1
|
Doering GN, Prebus MM, Suresh S, Greer JN, Bowden R, Linksvayer TA. Emergent collective behavior evolves more rapidly than individual behavior among acorn ant species. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2024; 121:e2420078121. [PMID: 39576350 PMCID: PMC11621464 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.2420078121] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/01/2024] [Accepted: 10/24/2024] [Indexed: 11/24/2024] Open
Abstract
Emergence is a fundamental concept in biology and other disciplines, but whether emergent phenotypes evolve similarly to nonemergent phenotypes is unclear. The hypothesized process of emergent evolution posits that evolutionary change in at least some collective behaviors will differ from evolutionary change in the corresponding intrinsic behaviors of isolated individuals. As a result, collective behavior might evolve more rapidly and diversify more between populations compared to individual behavior. To test whether collective behavior evolves emergently, we conducted a large comparative study using 22 ant species and gathered over 1,500 behavioral rhythm time series from hundreds of colonies and isolated individuals, totaling over 1.5 y of behavioral data. We show that analogous traits measured at individual and collective levels exhibit distinct evolutionary patterns. The estimated rates of phenotypic evolution for the rhythmicity of activity in ant colonies were faster than the evolutionary rates of the same behavior measured in isolated individual ants, and total variation across species in collective behavior was higher than variation in individual behavior. We hypothesize that more rapid evolution and higher variation is a widespread feature of emergent phenotypes relative to lower-level phenotypes across complex biological systems.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Grant Navid Doering
- School of Life Sciences, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ85287
- Department of Biological Sciences, Texas Tech University, Lubbock, TX79409
| | - Matthew M. Prebus
- School of Life Sciences, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ85287
- Department of Integrative Taxonomy of Insects, Institute of Biology, University of Hohenheim, Stuttgart70599, Germany
- Center for Biodiversity and Integrative Taxonomy, University of Hohenheim and State Museum of Natural History, Stuttgart70599, Germany
| | - Sachin Suresh
- School of Life Sciences, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ85287
- Department of Biological Sciences, Texas Tech University, Lubbock, TX79409
| | - Jordan N. Greer
- Department of Biological Sciences, Texas Tech University, Lubbock, TX79409
| | - Reilly Bowden
- Department of Biological Sciences, Texas Tech University, Lubbock, TX79409
| | - Timothy A. Linksvayer
- School of Life Sciences, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ85287
- Department of Biological Sciences, Texas Tech University, Lubbock, TX79409
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Jain PB, Nguyen TT, Mináč J, Muller LE, Budzinski RC. Composed solutions of synchronized patterns in multiplex networks of Kuramoto oscillators. CHAOS (WOODBURY, N.Y.) 2023; 33:103128. [PMID: 37844292 DOI: 10.1063/5.0161399] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/09/2023] [Accepted: 09/18/2023] [Indexed: 10/18/2023]
Abstract
Networks with different levels of interactions, including multilayer and multiplex networks, can display a rich diversity of dynamical behaviors and can be used to model and study a wide range of systems. Despite numerous efforts to investigate these networks, obtaining mathematical descriptions for the dynamics of multilayer and multiplex systems is still an open problem. Here, we combine ideas and concepts from linear algebra and graph theory with nonlinear dynamics to offer a novel approach to study multiplex networks of Kuramoto oscillators. Our approach allows us to study the dynamics of a large, multiplex network by decomposing it into two smaller systems: one representing the connection scheme within layers (intra-layer), and the other representing the connections between layers (inter-layer). Particularly, we use this approach to compose solutions for multiplex networks of Kuramoto oscillators. These solutions are given by a combination of solutions for the smaller systems given by the intra- and inter-layer systems, and in addition, our approach allows us to study the linear stability of these solutions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Priya B Jain
- Department of Mathematics, Western University, London, Ontario N6A 3K7, Canada
- Western Institute for Neuroscience, Western University, London, Ontario N6A 3K7, Canada
- Western Academy for Advanced Research, Western University, London, Ontario N6A 3K7, Canada
| | - Tung T Nguyen
- Department of Mathematics, Western University, London, Ontario N6A 3K7, Canada
- Western Institute for Neuroscience, Western University, London, Ontario N6A 3K7, Canada
- Western Academy for Advanced Research, Western University, London, Ontario N6A 3K7, Canada
| | - Ján Mináč
- Department of Mathematics, Western University, London, Ontario N6A 3K7, Canada
- Western Institute for Neuroscience, Western University, London, Ontario N6A 3K7, Canada
- Western Academy for Advanced Research, Western University, London, Ontario N6A 3K7, Canada
| | - Lyle E Muller
- Department of Mathematics, Western University, London, Ontario N6A 3K7, Canada
- Western Institute for Neuroscience, Western University, London, Ontario N6A 3K7, Canada
- Western Academy for Advanced Research, Western University, London, Ontario N6A 3K7, Canada
| | - Roberto C Budzinski
- Department of Mathematics, Western University, London, Ontario N6A 3K7, Canada
- Western Institute for Neuroscience, Western University, London, Ontario N6A 3K7, Canada
- Western Academy for Advanced Research, Western University, London, Ontario N6A 3K7, Canada
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Colpo KD, Reyes Jiménez LM. Changes in the activity budget of the fiddler crab Leptuca uruguayensis throughout the reproductive period in temperate estuaries. ZOOLOGY 2023; 159:126104. [PMID: 37515906 DOI: 10.1016/j.zool.2023.126104] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/10/2023] [Revised: 07/22/2023] [Accepted: 07/23/2023] [Indexed: 07/31/2023]
Abstract
Animal reproductive success implies the performance of several behaviours, such as courting, mate searching, copulation, offspring production and care. These behaviours usually have high energetic and ecological costs. Therefore, to maximise their reproductive success, animals should make choices throughout their lives, such as deciding how much energy to invest in different activities, according to their conditions and needs. In temperate estuaries, the fiddler crab L. uruguayensis has a short reproductive period, with two synchronous spawning events. Considering that reproductive behaviours incur high energetic cost to fiddler crabs, we estimated how this species manages its activity budget throughout the reproductive period, to quantify trade-offs between the time spent on reproductive behaviours versus time spent on other activities. By analysing videos of females and males recorded in the field at different moments of the reproductive period, we observed that pre-copulatory behaviours, such as female wandering and male waving were more intense at the beginning of the reproductive period, suggesting that most matings occurred before the first spawning event but not before the second one. The ecological conditions during the breeding season and the individual strategies adopted by males and females mostly determine when and how much time to spend on courtship behaviours, and behavioural plasticity can be expected whenever the conditions change. The strategy used by L. uruguayensis for energy management, females' ability to store male gametes and environmental temperatures might have been the main factors determining the relative time spent in courtship behaviours during the reproductive period.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Karine Delevati Colpo
- Instituto de Limnología Dr. Raúl A. Ringuelet (ILPLA), CONICET, Facultad de Ciencias Naturales y Museo, Universidad Nacional de La Plata, Boulevard 120 and 60, La Plata 1900, Argentina.
| | - Laura M Reyes Jiménez
- Instituto de Limnología Dr. Raúl A. Ringuelet (ILPLA), CONICET, Facultad de Ciencias Naturales y Museo, Universidad Nacional de La Plata, Boulevard 120 and 60, La Plata 1900, Argentina
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Perez DM, Klunk CL, Araujo SBL. Imperfect synchrony in animal displays: why does it occur and what is the true role of leadership? Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2021; 376:20200339. [PMID: 34420387 PMCID: PMC8384059 DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2020.0339] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Accepted: 05/26/2021] [Indexed: 11/12/2022] Open
Abstract
Synchrony can be defined as the precise coordination between independent individuals, and this behaviour is more enigmatic when it is imperfect. The traditional theoretical explanation for imperfect synchronous courtship is that it arises as a by-product of the competition between males to broadcast leading signals to attract female attention. This competition is considered an evolutionary stable strategy maintained through sexual selection. However, previous studies have revealed that leading signals are not honest indicators of male quality. We studied imperfect courtship synchrony in fiddler crabs to mainly test whether (i) signal leadership and rate are defined by male quality and (ii) signal leadership generates synchrony. Fiddler crab males wave their enlarged claws during courtship, and females prefer leading males-displaying ahead of their neighbour(s). We filmed groups of waving males in the field to detect how often individuals were leaders and if they engaged in synchrony. Overall, we found that courtship effort is not directly related to male size, a general proxy for quality. Contrary to the long-standing assumption, we also revealed that leadership is not directly related to group synchrony, but faster wave rate correlates with both leadership and synchrony. This article is part of the theme issue 'Synchrony and rhythm interaction: from the brain to behavioural ecology'.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Daniela M. Perez
- Graduate Program in Ecology and Conservation, Universidade Federal do Paraná, Curitiba, Parana 81531-990, Brazil
| | - Cristian L. Klunk
- Graduate Program in Ecology and Conservation, Universidade Federal do Paraná, Curitiba, Parana 81531-990, Brazil
| | - Sabrina B. L. Araujo
- Department of Physics, Laboratory of Biological Interactions, Universidade Federal do Paraná, Curitiba, Parana 81531-990, Brazil
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Greenfield MD, Aihara I, Amichay G, Anichini M, Nityananda V. Rhythm interaction in animal groups: selective attention in communication networks. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2021; 376:20200338. [PMID: 34420386 DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2020.0338] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 02/07/2023] Open
Abstract
Animals communicating interactively with conspecifics often time their broadcasts to avoid overlapping interference, to emit leading, as opposed to following, signals or to synchronize their signalling rhythms. Each of these adjustments becomes more difficult as the number of interactants increases beyond a pair. Among acoustic species, insects and anurans generally deal with the problem of group signalling by means of 'selective attention' in which they focus on several close or conspicuous neighbours and ignore the rest. In these animals, where signalling and receiving are often dictated by sex, the process of selective attention in signallers may have a parallel counterpart in receivers, which also focus on close neighbours. In birds and mammals, local groups tend to be extended families or clans, and group signalling may entail complex timing mechanisms that allow for attention to all individuals. In general, the mechanisms that allow animals to communicate in groups appear to be fully interwoven with the basic process of rhythmic signalling. This article is part of the theme issue 'Synchrony and rhythm interaction: from the brain to behavioural ecology'.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Michael D Greenfield
- Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, University of Kansas, Lawrence, KS 66045, USA.,Equipe Neuro-Ethologie Sensorielle, ENES/Neuro-PSI, CNRS UMR 9197, University of Lyon/Saint-Etienne, 42023 Saint Etienne, France
| | - Ikkyu Aihara
- Faculty of Engineering, Information and Systems, University of Tsukuba, Tsukuba 305-8573, Japan
| | - Guy Amichay
- Department of Collective Behaviour, Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior, 78467 Konstanz, Germany.,Centre for the Advanced Study of Collective Behaviour, University of Konstanz, 78464 Konstanz, Germany.,Department of Biology, University of Konstanz, 78464 Konstanz, Germany
| | - Marianna Anichini
- Hanse-Wissenschaftskolleg Institute for Advanced Study, 'Brain' Research Area, 27753 Delmenhorst, Germany.,Animal Physiology and Behavior Group, Department of Neuroscience, School of Medicine and Health Sciences, Carl von Ossietzky University of Oldenburg, 26129 Oldenburg, Germany
| | - Vivek Nityananda
- Biosciences Institute, Faculty of Medical Sciences, Newcastle University, Henry Wellcome Building, Framlington Place, Newcastle Upon Tyne NE2 4HH, UK
| |
Collapse
|
6
|
Khodaei L. Digest: Ecology and signal structure drive the evolution of synchronous displays in fiddler crabs. Evolution 2019; 74:504-505. [PMID: 31713845 DOI: 10.1111/evo.13872] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/14/2019] [Accepted: 10/18/2019] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
Behaviors are often influenced by both ecology and genetics. Perez et al. tested whether display patterns and the ecology of different species of fiddler crabs influence synchronous waving and whether this a phylogenetic phenomenon. They found that there was no phylogenetic signal in wave display synchronicity, and suggested that signal structure, microhabitat complexity, and different mating systems might instead be the cause. These results support the non-phylogenetic nature of synchronicity that can be observed across animal taxa.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Lucas Khodaei
- Biological and Geological Sciences Building (BGS), Department of Biology, Western University, Ontario, Canada
| |
Collapse
|