1
|
Rhodes M, Gelman SA, Leslie SJ. How generic language shapes the development of social thought. Trends Cogn Sci 2025; 29:122-132. [PMID: 39438162 DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2024.09.012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/22/2024] [Revised: 09/25/2024] [Accepted: 09/26/2024] [Indexed: 10/25/2024]
Abstract
Generic language, that is, language that refers to a category as an abstract whole (e.g., 'Girls like pink') rather than specific individuals (e.g., 'This girl likes pink'), is a common means by which children learn about social kinds. Here, we propose that children interpret generics as signaling that their referenced categories are natural, objective, and have distinctive features, and, thus, in the social domain, that such language affects children's beliefs about the social world in ways that extend far beyond the content they explicitly communicate. On this account, even generics expressing uncontentious content (e.g., 'Girls are great at math') can lead children to think of categories as defining fundamentally distinct kinds of people and contribute to the development of stereotypes and other problematic social phenomena.
Collapse
|
2
|
Flusberg SJ, Holmes KJ, Thibodeau PH, Nabi RL, Matlock T. The Psychology of Framing: How Everyday Language Shapes the Way We Think, Feel, and Act. Psychol Sci Public Interest 2024; 25:105-161. [PMID: 39704149 DOI: 10.1177/15291006241246966] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/21/2024]
Abstract
When we use language to communicate, we must choose what to say, what not to say, and how to say it. That is, we must decide how to frame the message. These linguistic choices matter: Framing a discussion one way or another can influence how people think, feel, and act in many important domains, including politics, health, business, journalism, law, and even conversations with loved ones. The ubiquity of framing effects raises several important questions relevant to the public interest: What makes certain messages so potent and others so ineffectual? Do framing effects pose a threat to our autonomy, or are they a rational response to variation in linguistic content? Can we learn to use language more effectively to promote policy reforms or other causes we believe in, or is this an overly idealistic goal? In this article, we address these questions by providing an integrative review of the psychology of framing. We begin with a brief history of the concept of framing and a survey of common framing effects. We then outline the cognitive, social-pragmatic, and emotional mechanisms underlying such effects. This discussion centers on the view that framing is a natural-and unavoidable-feature of human communication. From this perspective, framing effects reflect a sensible response to messages that communicate different information. In the second half of the article, we provide a taxonomy of linguistic framing techniques, describing various ways that the structure or content of a message can be altered to shape people's mental models of what is being described. Some framing manipulations are subtle, involving a slight shift in grammar or wording. Others are more overt, involving wholesale changes to a message. Finally, we consider factors that moderate the impact of framing, gaps in the current empirical literature, and opportunities for future research. We conclude by offering general recommendations for effective framing and reflecting on the place of framing in society. Linguistic framing is powerful, but its effects are not inevitable-we can always reframe an issue to ourselves or other people.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | | | - Robin L Nabi
- Department of Communication, University of California, Santa Barbara
| | - Teenie Matlock
- Department of Cognitive Science, University of California, Merced
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Levush KC, Butler LP. Children's developing ability to recognize deceptive use of true information. J Exp Child Psychol 2024; 244:105952. [PMID: 38718681 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105952] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/12/2023] [Revised: 03/01/2024] [Accepted: 04/08/2024] [Indexed: 06/10/2024]
Abstract
The strategic use of deliberate omissions, conveying true but selective information for deceptive purposes, is a prevalent and pernicious disinformation tactic. Crucially, its recognition requires engaging in a sophisticated, multi-part social cognitive reasoning process. In two preregistered studies, we investigated the development of children's ability to engage in this process and successfully recognize this form of deception, finding that children even as young as 5 years are capable of doing so, but only with sufficient scaffolding. This work highlights the key role that social cognition plays in the ability to recognize the manipulation techniques that underpin disinformation. It suggests that the interrelated development of pragmatic competence and epistemic vigilance can be harnessed in the design of tools and strategies to help bolster psychological resistance against disinformation in even our youngest citizens-children at the outset of formal education.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Karen C Levush
- Department of Human Development & Quantitative Methodology, University of Maryland, College Park, MD 20742, USA
| | - Lucas Payne Butler
- Department of Human Development & Quantitative Methodology, University of Maryland, College Park, MD 20742, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
4
|
Vasil N, Srinivasan M, Ellwood-Lowe ME, Delaney S, Gopnik A, Lombrozo T. Structural explanations lead young children and adults to rectify resource inequalities. J Exp Child Psychol 2024; 242:105896. [PMID: 38520769 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105896] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/14/2023] [Revised: 02/14/2024] [Accepted: 02/14/2024] [Indexed: 03/25/2024]
Abstract
Decisions about how to divide resources have profound social and practical consequences. Do explanations regarding the source of existing inequalities influence how children and adults allocate new resources? When 3- to 6-year-old children (N = 201) learned that inequalities were caused by structural forces (stable external constraints affecting access to resources) as opposed to internal forces (effort), they rectified inequalities, overriding previously documented tendencies to perpetuate inequality or divide resources equally. Adults (N = 201) were more likely than children to rectify inequality spontaneously; this was further strengthened by a structural explanation but reversed by an effort-based explanation. Allocation behaviors were mirrored in judgments of which allocation choices by others were appropriate. These findings reveal how explanations powerfully guide social reasoning and action from childhood through adulthood.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ny Vasil
- California State University, East Bay, Hayward, CA 94542, USA.
| | | | | | - Sierra Delaney
- University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, CA 94720, USA
| | - Alison Gopnik
- University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, CA 94720, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
5
|
Kinney D, Lombrozo T. Tell me your (cognitive) budget, and I'll tell you what you value. Cognition 2024; 247:105782. [PMID: 38593569 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105782] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/10/2023] [Revised: 03/15/2024] [Accepted: 03/21/2024] [Indexed: 04/11/2024]
Abstract
Consider the following two (hypothetical) generic causal claims: "Living in a neighborhood with many families with children increases purchases of bicycles" and "living in an affluent neighborhood with many families with children increases purchases of bicycles." These claims not only differ in what they suggest about how bicycle ownership is distributed across different neighborhoods (i.e., "the data"), but also have the potential to communicate something about the speakers' values: namely, the prominence they accord to affluence in representing and making decisions about the social world. Here, we examine the relationship between the level of granularity with which a cause is described in a generic causal claim (e.g., neighborhood vs. affluent neighborhood) and the value of the information contained in the causal model that generates that claim. We argue that listeners who know any two of the following can make reliable inferences about the third: 1) the level of granularity at which a speaker makes a generic causal claim, 2) the speaker's values, and 3) the data available to the speaker. We present results of four experiments (N = 1323) in the domain of social categories that provide evidence in keeping with these predictions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- David Kinney
- Yale University, 100 College Street, New Haven, CT 06510, United States of America.
| | | |
Collapse
|
6
|
Chuey A, Luo Y, Markman EM. Epistemic language in news headlines shapes readers' perceptions of objectivity. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2024; 121:e2314091121. [PMID: 38709916 PMCID: PMC11098081 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.2314091121] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/15/2023] [Accepted: 03/23/2024] [Indexed: 05/08/2024] Open
Abstract
How we reason about objectivity-whether an assertion has a ground truth-has implications for belief formation on wide-ranging topics. For example, if someone perceives climate change to be a matter of subjective opinion similar to the best movie genre, they may consider empirical claims about climate change as mere opinion and irrelevant to their beliefs. Here, we investigate whether the language employed by journalists might influence the perceived objectivity of news claims. Specifically, we ask whether factive verb framing (e.g., "Scientists know climate change is happening") increases perceived objectivity compared to nonfactive framing (e.g., "Scientists believe [...]"). Across eight studies (N = 2,785), participants read news headlines about unique, noncontroversial topics (studies 1a-b, 2a-b) or a familiar, controversial topic (climate change; studies 3a-b, 4a-b) and rated the truth and objectivity of the headlines' claims. Across all eight studies, when claims were presented as beliefs (e.g., "Tortoise breeders believe tortoises are becoming more popular pets"), people consistently judged those claims as more subjective than claims presented as knowledge (e.g., "Tortoise breeders know…"), as well as claims presented as unattributed generics (e.g., "Tortoises are becoming more popular pets"). Surprisingly, verb framing had relatively little, inconsistent influence over participants' judgments of the truth of claims. These results demonstrate how, apart from shaping whether we believe a claim is true or false, epistemic language in media can influence whether we believe a claim has an objective answer at all.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Aaron Chuey
- Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA94305
| | - Yiwei Luo
- Department of Linguistics, Stanford University, Stanford, CA94305
| | - Ellen M. Markman
- Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA94305
| |
Collapse
|
7
|
LaTourrette A, Chan DM, Waxman SR. A principled link between object naming and representation is available to infants by seven months of age. Sci Rep 2023; 13:14328. [PMID: 37653111 PMCID: PMC10471589 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-023-41538-y] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 03/22/2023] [Accepted: 08/28/2023] [Indexed: 09/02/2023] Open
Abstract
By their first birthdays, infants represent objects flexibly as a function of not only whether but how the objects are named. Applying the same name to a set of different objects from the same category supports object categorization, with infants encoding commonalities among objects at the expense of individuating details. In contrast, applying a distinct name to each object supports individuation, with infants encoding distinct features at the expense of categorical information. Here, we consider the development of this nuanced link between naming and representation in infants' first year. Infants at 12 months (Study 1; N = 55) and 7 months (Study 2; N = 96) participated in an online recognition memory task. All infants saw the same objects, but their recognition of these objects at test varied as a function of how they had been named. At both ages, infants successfully recognized objects that had been named with distinct labels but failed to recognize these objects when they had all been named with the same, consistent label. This new evidence demonstrates that a principled link between object naming and representation is available by 7 months, early enough to support infants as they begin mapping words to meaning.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Dana Michelle Chan
- Department of Psychology, Northwestern University, 2029 Sheridan Road, Evanston, IL, 60208, USA
| | - Sandra R Waxman
- Department of Psychology, Northwestern University, 2029 Sheridan Road, Evanston, IL, 60208, USA.
| |
Collapse
|
8
|
Benitez J, Leshin RA, Rhodes M. The influence of linguistic form and causal explanations on the development of social essentialism. Cognition 2022; 229:105246. [PMID: 35985103 PMCID: PMC9746922 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2022.105246] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/10/2022] [Revised: 06/24/2022] [Accepted: 07/28/2022] [Indexed: 12/15/2022]
Abstract
Generic descriptions of social categories (e.g., boys play baseball; girls have long hair) lead children and adults to think of the referenced categories (i.e., boys and girls) in essentialist terms-as natural ways of dividing up the world. Yet, key questions remain unanswered about how, why, and when generic language shapes the development of essentialist beliefs. The present experiment examined the scope of these effects by testing the extent to which generics elicit essentialist beliefs because of their linguistic form or because of the causal information they convey. Generic language led children (N = 199, Mage = 6.07 years, range = 4.5-7.95) to essentialize a novel social category, regardless of the causal information used to describe category-property relations (either biological or cultural). In contrast, both linguistic form and causal information influenced adults' (N = 234) beliefs. These findings reveal a unique role of linguistic form in the development and communication of essentialist beliefs in young children.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Josie Benitez
- New York University, Department of Psychology, 6 Washington Place, New York, NY 10003, United States of America.
| | - Rachel A Leshin
- New York University, Department of Psychology, 6 Washington Place, New York, NY 10003, United States of America
| | - Marjorie Rhodes
- New York University, Department of Psychology, 6 Washington Place, New York, NY 10003, United States of America
| |
Collapse
|
9
|
Pronovost MA, Scott RM. The influence of language input on 3-year-olds' learning about novel social categories. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2022; 230:103729. [PMID: 36084438 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2022.103729] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/14/2022] [Revised: 08/10/2022] [Accepted: 08/23/2022] [Indexed: 11/01/2022] Open
Abstract
There is considerable variability in the social categories that children essentialize and the types of expectations children form about these categories, suggesting children's essentialist beliefs are shaped by environmental input. Prior studies have shown that exposure to generic statements about a social category promotes essentialist beliefs in 4.5- to 8-year-old children. However, by this age children form essentialist beliefs quite robustly, and thus it is unclear whether generic statements impact children's expectations about social categories at younger ages when essentialist beliefs first begin to emerge. Moreover, in prior studies the generic statements were delivered by an experimenter and carefully controlled, and thus it is unclear whether these statements would have the same impact if they occurred in a somewhat less constrained setting, such as parents reading a picture book to their child. The current study addressed these open questions by investigating whether generic statements delivered during a picture-book interaction with their parents influenced 3-year-olds' expectations about members of a novel social category. Our results showed that children who heard generic statements during the picture-book interaction used social-group membership to make inferences about the likely behavior of a novel category member, whereas children who were not exposed to generic statements did not. These findings suggest that as early as 3 years of age, children's expectations about social categories are influenced by generic statements that occur during brief parent-child interactions.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Megan A Pronovost
- California State University Fresno, 5300 N Campus Drive, M/S FF12, Fresno, CA 93740, United States.
| | - Rose M Scott
- University of California, Merced, 5200 Lake Rd, Merced, CA 95343, United States
| |
Collapse
|
10
|
Mari MA. How cues to social categorization impact children's inferences about social categories. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2022; 229:103707. [PMID: 35985155 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2022.103707] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 02/14/2022] [Revised: 08/05/2022] [Accepted: 08/09/2022] [Indexed: 11/01/2022] Open
Abstract
Social categorization involves two crucial processes: First, children seek properties on which they can categorize individuals, i.e., they learn to form social categories; then children make inferences based on social category membership and might develop affective responses toward social categories. Over the last decade, a growing number of research in developmental psychology started to use novel social categories to investigate how children learn and reason about social categories. To date, three types of cues have been put forward as means to form social categories, namely linguistic, visual, and behavioral cues. Based on social category membership, children draw inferences about the shared properties of social category members and about how social category members ought to behave and interact with each other. With additional input, children might apply essentialist beliefs to social categories and develop affective responses toward social categories. This article aims to provide key insights on the development of stereotypes and intergroup biases by reviewing recent works that investigated how children learn to form novel social categories and the kind of inferences they make about these novel social categories.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Magali A Mari
- Cognitive Science Center, Rue de la Pierre-à-Mazel 7, University of Neuchâtel, Neuchâtel, Switzerland.
| |
Collapse
|
11
|
Lewis M, Cooper Borkenhagen M, Converse E, Lupyan G, Seidenberg MS. What Might Books Be Teaching Young Children About Gender? Psychol Sci 2021; 33:33-47. [PMID: 34939508 DOI: 10.1177/09567976211024643] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022] Open
Abstract
We investigated how gender is represented in children's books using a novel 200,000-word corpus comprising 247 popular, contemporary books for young children. Using adult human judgments and word co-occurrence data, we quantified gender biases of words in individual books and in the whole corpus. We found that children's books contain many words that adults judge as gendered. Semantic analyses based on co-occurrence data yielded word clusters related to gender stereotypes (e.g., feminine: emotions; masculine: tools). Co-occurrence data also indicated that many books instantiate gender stereotypes identified in other research (e.g., girls are better at reading, and boys are better at math). Finally, we used large-scale data to estimate the gender distribution of the audience for individual books, and we found that children are more often exposed to stereotypes for their own gender. Together, the data suggest that children's books may be an early source of gender associations and stereotypes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Molly Lewis
- Department of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University.,Department of Social and Decision Sciences, Carnegie Mellon University
| | | | - Ellen Converse
- Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin-Madison
| | - Gary Lupyan
- Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin-Madison
| | | |
Collapse
|
12
|
Hoicka E, Saul J, Prouten E, Whitehead L, Sterken R. Language Signaling High Proportions and Generics Lead to Generalizing, but Not Essentializing, for Novel Social Kinds. Cogn Sci 2021; 45:e13051. [PMID: 34758149 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13051] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/05/2021] [Revised: 08/10/2021] [Accepted: 09/20/2021] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
Generics (e.g., "Dogs bark") are thought by many to lead to essentializing: to assuming that members of the same category share an internal property that causally grounds shared behaviors and traits, even without evidence of such a shared property. Similarly, generics are thought to increase generalizing, that is, attributing properties to other members of the same group given evidence that some members of the group have the property. However, it is not clear from past research what underlies the capacity of generic language to increase essentializing and generalizing. Is it specific to generics, or are there broader mechanisms at work, such as the fact that generics are terms that signal high proportions? Study 1 (100 5-6 year-olds, 140 adults) found that neither generics, nor high-proportion quantifiers ("most," "many") elicited essentializing about a novel social kind (Zarpies). However, both generics and high-proportion quantifiers led adults and, to a lesser extent, children, to generalize, with high-proportion quantifiers doing so more than generics for adults. Specifics ("this") did not protect against either essentializing or generalizing when compared to the quantifier "some." Study 2 (100 5-6 year-olds, 112 adults) found that neither generics nor visual imagery signaling high proportions led to essentializing. While generics increased generalizing compared to specifics and visual imagery signaling both low and high proportions for adults, there was no difference in generalizing for children. Our findings suggest high-proportion quantifiers, including generics, lead adults, and to some extent children, to generalize, but not essentialize, about novel social kinds.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | - Jennifer Saul
- Department of Philosophy, University of Sheffield.,Philosophy Department, University of Waterloo
| | | | | | - Rachel Sterken
- Department of Philosophy, University of Oslo.,Philosophy, Hong Kong University
| |
Collapse
|
13
|
|
14
|
Hsiao Y, Banerji N, Nation K. Boys Write About Boys: Androcentrism in Children's Reading Experience and Its Emergence in Children's Own Writing. Child Dev 2021; 92:2194-2204. [PMID: 34228830 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.13623] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/01/2022]
Abstract
Gender bias exists in our language environment. We investigated personal name usage in two large corpora of language written for and by U.K. children aged 5-13. Study 1 found an overrepresentation of male names in children's books, largely attributable to male authors. In stories written by over 100,000 children, Study 2 found an overall male bias that interacted with age. Younger children wrote more about their own gender. With age, girls became more balanced yet boys continued to show a strong male bias. Our findings demonstrate a male-centered bias in both children's books and their own writing. We consider the power of written language to both shape and be shaped by cultural stereotypes via systematic biases in gender associations.
Collapse
|
15
|
Moty K, Rhodes M. The Unintended Consequences of the Things We Say: What Generic Statements Communicate to Children About Unmentioned Categories. Psychol Sci 2021; 32:189-203. [PMID: 33450169 PMCID: PMC8258311 DOI: 10.1177/0956797620953132] [Citation(s) in RCA: 17] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/29/2019] [Accepted: 07/14/2020] [Indexed: 11/17/2022] Open
Abstract
Adults frequently use generic language (e.g., "Boys play sports") to communicate information about social groups to children. Whereas previous research speaks to how children often interpret information about the groups described by generic statements, less is known about what generic claims may implicitly communicate about unmentioned groups (e.g., the possibility that "Boys play sports" implies that girls do not). Study 1 (287 four- to six-year-olds, 56 adults) and Study 2 (84 four- to six-year-olds) found that children as young as 4.5 years draw inferences about unmentioned categories from generic claims (but not matched specific statements)-and that the tendency to make these inferences strengthens with age. Study 3 (181 four- to seven-year-olds, 65 adults) provides evidence that pragmatic reasoning serves as a mechanism underlying these inferences. We conclude by discussing the role that generic language may play in inadvertently communicating social stereotypes to young children.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Kelsey Moty
- Department of Psychology, New York
University
| | | |
Collapse
|
16
|
Leshin RA, Leslie SJ, Rhodes M. Does It Matter How We Speak About Social Kinds? A Large, Preregistered, Online Experimental Study of How Language Shapes the Development of Essentialist Beliefs. Child Dev 2021; 92:e531-e547. [PMID: 33511701 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.13527] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Abstract
A problematic way to think about social categories is to essentialize them-to treat particular differences between people as marking fundamentally distinct social kinds. From where do these beliefs arise? Language that expresses generic claims about categories elicits some aspects of essentialism, but the scope of these effects remains unclear. This study (N = 204, ages 4.5-8 years, 73% White; recruited predominantly from the United States and the United Kingdom to participate online in 2019) found that generic language increases two critical aspects of essentialist thought: Beliefs that (a) category-related properties arise from intrinsic causal mechanisms and (b) category boundaries are inflexible. These findings have implications for understanding the spread of essentialist beliefs across communities and the development of intergroup behavior.
Collapse
|
17
|
Goudeau S, Cimpian A. How Do Young Children Explain Differences in the Classroom? Implications for Achievement, Motivation, and Educational Equity. PERSPECTIVES ON PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE 2021; 16:533-552. [DOI: 10.1177/1745691620953781] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/15/2022]
Abstract
Classroom settings bring to light many differences between children—differences that children notice and attempt to explain. Here, we advance theory on the psychological processes underlying how children explain the differences they observe in the classroom. Integrating evidence from cognitive, social, cultural, developmental, and educational psychology, we propose that young children tend to explain differences among their peers by appealing to the inherent characteristics of those individuals and, conversely, tend to overlook extrinsic reasons for such differences—that is, reasons having to do with external circumstances and structural factors. We then outline how this inherence bias in children’s explanations affects their motivation and performance in school, exacerbating inequalities in achievement and making these inequalities seem legitimate. We conclude by suggesting several means of counteracting the inherence bias in children’s explanations and its effects on their educational outcomes. Throughout, we highlight new directions for research on the relation between children’s explanations, their motivation and achievement, and the inequalities observed in elementary school and beyond.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Sébastien Goudeau
- Centre de Recherches sur la Cognition et l’Apprentissage (CeRCA, UMR CNRS 7295), University of Poitiers
| | | |
Collapse
|
18
|
Essa F, Weinsdörfer A, Shilo R, Diesendruck G, Rakoczy H. Children explain in‐ and out‐group behavior differently. SOCIAL DEVELOPMENT 2021. [DOI: 10.1111/sode.12499] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Francine Essa
- Department of Psychology & Gonda Brain Research Center Bar‐Ilan University Ramat‐Gan Israel
| | - Anika Weinsdörfer
- Department of Developmental Psychology University of Goettingen Goettingen Germany
| | - Reut Shilo
- Department of Psychology & Gonda Brain Research Center Bar‐Ilan University Ramat‐Gan Israel
| | - Gil Diesendruck
- Department of Psychology & Gonda Brain Research Center Bar‐Ilan University Ramat‐Gan Israel
| | - Hannes Rakoczy
- Department of Developmental Psychology University of Goettingen Goettingen Germany
| |
Collapse
|
19
|
Abstract
A foundation of human cognition is the flexibility with which we can represent any object as either a unique individual (my dog Fred) or a member of an object category (dog, animal). This conceptual flexibility is supported by language; the way we name an object is instrumental to our construal of that object as an individual or a category member. Evidence from a new recognition memory task reveals that infants are sensitive to this principled link between naming and object representation by age 12 mo. During training, all infants (n = 77) viewed four distinct objects from the same object category, each introduced in conjunction with either the same novel noun (Consistent Name condition), a distinct novel noun for each object (Distinct Names condition), or the same sine-wave tone sequence (Consistent Tone condition). At test, infants saw each training object again, presented in silence along with a new object from the same category. Infants in the Consistent Name condition showed poor recognition memory at test, suggesting that consistently applied names focused them primarily on commonalities among the named objects at the expense of distinctions among them. Infants in the Distinct Names condition recognized three of the four objects, suggesting that applying distinct names enhanced infants' encoding of the distinctions among the objects. Infants in the control Consistent Tone condition recognized only the object they had most recently seen. Thus, even for infants just beginning to speak their first words, the way in which an object is named guides infants' encoding, representation, and memory for that object.
Collapse
|
20
|
Gender stereotypes are reflected in the distributional structure of 25 languages. Nat Hum Behav 2020; 4:1021-1028. [PMID: 32747806 DOI: 10.1038/s41562-020-0918-6] [Citation(s) in RCA: 34] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/24/2019] [Accepted: 06/26/2020] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
Abstract
Cultural stereotypes such as the idea that men are more suited for paid work and women are more suited for taking care of the home and family, may contribute to gender imbalances in science, technology, engineering and mathematics (STEM) fields, among other undesirable gender disparities. Might these stereotypes be learned from language? Here we examine whether gender stereotypes are reflected in the large-scale distributional structure of natural language semantics. We measure gender associations embedded in the statistics of 25 languages and relate these to data on an international dataset of psychological gender associations (N = 656,636). People's implicit gender associations are strongly predicted by gender associations encoded in the statistics of the language they speak. These associations are further related to the extent that languages mark gender in occupation terms (for example, 'waiter'/'waitress'). Our pattern of findings is consistent with the possibility that linguistic associations shape people's implicit judgements.
Collapse
|
21
|
Vasilyeva N, Lombrozo T. Structural thinking about social categories: Evidence from formal explanations, generics, and generalization. Cognition 2020; 204:104383. [PMID: 32645521 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104383] [Citation(s) in RCA: 19] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/01/2019] [Revised: 06/16/2020] [Accepted: 06/17/2020] [Indexed: 11/19/2022]
Abstract
Many theories of kind representation suggest that people posit internal, essence-like factors that underlie kind membership and explain properties of category members. Across three studies (N = 281), we document the characteristics of an alternative form of construal according to which the properties of social kinds are seen as products of structural factors: stable, external constraints that obtain due to the kind's social position. Internalist and structural construals are similar in that both support formal explanations (i.e., "category member has property P due to category membership C"), generic claims ("Cs have P"), and the generalization of category properties to individual category members when kind membership and social position are confounded. Our findings thus challenge these phenomena as signatures of internalist thinking. However, once category membership and structural position are unconfounded, different patterns of generalization emerge across internalist and structural construals, as do different judgments concerning category definitions and the dispensability of properties for category membership. We discuss the broader implications of these findings for accounts of formal explanation, generic language, and kind representation.
Collapse
|
22
|
Ritchie K, Knobe J. Kindhood and essentialism: Evidence from language. ADVANCES IN CHILD DEVELOPMENT AND BEHAVIOR 2020; 59:133-164. [PMID: 32564792 DOI: 10.1016/bs.acdb.2020.05.005] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
Abstract
A large body of existing research suggests that people think very differently about categories that are seen as kinds (e.g., women) and categories that are not seen as kinds (e.g., people hanging out in the park right now). Drawing on work in linguistics, we suggest that people represent these two sorts of categories using fundamentally different representational formats. Categories that are not seen as kinds are simply represented as collections of individuals. By contrast, when it comes to kinds, people have two distinct representations: a representation of a collection of individual people and a representation of the kind itself. The distinction between these two representational formats helps to shed light on otherwise puzzling findings about stereotyping and essentialism. Stereotyping appears to involve a representation of a collection of people, while essentialism involves a representation of a kind itself.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Katherine Ritchie
- Department of Philosophy, City College of New York, New York, NY, United States; Program in Philosophy, The Graduate Center, CUNY, New York, NY, United States.
| | - Joshua Knobe
- Program in Cognitive Science & Department of Philosophy, Yale University, New Haven, CT, United States
| |
Collapse
|
23
|
Diesendruck G. Why do children essentialize social groups? ADVANCES IN CHILD DEVELOPMENT AND BEHAVIOR 2020; 59:31-64. [PMID: 32564795 DOI: 10.1016/bs.acdb.2020.05.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 10/24/2022]
Abstract
The tendency to essentialize social groups is universal, and arises early in development. This tendency is associated with negative intergroup attitudes and behaviors, and has thus encouraged the search for remedies for the emergence of essentialism. In this vein, great attention has been devoted to uncovering the cognitive foundations of essentialism. In this chapter, I suggest that attention should also be turned toward the motivational foundations of essentialism. I propose that considerations of power and group identity, but especially a "need to belong," may encourage children's essentialization of social groups. Namely, from a young age, children are keen to feel members of a group, and that their membership is secure and exclusive. Essentialism is the conceptual gadget that satisfies these feelings. And to the extent that groups are defined by what they do, this motivated essentialism also impels children to be adamant about the maintenance of unique group behaviors.
Collapse
|
24
|
Abstract
According to the dominant view of category representation, people preferentially infer that kinds (richly structured categories) reflect essences. Generic language (“Boys like blue”) often occupies the central role in accounts of the formation of essentialist interpretations—especially in the context of social categories. In a preregistered study (n = 240 American children, ages 4 to 9 y), we tested whether children assume essences in the presence of generic language or whether they flexibly assume diverse causal structures. Children learned about a novel social category described with generic statements containing either biological properties or cultural properties. Although generic language always led children to believe that properties were nonaccidental, young children (4 or 5 y) in this sample inferred the nonaccidental structure was socialization. Older children (6 to 9 y) flexibly interpreted the category as essential or socialized depending on the type of properties that generalized. We uncovered early-emerging flexibility and no privileged link between kinds and essences.
Collapse
|
25
|
King RA, Scott KE, Renno MP, Shutts K. Counterstereotyping can change children's thinking about boys' and girls' toy preferences. J Exp Child Psychol 2020; 191:104753. [PMID: 31841820 PMCID: PMC11370625 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2019.104753] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 06/21/2019] [Revised: 10/16/2019] [Accepted: 11/01/2019] [Indexed: 12/17/2022]
Abstract
Children think that peers prefer gender-stereotypical toys over gender-counterstereotypical toys. These beliefs can limit children's exploration of gender-counterstereotypical behaviors and prevent the development of broad skills and interests. The current research tested interventions to combat gender-based stereotyping about toys among children aged 4 to 7 years (N = 373). Across four experiments featuring seven different intervention versions, participants saw videos where a teacher provided counterstereotypical messages about toy preferences (e.g., "boys like dolls," "girls like trucks"). The phrasing of the messages (e.g., generic vs. demonstrative) and accompanying photographs (e.g., images of many children vs. one child) varied across experiments. In all intervention conditions, participants made more counterstereotypical (and fewer stereotypical) predictions about peers' toy preferences after viewing intervention videos; differences in the phrasing of the intervention message (e.g., "boys like dolls" vs. "this kid likes dolls") had little effect on participants' predictions. In Experiment 4, an intervention condition containing generic phrasing and gender noun labels (e.g., "boys like dolls") changed children's selection of toys for peers. This research provides promise for counterstereotyping as an impactful and easily implementable intervention strategy.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Rachel Ann King
- University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI 53706, USA; Cornell University, Ithaca, NY 14853, USA.
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
26
|
Abstract
People believe that some categories are kinds with reliable causal structure and high inductive potential (e.g., tigers). Widely endorsed theories propose that people are biased to assume kinds are essential, and so naturally determined by internal causal properties. Generic language (e.g., "men like sports") is 1 mechanism thought to evoke this bias. We propose instead that generics principally designate that categories are kinds. Participants can entertain diverse causal structures in the presence of generics: Hearing that biological properties generalize to a category (e.g., "men grow beards") prompts participants to infer essential structure, but hearing neutral or social properties ("women are underpaid") generalized prompts other causal beliefs. Thus, generics induce essentialism only in interaction with cues that reasonably prompt essentialist explanation. We tested our model with adult participants (n = 739 total), using measures that disentangle essentialist beliefs from kind beliefs. In study 1, we replicate prior methods with our new measures, and find that generics influence kind beliefs more than essentialism. In study 2, we vary property content (biological vs. cultural properties), and show that generics only increase essentialism when paired with biological properties. In study 3, we show that generics designate kinds but not essentialism when neutral properties are used across animals, tools, and people. In study 4, we show that believing a category is a kind increases the spontaneous production of generic statements, regardless of whether the kind is essential or socially constructed. Generics do not necessitate essentialist beliefs. Participants were flexible in their reasoning about kinds.
Collapse
|
27
|
Horne Z, Muradoglu M, Cimpian A. Explanation as a Cognitive Process. Trends Cogn Sci 2019; 23:187-199. [DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2018.12.004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 7] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/05/2018] [Revised: 12/09/2018] [Accepted: 12/11/2018] [Indexed: 10/27/2022]
|
28
|
Sutherland SL, Cimpian A. Developmental evidence for a link between the inherence bias in explanation and psychological essentialism. J Exp Child Psychol 2018; 177:265-281. [PMID: 30286389 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2018.06.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 04/09/2018] [Revised: 06/07/2018] [Accepted: 06/22/2018] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
The assumption that natural and social categories have deeper "essences" is a fundamental feature of the conceptual system, with wide-ranging consequences for behavior. What are the developmental origins of this assumption? We propose that essentialism emerges in part from a bias in the process of generating explanations that leads reasoners to overuse inherent or intrinsic features. Consistent with this proposal, the inherence bias in 4-year-olds' explanations predicted the strength of their essentialist beliefs (Study 1; N = 64), and manipulations of the inherence bias in 4- to 7-year-olds (Studies 2 and 3; N = 112 each) led to subsequent changes in the essentialist beliefs of children who attended to the manipulation. These findings contribute to our understanding of the origins of essentialism.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Shelbie L Sutherland
- Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario M5S 3G3, Canada.
| | - Andrei Cimpian
- Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, NY 10003, USA
| |
Collapse
|
29
|
Vasilyeva N, Blanchard T, Lombrozo T. Stable Causal Relationships Are Better Causal Relationships. Cogn Sci 2018; 42:1265-1296. [DOI: 10.1111/cogs.12605] [Citation(s) in RCA: 11] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/03/2016] [Revised: 02/01/2018] [Accepted: 02/06/2018] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Tania Lombrozo
- Department of Psychology; University of California Berkeley
| |
Collapse
|
30
|
Goldfarb D, Lagattuta KH, Kramer HJ, Kennedy K, Tashjian SM. When Your Kind Cannot Live Here: How Generic Language and Criminal Sanctions Shape Social Categorization. Psychol Sci 2017; 28:1597-1609. [PMID: 28968175 DOI: 10.1177/0956797617714827] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/17/2022] Open
Abstract
Using generic language to describe groups (applying characteristics to entire categories) is ubiquitous and affects how children and adults categorize other people. Five-year-olds, 8-year-olds, and adults ( N = 190) learned about a novel social group that separated into two factions (citizens and noncitizens). Noncitizens were described in either generic or specific language. Later, the children and adults categorized individuals in two contexts: criminal (individuals labeled as noncitizens faced jail and deportation) and noncriminal (labeling had no consequences). Language genericity influenced decision making. Participants in the specific-language condition, but not those in the generic-language condition, reduced the rate at which they identified potential noncitizens when their judgments resulted in criminal penalties compared with when their judgments had no consequences. In addition, learning about noncitizens in specific language (vs. generic language) increased the amount of matching evidence participants needed to identify potential noncitizens (preponderance standard) and decreased participants' certainty in their judgments. Thus, generic language encourages children and adults to categorize individuals using a lower evidentiary standard regardless of negative consequences for presumed social-group membership.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Deborah Goldfarb
- Department of Psychology and Center for Mind and Brain, University of California, Davis
| | | | - Hannah J Kramer
- Department of Psychology and Center for Mind and Brain, University of California, Davis
| | - Katie Kennedy
- Department of Psychology and Center for Mind and Brain, University of California, Davis
| | - Sarah M Tashjian
- Department of Psychology and Center for Mind and Brain, University of California, Davis
| |
Collapse
|
31
|
Yeager DS. Dealing with Social Difficulty During Adolescence: The Role of Implicit Theories of Personality. CHILD DEVELOPMENT PERSPECTIVES 2017; 11:196-201. [PMID: 28983325 PMCID: PMC5624341 DOI: 10.1111/cdep.12234] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
Abstract
Social difficulty during adolescence contributes to internalizing problems (e.g., depression, stress) and spurs cycles of aggression and retaliation. In this article, I review how implicit theories of personality-beliefs about whether people can change their socially relevant characteristics-can help explain why some adolescents respond to social difficulty in these ways while others do not. Believing an entity theory of personality-the belief that people cannot change-causes people to blame their own and others' traits for social difficulty, and predicts more extreme affective, physiological, and behavioral responses (e.g., depression, aggression). Interventions that teach an incremental theory of personality-the belief that people can change-can reduce problematic reactions to social difficulty. I discuss why interventions to alter implicit theories improve adolescents' responses to conflict and propose suggestions for research.
Collapse
|
32
|
Memory accessibility shapes explanation: Testing key claims of the inherence heuristic account. Mem Cognit 2017; 46:68-88. [DOI: 10.3758/s13421-017-0746-8] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/08/2022]
|
33
|
Rhodes M, Mandalaywala TM. The development and developmental consequences of social essentialism. WILEY INTERDISCIPLINARY REVIEWS. COGNITIVE SCIENCE 2017; 8:10.1002/wcs.1437. [PMID: 28273398 PMCID: PMC5591057 DOI: 10.1002/wcs.1437] [Citation(s) in RCA: 53] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/31/2016] [Accepted: 01/08/2017] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
Abstract
People often view certain ways of classifying people (e.g., by gender, race, or ethnicity) as reflecting real distinctions found in nature. Such categories are viewed as marking meaningful, fundamental, and informative differences between distinct kinds of people. This article examines the development of these essentialist intuitive theories of how the social world is structured, along with the developmental consequences of these beliefs. We first examine the processes that give rise to social essentialism, arguing that essentialism emerges as children actively attempt to make sense of their environment by relying on several basic representational and explanatory biases. These developmental processes give rise to the widespread emergence of social essentialist views in early childhood, but allow for vast variability across development and cultural contexts in the precise nature of these beliefs. We then examine what is known and still to be discovered about the implications of essentialism for stereotyping, inter-group interaction, and the development of social prejudice. We conclude with directions for future research, particularly on the theoretical payoff that could be gained by including more diverse samples of children in future developmental investigations. WIREs Cogn Sci 2017, 8:e1437. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1437 This article is categorized under: Psychology > Development and Aging Philosophy > Knowledge and Belief.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marjorie Rhodes
- Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, NY, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
34
|
Rule NO, Sutherland SL. Social Categorization From Faces: Evidence From Obvious and Ambiguous Groups. CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE 2017. [DOI: 10.1177/0963721417697970] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/28/2022]
Abstract
People use facial features (e.g., face shape, skin color, eye structure) both in isolation and in combination to identify others as members of a variety of social categories. For some categories (e.g., age, race, and sex), the markers are obvious and people categorize their members almost perfectly. For others, however (e.g., political affiliation, religious following, and sexual orientation), the markers are ambiguous, yet people can still categorize members of these groups with better than chance accuracy and little effort or awareness. Here, we describe how people categorize others into both perceptually obvious and perceptually ambiguous social groups from their faces, discussing potential mechanisms that may underlie categorization accuracy and noting some of the social consequences that result from categorizing other people into groups.
Collapse
|
35
|
Kim NS, Johnson SGB, Ahn WK, Knobe J. The effect of abstract versus concrete framing on judgments of biological and psychological bases of behavior. COGNITIVE RESEARCH-PRINCIPLES AND IMPLICATIONS 2017; 2:17. [PMID: 28367497 PMCID: PMC5357666 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-017-0056-5] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/14/2016] [Accepted: 02/03/2017] [Indexed: 12/22/2022]
Abstract
Human behavior is frequently described both in abstract, general terms and in concrete, specific terms. We asked whether these two ways of framing equivalent behaviors shift the inferences people make about the biological and psychological bases of those behaviors. In five experiments, we manipulated whether behaviors are presented concretely (i.e. with reference to a specific person, instantiated in the particular context of that person's life) or abstractly (i.e. with reference to a category of people or behaviors across generalized contexts). People judged concretely framed behaviors to be less biologically based and, on some dimensions, more psychologically based than the same behaviors framed in the abstract. These findings held true for both mental disorders (Experiments 1 and 2) and everyday behaviors (Experiments 4 and 5), and yielded downstream consequences for the perceived efficacy of disorder treatments (Experiment 3). Implications for science educators, students of science, and members of the lay public are discussed.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Nancy S Kim
- Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, 125 Nightingale Hall, 360 Huntington Avenue, Boston, MA 02115 USA
| | - Samuel G B Johnson
- Department of Psychology, Yale University, Box 208205, New Haven, CT 06520-8205 USA
| | - Woo-Kyoung Ahn
- Department of Psychology, Yale University, Box 208205, New Haven, CT 06520-8205 USA
| | - Joshua Knobe
- Department of Philosophy, Yale University, 344 College Street, New Haven, CT 06511 USA
| |
Collapse
|
36
|
Abstract
In his 2012 book, Jussim suggests that people's beliefs about various groups (i.e., their stereotypes) are largely accurate. We unpack this claim using the distinction between generic and statistical beliefs - a distinction supported by extensive evidence in cognitive psychology, linguistics, and philosophy. Regardless of whether one understands stereotypes as generic or statistical beliefs about groups, skepticism remains about the rationality of social judgments.
Collapse
|
37
|
Rhodes M, Leslie SJ, Saunders K, Dunham Y, Cimpian A. How does social essentialism affect the development of inter-group relations? Dev Sci 2017; 21. [PMID: 28229563 DOI: 10.1111/desc.12509] [Citation(s) in RCA: 48] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/09/2015] [Accepted: 08/24/2016] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
Psychological essentialism is a pervasive conceptual bias to view categories as reflecting something deep, stable, and informative about their members. Scholars from diverse disciplines have long theorized that psychological essentialism has negative ramifications for inter-group relations, yet little previous empirical work has experimentally tested the social implications of essentialist beliefs. Three studies (N = 127, ages 4.5-6) found that experimentally inducing essentialist beliefs about a novel social category led children to share fewer resources with category members, but did not lead to the out-group dislike that defines social prejudice. These findings indicate that essentialism negatively influences some key components of inter-group relations, but does not lead directly to the development of prejudice.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Marjorie Rhodes
- Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, USA
| | | | - Katya Saunders
- Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, USA
| | - Yarrow Dunham
- Department of Psychology, Yale University, New Haven, CT, USA
| | - Andrei Cimpian
- Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, USA
| |
Collapse
|
38
|
Tasimi A, Gelman SA, Cimpian A, Knobe J. Differences in the Evaluation of Generic Statements About Human and Non-Human Categories. Cogn Sci 2016; 41:1934-1957. [PMID: 27886394 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.12440] [Citation(s) in RCA: 4] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/26/2015] [Revised: 06/22/2016] [Accepted: 07/14/2016] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
Generic statements (e.g., "Birds lay eggs") express generalizations about categories. Current theories suggest that people should be especially inclined to accept generics that involve threatening information. However, previous tests of this claim have focused on generics about non-human categories, which raises the question of whether this effect applies as readily to human categories. In Experiment 1, adults were more likely to accept generics involving a threatening (vs. a non-threatening) property for artifacts, but this negativity bias did not also apply to human categories. Experiment 2 examined an alternative hypothesis for this result, and Experiments 3 and 4 served as conceptual replications of the first experiment. Experiment 5 found that even preschoolers apply generics differently for humans and artifacts. Finally, Experiment 6 showed that these effects reflect differences between human and non-human categories more generally, as adults showed a negativity bias for categories of non-human animals, but not for categories of humans. These findings suggest the presence of important, early-emerging domain differences in people's judgments about generics.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
| | | | - Andrei Cimpian
- Psychology Department, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign.,Department of Psychology, New York University
| | | |
Collapse
|
39
|
Chestnut EK, Markman EM. Are Horses Like Zebras, or Vice Versa? Children's Sensitivity to the Asymmetries of Directional Comparisons. Child Dev 2016; 87:568-82. [DOI: 10.1111/cdev.12476] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
|
40
|
Meyer M, Gelman SA. Generic Reference is Less Marked Than Specific Reference in Children’s Gestures. JOURNAL OF NONVERBAL BEHAVIOR 2015. [DOI: 10.1007/s10919-015-0220-x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 2] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/29/2022]
|
41
|
Sutherland SL, Cimpian A. An explanatory heuristic gives rise to the belief that words are well suited for their referents. Cognition 2015; 143:228-40. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.07.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 10/28/2014] [Revised: 07/01/2015] [Accepted: 07/02/2015] [Indexed: 10/23/2022]
|
42
|
Abstract
The language used to describe concepts influences individuals’ cognition, affect, and behavior. A striking example comes from research on gendered language, or words that denote individuals’ gender (e.g., she, woman, daughter). Gendered language contributes to gender biases by making gender salient, treating gender as a binary category, and causing stereotypic views of gender. In our review, we first summarize some of the major ways that language marks individuals’ gender, focusing on the English language but noting patterns in other languages as well. Second, we describe research on the relation between gendered language, on one hand, and gender-related cognition, affect, and behavior (e.g., gender salience, categorization, stereotyping, and prejudice), on the other hand. Third, we review past and contemporary efforts at changing gendered language, including calls for the use of gender-neutral nouns (e.g., “Good evening, folks” instead of “Good evening, ladies and gentlemen”) and pronouns (e.g., ze instead of he or she). Finally, we highlight the role of values in shaping views of language policies that may mitigate the pervasiveness and consequences of gendered language.
Collapse
|
43
|
Diesendruck G, Menahem R. Essentialism promotes children's inter-ethnic bias. Front Psychol 2015; 6:1180. [PMID: 26321992 PMCID: PMC4532908 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01180] [Citation(s) in RCA: 39] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 05/07/2015] [Accepted: 07/27/2015] [Indexed: 11/13/2022] Open
Abstract
The present study investigated the developmental foundation of the relation between social essentialism and attitudes. Forty-eight Jewish Israeli secular 6-year-olds were exposed to either a story emphasizing essentialism about ethnicity, or stories controlling for the salience of ethnicity or essentialism per se. After listening to a story, children's attitudes were assessed in a drawing and in an IAT task. Compared to the control conditions, children in the ethnic essentialism condition drew a Jewish and an Arab character as farther apart from each other, and the Jewish character with a more positive affect than the Arab character. Moreover, boys in the ethnic essentialism condition manifested a stronger bias in the IAT. These findings reveal an early link between essentialism and inter-group attitudes.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Gil Diesendruck
- Department of Psychology, Bar-Ilan University Ramat-Gan, Israel
| | - Roni Menahem
- Department of Psychology, Bar-Ilan University Ramat-Gan, Israel
| |
Collapse
|
44
|
Fu G, Heyman GD, Qian M, Guo T, Lee K. Young children with a positive reputation to maintain are less likely to cheat. Dev Sci 2015; 19:275-83. [DOI: 10.1111/desc.12304] [Citation(s) in RCA: 51] [Impact Index Per Article: 5.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 07/02/2014] [Accepted: 02/10/2015] [Indexed: 11/28/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Genyue Fu
- Department of Psychology; Hangzhou Normal University; PR China
| | - Gail D. Heyman
- Department of Psychology; Hangzhou Normal University; PR China
- Department of Psychology; University of California San Diego; USA
| | - Miao Qian
- Department of Psychology; Hangzhou Normal University; PR China
| | - Tengfei Guo
- School of Education; Guizhou Normal University; PR China
| | - Kang Lee
- Department of Psychology; Hangzhou Normal University; PR China
- Dr Eric Jackman Institute of Child Study; University of Toronto; Canada
| |
Collapse
|
45
|
Gülgöz S, Gelman SA. Children's Recall of Generic and Specific Labels Regarding Animals and People. COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT 2015; 33:84-98. [PMID: 25598575 PMCID: PMC4292889 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2014.05.002] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.9] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022]
Abstract
Although children tend to categorize objects at the basic level, we hypothesized that generic sentences would direct children's attention to different levels of categorization. We tested children's and adults' short-term recall (Study 1) and longer-term recall (Study 2) for labels presented in generic sentences (e.g., Kids like to play jimjam) versus specific sentences (e.g., This kid likes to play jimjam). Label content was either basic level (e.g., cat, boy) or superordinate (e.g., animal, kid). As predicted, participants showed better memory for label content in generic than specific sentences (short-term recall for children; both short and longer-term recall for adults). Errors typically involved recalling specific noun phrases as generic, and recalling superordinate labels as basic. These results demonstrate that language influences children's representations of new factual information, but that cognitive biases also lead to distortions in recall.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Selin Gülgöz
- University of Michigan, 530 Church St., Department of Psychology, Ann Arbor, MI 48109-1043
| | - Susan A. Gelman
- University of Michigan, 530 Church St., Department of Psychology, Ann Arbor, MI 48109-1043
| |
Collapse
|
46
|
The inherence heuristic across development: Systematic differences between children’s and adults’ explanations for everyday facts. Cogn Psychol 2014; 75:130-54. [DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2014.09.001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 28] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 11/25/2013] [Revised: 09/11/2014] [Accepted: 09/15/2014] [Indexed: 11/23/2022]
|
47
|
Refining and expanding the proposal of an inherence heuristic in human understanding. Behav Brain Sci 2014; 37:506-27. [DOI: 10.1017/s0140525x14000028] [Citation(s) in RCA: 16] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/06/2022]
Abstract
AbstractThe inherence heuristic is a cognitive process that supplies quick and effortless explanations for a wide variety of observations. Due in part to biases in memory retrieval, this heuristic tends to overproduce explanations that appeal to the inherent features of the entities in the observations being explained (hence the heuristic's name). In this response, we use the commentators' input to clarify, refine, and expand the inherence heuristic model. The end result is a piece that complements the target article, amplifying its theoretical contribution.
Collapse
|
48
|
Brandone AC, Gelman SA, Hedglen J. Children's Developing Intuitions About the Truth Conditions and Implications of Novel Generics Versus Quantified Statements. Cogn Sci 2014; 39:711-38. [PMID: 25297340 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.12176] [Citation(s) in RCA: 18] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 08/26/2011] [Revised: 02/05/2014] [Accepted: 03/04/2014] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Abstract
Generic statements express generalizations about categories and present a unique semantic profile that is distinct from quantified statements. This paper reports two studies examining the development of children's intuitions about the semantics of generics and how they differ from statements quantified by all, most, and some. Results reveal that, like adults, preschoolers (a) recognize that generics have flexible truth conditions and are capable of representing a wide range of prevalence levels; and (b) interpret novel generics as having near-universal prevalence implications. Results further show that by age 4, children are beginning to differentiate the meaning of generics and quantified statements; however, even 7- to 11-year-olds are not adultlike in their intuitions about the meaning of most-quantified statements. Overall, these studies suggest that by preschool, children interpret generics in much the same way that adults do; however, mastery of the semantics of quantified statements follows a more protracted course.
Collapse
|
49
|
Segall G, Birnbaum D, Deeb I, Diesendruck G. The intergenerational transmission of ethnic essentialism:howparents talk counts the most. Dev Sci 2014; 18:543-55. [DOI: 10.1111/desc.12235] [Citation(s) in RCA: 47] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 09/17/2013] [Accepted: 07/08/2014] [Indexed: 11/30/2022]
Affiliation(s)
- Gili Segall
- Department of Psychology and Gonda Brain Research Center; Bar-Ilan University; Israel
| | - Dana Birnbaum
- Department of Psychology and Gonda Brain Research Center; Bar-Ilan University; Israel
| | - Inas Deeb
- Department of Psychology and Gonda Brain Research Center; Bar-Ilan University; Israel
| | - Gil Diesendruck
- Department of Psychology and Gonda Brain Research Center; Bar-Ilan University; Israel
| |
Collapse
|
50
|
The inherence heuristic: An intuitive means of making sense of the world, and a potential precursor to psychological essentialism. Behav Brain Sci 2014; 37:461-80. [DOI: 10.1017/s0140525x13002197] [Citation(s) in RCA: 117] [Impact Index Per Article: 10.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/07/2022]
Abstract
AbstractWe propose that human reasoning relies on aninherence heuristic, an implicit cognitive process that leads people to explain observed patterns (e.g., girls wear pink) predominantly in terms of the inherent features of their constituents (e.g., pink is a delicate color). We then demonstrate how this proposed heuristic can provide a unified account for a broad set of findings spanning areas of research that might at first appear unrelated (e.g., system justification, nominal realism, is–ought errors in moral reasoning). By revealing the deep commonalities among the diverse phenomena that fall under its scope, our account is able to generate new insights into these phenomena, as well as new empirical predictions. A second main goal of this article, aside from introducing the inherence heuristic, is to articulate the proposal that the heuristic serves as a foundation for the development of psychological essentialism. More specifically, we propose thatessentialism – which is the common belief that natural and social categories are underlain by hidden, causally powerful essences – emerges over the first few years of life as an elaboration of the earlier, and more open-ended, intuitions supplied by the inherence heuristic. In the final part of the report, we distinguish our proposal from competing accounts (e.g., Strevens's K-laws) and clarify the relationship between the inherence heuristic and related cognitive tendencies (e.g., the correspondence bias). In sum, this article illuminates a basic cognitive process that emerges early in life and is likely to have profound effects on many aspects of human psychology.
Collapse
|