 11% and 14%, respectively. Increased urinary frequency, retention, and incontinence seemed even more common ≥12 months postoperatively, with pooled frequencies of 37%, 20%, and 23%, respectively. In conclusion, one in five patients experienced urination dysfunction more than a year following an operation for rectal cancer.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Dagný Halla Ágústsdóttir
- Center for Perioperative Optimization and Copenhagen Sequelae Center CARE, Department of Surgery, Herlev and Gentofte Hospital, University of Copenhagen, 2730 Herlev, Denmark; (S.Ö.); (B.T.O.); (J.R.)
| | | | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
4
|
Butler LH, Lamont P, Wan DLY, Prike T, Nasim M, Walker B, Fay N, Ecker UKH. The (Mis)Information Game: A social media simulator. Behav Res Methods 2024; 56:2376-2397. [PMID: 37433974 PMCID: PMC10991066 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-023-02153-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 05/25/2023] [Indexed: 07/13/2023]
Abstract
Given the potential negative impact reliance on misinformation can have, substantial effort has gone into understanding the factors that influence misinformation belief and propagation. However, despite the rise of social media often being cited as a fundamental driver of misinformation exposure and false beliefs, how people process misinformation on social media platforms has been under-investigated. This is partially due to a lack of adaptable and ecologically valid social media testing paradigms, resulting in an over-reliance on survey software and questionnaire-based measures. To provide researchers with a flexible tool to investigate the processing and sharing of misinformation on social media, this paper presents The Misinformation Game-an easily adaptable, open-source online testing platform that simulates key characteristics of social media. Researchers can customize posts (e.g., headlines, images), source information (e.g., handles, avatars, credibility), and engagement information (e.g., a post's number of likes and dislikes). The platform allows a range of response options for participants (like, share, dislike, flag) and supports comments. The simulator can also present posts on individual pages or in a scrollable feed, and can provide customized dynamic feedback to participants via changes to their follower count and credibility score, based on how they interact with each post. Notably, no specific programming skills are required to create studies using the simulator. Here, we outline the key features of the simulator and provide a non-technical guide for use by researchers. We also present results from two validation studies. All the source code and instructions are freely available online at https://misinfogame.com .
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lucy H Butler
- School of Psychological Science, University of Western Australia, Crawley, WA, Australia
| | - Padraig Lamont
- School of Engineering, University of Western Australia, Crawley, WA, Australia
| | - Dean Law Yim Wan
- School of Physics, Mathematics and Computing, University of Western Australia, Crawley, WA, Australia
| | - Toby Prike
- School of Psychological Science, University of Western Australia, Crawley, WA, Australia
| | - Mehwish Nasim
- School of Physics, Mathematics and Computing, University of Western Australia, Crawley, WA, Australia
| | - Bradley Walker
- School of Psychological Science, University of Western Australia, Crawley, WA, Australia
| | - Nicolas Fay
- School of Psychological Science, University of Western Australia, Crawley, WA, Australia
| | - Ullrich K H Ecker
- School of Psychological Science, University of Western Australia, Crawley, WA, Australia.
- Public Policy Institute, University of Western Australia, Crawley, WA, Australia.
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Meeuwis SH, Kłosowska J, Bajcar EA, Wasylewski MT, Badzińska J, Rubanets D, Di Nardo M, Mazzoni G, Bąbel P. Placebo Hypoalgesia and Nocebo Hyperalgesia Induced by Observational Learning May Be Difficult to Disentangle in a Laboratory Setting. THE JOURNAL OF PAIN 2024; 25:805-818. [PMID: 37871681 DOI: 10.1016/j.jpain.2023.10.011] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 12/23/2022] [Revised: 10/11/2023] [Accepted: 10/16/2023] [Indexed: 10/25/2023]
Abstract
Observational learning (OBL) (seeing pain/pain treatment in others) can evoke placebo hypoalgesia and nocebo hyperalgesia. Data that compare these effects and illuminates the role of expectations and empathy are scarce. Healthy participants (n = 105) were randomized to: 1) placebo OBL, 2) nocebo OBL, or 3) no-observation control group. OBL consisted of a model simulating pain relief or increase after a sham ointment was applied to one arm. Pain was evoked with thermal stimuli on both arms (ointment, contralateral) at baseline and postobservation. Expectations, pain ratings, and physiological data (eg, skin conductance level) were collected. A 3 × 2 × 2 (Group × Arm × Phase) mixed analyses of variance revealed a 3-way interaction that confirmed that OBL modulates pain: F(2, 93) = 6.08, P = .003, ηp2 = .12. Significant baseline-to-post-observation pain increases were shown in the nocebo OBL group, with a bigger increase for the arm with ointment (both P ≤ .007). In the placebo OBL group, pain was higher for the contralateral relative to the ointment arm (P < .001). Baseline-to-post-observation pain increase was significant for the contralateral arm (P < .001). Expectation mediated these effects. Skin conductance level decreased over time during ointment trials in the nocebo OBL group, suggesting reduced physiological arousal. The findings illustrate that OBL modulates pain through expectations. In the placebo OBL group, the pain did not decrease for the ointment but increased for the contralateral stimuli, which may reflect nocebo learning. Experimental OBL paradigms typically examine relative differences between ointment and contralateral cues. This can complicate disentangling placebo hypoalgesia and nocebo hyperalgesia in laboratory settings. Implications for existing theories are discussed. PERSPECTIVE: Data that systematically compare placebo hypoalgesia and nocebo hyperalgesia induced by OBL are scarce. The current work illustrates that these effects may be more difficult to disentangle than previously assumed, which could have implications for existing theories on OBL and placebo effects and their translation to clinical practice.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Stefanie H Meeuwis
- Pain Research Group, Institute of Psychology, Jagiellonian University, Kraków, Poland
| | - Joanna Kłosowska
- Pain Research Group, Institute of Psychology, Jagiellonian University, Kraków, Poland
| | - Elżbieta A Bajcar
- Pain Research Group, Institute of Psychology, Jagiellonian University, Kraków, Poland
| | - Mateusz T Wasylewski
- Pain Research Group, Institute of Psychology, Jagiellonian University, Kraków, Poland
| | - Julia Badzińska
- Pain Research Group, Institute of Psychology, Jagiellonian University, Kraków, Poland; Doctoral School in the Social Sciences, Jagiellonian University, Kraków, Poland
| | - Daryna Rubanets
- Pain Research Group, Institute of Psychology, Jagiellonian University, Kraków, Poland; Doctoral School in the Social Sciences, Jagiellonian University, Kraków, Poland
| | - Marianna Di Nardo
- Department of Dynamic, Clinical Psychology and Health, Sapienza University of Rome, Roma, Italy
| | - Giuliana Mazzoni
- Department of Dynamic, Clinical Psychology and Health, Sapienza University of Rome, Roma, Italy
| | - Przemysław Bąbel
- Pain Research Group, Institute of Psychology, Jagiellonian University, Kraków, Poland
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Altay S, Majima Y, Mercier H. Happy thoughts: The role of communion in accepting and sharing (mis)beliefs. BRITISH JOURNAL OF SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 2023; 62:1672-1692. [PMID: 37211921 DOI: 10.1111/bjso.12650] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/19/2022] [Accepted: 04/27/2023] [Indexed: 05/23/2023]
Abstract
The negativity bias favours the cultural diffusion of negative beliefs, yet many common (mis)beliefs-naturopathy works, there's a heaven-are positive. Why? People might share 'happy thoughts'-beliefs that might make others happy-to display their kindness. Five experiments conducted among Japanese and English-speaking participants (N = 2412) show that: (i) people higher on communion are more likely to believe and share happier beliefs, by contrast with people higher in competence and dominance; (ii) when they want to appear nice and kind, rather than competent and dominant, people avoid sharing sad beliefs, and instead prefer sharing happy beliefs; (iii) sharing happier beliefs instead of sad beliefs leads to being perceived as nicer and kinder; and (iv) sharing happy beliefs instead of sad beliefs fleads to being perceived as less dominant. Happy beliefs could spread, despite a general negativity bias, because they allow their senders to signal kindness.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sacha Altay
- Département d'études Cognitives, Institut Jean Nicod, ENS, EHESS, PSL University, CNRS, Paris, France
| | - Yoshimasa Majima
- Department of Psychology for Well-being, School of Social Welfare, Hokusei Gakuen University, Sapporo, Japan
| | - Hugo Mercier
- Département d'études Cognitives, Institut Jean Nicod, ENS, EHESS, PSL University, CNRS, Paris, France
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
Diaz-Diaz F, San Miguel M, Meloni S. Echo chambers and information transmission biases in homophilic and heterophilic networks. Sci Rep 2022; 12:9350. [PMID: 35672432 PMCID: PMC9174247 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-022-13343-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/17/2022] [Accepted: 05/23/2022] [Indexed: 12/04/2022] Open
Abstract
We study how information transmission biases arise by the interplay between the structural properties of the network and the dynamics of the information in synthetic scale-free homophilic/heterophilic networks. We provide simple mathematical tools to quantify these biases. Both Simple and Complex Contagion models are insufficient to predict significant biases. In contrast, a Hybrid Contagion model-in which both Simple and Complex Contagion occur-gives rise to three different homophily-dependent biases: emissivity and receptivity biases, and echo chambers. Simulations in an empirical network with high homophily confirm our findings. Our results shed light on the mechanisms that cause inequalities in the visibility of information sources, reduced access to information, and lack of communication among distinct groups.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Fernando Diaz-Diaz
- IFISC (UIB-CSIC), Institute for Cross-Disciplinary Physics and Complex Systems, Campus Universitat de les Illes Balears, 07122, Palma de Mallorca, Spain
| | - Maxi San Miguel
- IFISC (UIB-CSIC), Institute for Cross-Disciplinary Physics and Complex Systems, Campus Universitat de les Illes Balears, 07122, Palma de Mallorca, Spain
| | - Sandro Meloni
- IFISC (UIB-CSIC), Institute for Cross-Disciplinary Physics and Complex Systems, Campus Universitat de les Illes Balears, 07122, Palma de Mallorca, Spain.
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Tokiya M, Hara M, Matsumoto A, Ashenagar MS, Nakano T, Hirota Y. Association of Vaccine Confidence and Hesitancy in Three Phases of COVID-19 Vaccine Approval and Introduction in Japan. Vaccines (Basel) 2022; 10:vaccines10030423. [PMID: 35335055 PMCID: PMC8954745 DOI: 10.3390/vaccines10030423] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/20/2022] [Revised: 03/08/2022] [Accepted: 03/09/2022] [Indexed: 02/01/2023] Open
Abstract
Understanding vaccine hesitancy, considering the target region and phase, is an urgent issue to quell the coronavirus disease (COVID-19) pandemic. This study aimed to monitor COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy in the Japanese population during the three phases of vaccine approval and introduction, and evaluate the association of vaccine hesitancy with vaccine confidence and literacy. We conducted web-based cross-sectional surveys during the three phases of COVID-19 vaccine introduction: January 2021, before approval; June, start of vaccination of the elderly; and September, when about 70% of the target population was vaccinated with at least one dose. There were 7210 participants, aged 20−80 years. We evaluated the association of vaccine hesitancy with vaccine confidence and literacy in the three phases using multivariate logistic regression analysis. The proportion of hesitancy in January, June, and September was 17.5%, 65.3%, and 19.4%, respectively. In any phase, lower vaccine confidence and literacy showed a higher adjusted odds ratio (AOR) of vaccine hesitancy in most items (AOR > 1, p < 0.001). Vaccine hesitancy in June had a different trend in perception of COVID-19 compared to that in the January and September surveys. The findings suggested that hesitancy increases transiently during vaccination introduction phases, and changes as the vaccination program progressed or waves of epidemic. Careful risk communication to increase vaccine confidence and literacy is essential to reduce vaccine hesitancy, especially in the introduction phase.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Mikiko Tokiya
- Department of Social and Environmental Medicine, Faculty of Medicine, Saga University, 5-1-1 Nabeshima, Saga 849-8501, Japan; (A.M.); (M.S.A.)
- Correspondence: ; Tel.: +81-952-34-2289
| | - Megumi Hara
- Department of Preventive Medicine, Faculty of Medicine, Saga University, 5-1-1 Nabeshima, Saga 849-8501, Japan;
| | - Akiko Matsumoto
- Department of Social and Environmental Medicine, Faculty of Medicine, Saga University, 5-1-1 Nabeshima, Saga 849-8501, Japan; (A.M.); (M.S.A.)
| | - Mohammad Said Ashenagar
- Department of Social and Environmental Medicine, Faculty of Medicine, Saga University, 5-1-1 Nabeshima, Saga 849-8501, Japan; (A.M.); (M.S.A.)
| | - Takashi Nakano
- Department of Pediatrics, Kawasaki Medical School, 577 Matsushima, Kurashiki 701-0192, Japan;
| | - Yoshio Hirota
- Clinical Epidemiology Research Center, Medical Co. LTA (SOUSEIKAI), Higashi-ku, Fukuoka 813-0017, Japan;
| |
Collapse
|