1
|
Yan RJ, Gong HQ, Zhang PM, Liang PJ. Coding Properties of Mouse Retinal Ganglion Cells with Dual-Peak Patterns with Respect to Stimulus Intervals. Front Comput Neurosci 2016; 10:75. [PMID: 27486396 PMCID: PMC4949255 DOI: 10.3389/fncom.2016.00075] [Citation(s) in RCA: 1] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [Track Full Text] [Download PDF] [Figures] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/18/2016] [Accepted: 07/05/2016] [Indexed: 11/16/2022] Open
Abstract
How visual information is encoded in spikes of retinal ganglion cells (RGCs) is essential in visual neuroscience. In the present study, we investigated the coding properties of mouse RGCs with dual-peak patterns with respect to visual stimulus intervals. We first analyzed the response properties, and observed that the latencies and spike counts of the two response peaks in the dual-peak pattern exhibited systematic changes with the preceding light-OFF interval. We then applied linear discriminant analysis (LDA) to assess the relative contributions of response characteristics of both peaks in information coding regarding the preceding stimulus interval. It was found that for each peak, the discrimination results were far better than chance level based on either latency or spike count, and were further improved by using the combination of the two parameters. Furthermore, the best discrimination results were obtained when latencies and spike counts of both peaks were considered in combination. In addition, the correct rate for stimulation discrimination was higher when RGC population activity was considered as compare to single neuron's activity, and the correct rate was increased with the group size. These results suggest that rate coding, temporal coding, and population coding are all involved in encoding the different stimulus-interval patterns, and the two response peaks in the dual-peak pattern carry complementary information about stimulus interval.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Ru-Jia Yan
- School of Biomedical Engineering, Shanghai Jiao Tong University Shanghai, China
| | - Hai-Qing Gong
- School of Biomedical Engineering, Shanghai Jiao Tong University Shanghai, China
| | - Pu-Ming Zhang
- School of Biomedical Engineering, Shanghai Jiao Tong University Shanghai, China
| | - Pei-Ji Liang
- School of Biomedical Engineering, Shanghai Jiao Tong University Shanghai, China
| |
Collapse
|
2
|
Abstract
Illusions of lightness offer valuable clues to how lightness values are computed by the visual system. The traditional domain of lightness illusions must be expanded to include failures of constancy, as there is no distinction between these categories. Just as lightness is (relatively) constant in the face of changes in illumination level, so it is equally constant in the face of changes in background reflectance. Simultaneous lightness contrast, the most familiar lightness illusion, is fairly weak, and represents a failure of background-independent lightness constancy. It is argued that a combination of the highest-luminance rule of anchoring plus the Kardos idea of codetermination can account for most lightness illusions. Kardos suggested that the lightness value of a target surface is partly determined relative to the field of illumination (or framework) in which it is embedded, and partly relative to the neighboring field of illumination. Although Kardos did not apply his principle of codetermination to failures of background-independent constancy such as the simultaneous contrast illusion, this can be done rather easily by defining a framework as a perceptual group instead of identifying it strictly with an objective field of illumination.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Alan Gilchrist
- Department of Psychology, Rutgers University, 101 Warren Street, Newark, NJ 07102, USA
| |
Collapse
|
3
|
Ongoing temporal coding of a stochastic stimulus as a function of intensity: time-intensity trading. J Neurosci 2012; 32:9517-27. [PMID: 22787037 DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.0103-12.2012] [Citation(s) in RCA: 10] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/21/2022] Open
Abstract
Stimulus-locked temporal codes are increasingly seen as relevant to perception. The timing of action potentials typically varies with stimulus intensity, and the invariance of temporal representations with intensity is therefore an issue. We examine the timing of action potentials in cat auditory nerve to broadband noise presented at different intensities, using an analysis inspired by coincidence detection and by the binaural "latency hypothesis." It is known that the two cues for azimuthal sound localization, interaural intensity or level differences and interaural time differences (ITDs), interact perceptually. According to the latency hypothesis, the increase in intensity for the ear nearest to a sound source off the midline causes a decrease in response latency in that ear relative to the other ear. We found that changes in intensity cause small but systematic shifts in the ongoing timing of responses in the auditory nerve, generally but not always resulting in shorter delays between stimulus onset and neural response for increasing intensity. The size of the temporal shifts depends on characteristic frequency with a pattern indicating a fine-structure and an envelope response regime. Overall, the results show that ongoing timing is remarkably stable with intensity at the most peripheral neural level. The results are not consistent in a simple way with the latency hypothesis, but because of the acute sensitivity to ITDs, the subtle effects of intensity on timing may nevertheless have perceptual consequences.
Collapse
|
4
|
Thoreson WB, Mangel SC. Lateral interactions in the outer retina. Prog Retin Eye Res 2012; 31:407-41. [PMID: 22580106 PMCID: PMC3401171 DOI: 10.1016/j.preteyeres.2012.04.003] [Citation(s) in RCA: 165] [Impact Index Per Article: 12.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Key Words] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Received: 01/31/2012] [Revised: 03/05/2012] [Accepted: 03/09/2012] [Indexed: 10/28/2022]
Abstract
Lateral interactions in the outer retina, particularly negative feedback from horizontal cells to cones and direct feed-forward input from horizontal cells to bipolar cells, play a number of important roles in early visual processing, such as generating center-surround receptive fields that enhance spatial discrimination. These circuits may also contribute to post-receptoral light adaptation and the generation of color opponency. In this review, we examine the contributions of horizontal cell feedback and feed-forward pathways to early visual processing. We begin by reviewing the properties of bipolar cell receptive fields, especially with respect to modulation of the bipolar receptive field surround by the ambient light level and to the contribution of horizontal cells to the surround. We then review evidence for and against three proposed mechanisms for negative feedback from horizontal cells to cones: 1) GABA release by horizontal cells, 2) ephaptic modulation of the cone pedicle membrane potential generated by currents flowing through hemigap junctions in horizontal cell dendrites, and 3) modulation of cone calcium currents (I(Ca)) by changes in synaptic cleft proton levels. We also consider evidence for the presence of direct horizontal cell feed-forward input to bipolar cells and discuss a possible role for GABA at this synapse. We summarize proposed functions of horizontal cell feedback and feed-forward pathways. Finally, we examine the mechanisms and functions of two other forms of lateral interaction in the outer retina: negative feedback from horizontal cells to rods and positive feedback from horizontal cells to cones.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Wallace B. Thoreson
- Departments of Ophthalmology & Visual Sciences and Pharmacology & Experimental Neuroscience, University of Nebraska Medical Center, Omaha, NE 68198 USA
| | - Stuart C. Mangel
- Department of Neuroscience, The Ohio State University College of Medicine, Columbus, OH 43210 USA
| |
Collapse
|
5
|
Freeman DK, Graña G, Passaglia CL. Retinal ganglion cell adaptation to small luminance fluctuations. J Neurophysiol 2010; 104:704-12. [PMID: 20538771 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00767.2009] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
To accommodate the wide input range over which the visual system operates within the narrow output range of spiking neurons, the retina adjusts its sensitivity to the mean light level so that retinal ganglion cells can faithfully signal contrast, or relative deviations from the mean luminance. Given the large operating range of the visual system, the majority of work on luminance adaptation has involved logarithmic changes in light level. We report that luminance gain controls are recruited for remarkably small fluctuations in luminance as well. Using spike recordings from the rat optic tract, we show that ganglion cell responses to a brief flash of light are modulated in amplitude by local background fluctuations as little as 15% contrast. The time scale of the gain control is rapid (<125 ms), at least for on cells. The retinal locus of adaptation precedes the ganglion cell spike generator because response gain changes of on cells were uncorrelated with firing rate. The mechanism seems to reside within the inner retinal network and not in the photoreceptors, because the adaptation profiles of on and off cells differed markedly. The response gain changes follow Weber's law, suggesting that network mechanisms of luminance adaptation described in previous work modulates retinal ganglion cell sensitivity, not just when we move between different lighting environments, but also as our eyes scan a visual scene. Finally, we show that response amplitude is uniformly reduced for flashes on a modulated background that has spatial contrast, indicating that another gain control that integrates luminance signals nonlinearly over space operates within the receptive field center of rat ganglion cells.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Daniel K Freeman
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts 02215, USA
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
6
|
Huang X, Paradiso MA. Background changes delay information represented in macaque V1 neurons. J Neurophysiol 2005; 94:4314-30. [PMID: 16107522 DOI: 10.1152/jn.01309.2004] [Citation(s) in RCA: 14] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
In natural behavioral situations, saccadic eye movements not only introduce new stimuli into V1 receptive fields, they also cause changes in the background. We recorded in awake macaque V1 using a fixation paradigm and compared evoked activity to small stimuli when the background was either static or changing as with a saccade. When a stimulus was shown on a static background, as in most previous experiments, the initial response was orientation selective and contrast was inversely correlated with response latency. When a stimulus was introduced with a background change, V1 neurons showed a qualitatively different temporal response pattern in which information about stimulus orientation and contrast was delayed. The delay in the representation of visual information was found with three different types of background change-luminance increment, luminance decrement, and a pattern change with fixed mean luminance. We also found that with a background change, V1 off responses were suppressed and had a shorter time course compared with the static-background situation. Our results suggest that the distribution of temporal changes across the visual field plays a fundamental role in determining V1 responses. In the static-background condition, temporal change in the visual input occurs only in a small portion of the visual field. In the changing-background condition, and presumably in natural vision, temporal changes are widely distributed. Thus a delayed representation of visual information may be more representative of natural visual situations.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- Xin Huang
- Department of Neuroscience, Brown University, Providence, RI 02912, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
7
|
Levick WR. Blood vessels casting a shadow. Science 2003; 299:1983-5; author reply 1983-5. [PMID: 12663898 DOI: 10.1126/science.299.5615.1983] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/02/2022]
|
8
|
Troy JB, Shou T. The receptive fields of cat retinal ganglion cells in physiological and pathological states: where we are after half a century of research. Prog Retin Eye Res 2002; 21:263-302. [PMID: 12052385 DOI: 10.1016/s1350-9462(02)00002-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 55] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/25/2022]
Abstract
Studies on the receptive field properties of cat retinal ganglion cells over the past half-century are reviewed within the context of the role played by the receptive field in visual information processing. Emphasis is placed on the work conducted within the past 20 years, but a summary of key contributions from the 1950s to 1970s is provided. We have sought to review aspects of the ganglion cell receptive field that have not been featured prominently in previous review articles. Our review of the receptive field properties of X- and Y-cells focuses on quantitative studies and includes consideration of the function of the receptive field in visual signal processing. We discuss the non-classical as well as the classical receptive field. Attention is also given to the receptive field properties of the less well-studied cat ganglion cells-the W-cells-and the effect of pathology on cat ganglion cell properties. Although work from our laboratories is highlighted, we hope that we have given a reasonably balanced view of the current state of the field.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- J B Troy
- Department of Biomedical Engineering & Neuroscience Institute, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, USA
| | | |
Collapse
|
9
|
Reich DS, Mechler F, Victor JD. Temporal coding of contrast in primary visual cortex: when, what, and why. J Neurophysiol 2001; 85:1039-50. [PMID: 11247974 DOI: 10.1152/jn.2001.85.3.1039] [Citation(s) in RCA: 144] [Impact Index Per Article: 6.0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/22/2022] Open
Abstract
How do neurons in the primary visual cortex (V1) encode the contrast of a visual stimulus? In this paper, the information that V1 responses convey about the contrast of static visual stimuli is explicitly calculated. These responses often contain several easily distinguished temporal components, which will be called latency, transient, tonic, and off. Calculating the information about contrast conveyed in each component and in groups of components makes it possible to delineate aspects of the temporal structure that may be relevant for contrast encoding. The results indicate that as much or more contrast-related information is encoded into the temporal structure of spike train responses as into the firing rate and that the temporally coded information is manifested most strongly in the latency to response onset. Transient, tonic, and off responses contribute relatively little. The results also reveal that temporal coding is important for distinguishing subtle contrast differences, whereas firing rates are useful for gross discrimination. This suggests that the temporal structure of neurons' responses may extend the dynamic range for contrast encoding in the primate visual system.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- D S Reich
- Laboratory of Biophysics, The Rockefeller University, New York, New York 10021, USA.
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
10
|
Dahari R, Spitzer H. Spatiotemporal adaptation model for retinal ganglion cells. JOURNAL OF THE OPTICAL SOCIETY OF AMERICA. A, OPTICS, IMAGE SCIENCE, AND VISION 1996; 13:419-435. [PMID: 8627409 DOI: 10.1364/josaa.13.000419] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 05/22/2023]
Abstract
An adaptation model for the level of the ganglion cell in the retina is presented. The model assumes separate adaptation mechanisms for each of the receptive field (RF) regions, i.e., before edge detection. According to the model, the decay in the response time course of each RF region reflects its adaptation process. A mathematical description of adaptation that includes its temporal properties is developed through the change in the semisaturation constant theta in the Naka-Rushton equation. The model and its simulations show a good agreement with a wide variety of physiological studies.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- R Dahari
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Faculty of Engineering, Tel Aviv University, Israel
| | | |
Collapse
|
11
|
Xin D, Seiple W, Holopigian K, Kupersmith MJ. Visual evoked potentials following abrupt contrast changes. Vision Res 1994; 34:2813-21. [PMID: 7975316 DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(94)90050-7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 8] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/28/2023]
Abstract
The timing of visual evoked potential (VEP) amplitude and phase changes following abrupt increases or decreases in contrast was examined. Gratings (1 c/deg) were presented at a low contrast for 8 sec, increased to a higher contrast for 8 sec, and then decreased to the initial lower contrast for another 8 sec. Second harmonic VEP amplitude and phase were recorded continuously and averaged in 1 sec epochs. Both amplitude and phase exhibited delays in reaching a stable level following the contrast change. For amplitude, the length of the delay was dependent on the magnitude and direction of the contrast step and on the spatial frequency of the stimulus. Time constants for the change in amplitude following step increases in contrast ranged from 0.2 sec for a 12% contrast step to 1.34 sec for a 37% contrast step. The timing of phase changes, however, was independent of the size of the contrast increases (tau = 0.7 sec). For step decreases in contrast, both amplitude and phase were relatively independent of the size of the change (tau = approx. 0.9 sec for amplitude and tau = 0.15 sec for phase). Amplitude time constants also increased with increasing spatial frequency (tau = 1.2 sec for 1 c/deg, tau = 1.6 sec for 4 c/deg and tau = 2.3 sec for 8 c/deg); phase time constants, however, did not change as a function of spatial frequency (tau = 0.7 for all spatial frequencies). These findings demonstrate that a unitary process may not always be tapped by signal averaging techniques. Additionally, swept stimulus VEP techniques may produce considerable errors in threshold estimation depending on the stimulus spatial frequency and on the slope and direction of the contrast change.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- D Xin
- Department of Ophthalmology, New York University Medical Center, NY 10016
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
12
|
Troy JB, Oh JK, Enroth-Cugell C. Effect of ambient illumination on the spatial properties of the center and surround of Y-cell receptive fields. Vis Neurosci 1993; 10:753-64. [PMID: 8338811 DOI: 10.1017/s0952523800005447] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/30/2023]
Abstract
The primary goal of this study was to expand the description of the filtering properties of the Y-cell receptive field, by quantitatively characterizing the spatial filtering properties of the receptive field's center-and-surround components as a function of adapting light level. A range of more than five orders of magnitude in retinal illuminance were covered, including the vast majority of the cat's functional range of vision. Recordings were taken from optic tract fibers of Y cells in cats under general anesthesia. Sinusoidal gratings and a stimulus designed to selectively probe the properties of the surround mechanism were used. The cells' responses to these stimuli were fit to a Gaussian center-surround receptive-field model, in which six parameters define the properties of the center and surround. Fits were made independently to data collected at each light level and changes in the values of the model's parameters with illuminance are reported. A set of equations that summarize the changes in parameter values is given. From these summary equations, reasonable estimates of the parameters' values can be determined across a wide range of illuminances. Hence, a quantitative model of the spatial properties of the center and surround of the Y-cell receptive field can now be derived from these equations for most of the levels of retinal illuminance experienced by a Y cell. The consistency between the description provided by our equations and results from earlier work is considered.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- J B Troy
- Department of Biomedical Engineering & Neurobiology, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL 60208
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
13
|
Troy JB, Enroth-Cugell C. X and Y ganglion cells inform the cat's brain about contrast in the retinal image. Exp Brain Res 1993; 93:383-90. [PMID: 8519332 DOI: 10.1007/bf00229354] [Citation(s) in RCA: 52] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/31/2023]
Abstract
It has been suggested for a number of years that ganglion cells inform the rest of the brain about contrast in the retinal image. The purpose of the work undertaken here was to demonstrate this fact explicitly. Extracellular recordings were made from X- and Y-cell axons of the optic tracts of anesthetized cats. Responses of these cells to gratings that were near optimal in spatial and temporal frequency were measured for a range of contrasts. For each cell, similar measurements were made at a number of light levels, spanning the photopic to high scotopic (inclusive) ranges. A monotonic relationship between response and contrast was found at all light levels studied, and the same relationship was retained to a good approximation across all light levels. A similar result was also found when nonoptimal spatial frequencies were used as stimuli. These results indicate strongly that X and Y cells inform the cat's brain about contrast in the retinal image. It was also observed that the mean discharge rate of X and Y cells did not change with light level, indicating that no information is relayed to the brain by these cells on the mean light level.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- J B Troy
- Department of Biomedical Engineering, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL 60208
| | | |
Collapse
|
14
|
Muise JG, LeBlanc RS, Lavoie ME, Arsenault AS. Two-stage model of visual backward masking: sensory transmission and accrual of effective information as a function of target intensity and similarity. PERCEPTION & PSYCHOPHYSICS 1991; 50:197-204. [PMID: 1754360 DOI: 10.3758/bf03206742] [Citation(s) in RCA: 12] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/28/2022]
Abstract
A functional model is used to describe the effect of target intensity and target-set similarity on backward visual masking. The model consists of two distinct stages of visual information processing. The first stage is related to sensory transduction and transmission and is assumed to require a finite and measurable amount of time during which performance remains at chance. The second stage, associated with central processing, is characterized by a negatively accelerated growth function reflecting the accrual of effective information. Results show that the duration of the transmission stage is inversely related to target intensity. Surprisingly, the rate of information accrual is an interactive function of both target intensity and target-set similarity. The pattern of results is consistent with the interpretation that both intensity and similarity mediate their effect through a common mechanism--the accrual of effective information.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- J G Muise
- Département de psychologie, University of Moncton, New Brunswick, Canada
| | | | | | | |
Collapse
|
15
|
Sestokas AK, Lehmkuhle S, Kratz KE. Relationship between response latency and amplitude for ganglion and geniculate X- and Y-cells in the cat. Int J Neurosci 1991; 60:59-64. [PMID: 1774149 DOI: 10.3109/00207459109082037] [Citation(s) in RCA: 6] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/28/2022]
Abstract
This study investigates the relationship between visual response latency and amplitude in the retina and dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus (dLGN) of the anesthetized, paralyzed cat. The discharge rate profiles of retinal ganglion and dLGN X- and Y-cells were measured on a trial by trial basis during repeated stimulation with sinusoidal grating patterns. Latencies of response onsets and peaks were regressed linearly against different measures of response amplitude to determine the extent of covariance. In general, response amplitude was a poor predictor of response latency for both retinal ganglion and geniculate cells. The results suggest that response latency, which changes systematically with stimulus spatial frequency and/or contrast, is not a trivial consequence of discharge rate at either level of the visual system.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- A K Sestokas
- Division of Neurosurgery, Maryland Institute for E.M.S. Systems, Baltimore
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
16
|
Purpura K, Kaplan E, Shapley RM. Background light and the contrast gain of primate P and M retinal ganglion cells. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1988; 85:4534-7. [PMID: 3380804 PMCID: PMC280465 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.85.12.4534] [Citation(s) in RCA: 176] [Impact Index Per Article: 4.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/05/2023] Open
Abstract
Retinal ganglion cells projecting to the monkey lateral geniculate nucleus fall into two classes: those projecting to the magnocellular layers of the nucleus (M cells) have a higher contrast gain to luminance patterns at photopic levels of retinal illumination than those projecting to the parvocellular layers (P cells). We report here that this difference in luminance contrast gain between M and P cells is maintained at low levels of mean retinal illumination. In fact, our results suggest that in the mesopic and scotopic ranges of mean illumination, the M-cell/magnocellular pathway is the predominant conveyor of information about spatial contrast to the visual cortex.
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- K Purpura
- Rockefeller University, New York, NY 10021-6399
| | | | | |
Collapse
|
17
|
Rauschecker JP, Schrader W. Effects of monocular strobe rearing on kitten striate cortex. Exp Brain Res 1987; 68:525-32. [PMID: 3691724 DOI: 10.1007/bf00249796] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/07/2023]
Abstract
Monocular deprivation in kittens does not lead to an ocular dominance shift in striate cortex if the visual stimuli do not contain contours. In the present study we sought to find out whether an ocular dominance shift is produced if the visual environment does contain contours but is devoid of motion. Six kittens were reared with one eye occluded in a visual environment that was lit only by the light of a stroboscope (2 flashes per sec). Exposure was started at 5-6 weeks of age after dark-rearing from birth and extended until 8-12 weeks of age for 8 h per day. The rest of the time was spent in total darkness. Thus, the animals were completely deprived of vision in one eye, while the other eye experienced only stationary flashing contours. Single units in area 17 of these animals were studied and compared to normally reared cats. In all six animals ocular dominance was clearly shifted towards the eye with strobe experience. The ocular dominance shift showed, however, the following interdependencies with other parameters: neurones that responded to stationary flashing test stimuli were nearly always dominated by the strobe eye; neurones that responded only to moving bars or edges remained binocular. In the normal control animals the ocular dominance distribution was similar for both groups of cells. Track analysis according to cortical lamination revealed that neurones in infragranular layers consistently showed a weaker OD shift towards the strobe eye than neurones in supragranular layers (including layer 4).(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 250 WORDS)
Collapse
Affiliation(s)
- J P Rauschecker
- Max-Planck-Institut für biologische Kybernetik, Tübingen, Federal Republic of Germany
| | | |
Collapse
|
18
|
Abstract
A change in responsiveness caused by a spot of light (conditioning spot, CS; 3 sec in duration) presented within a central region of the receptive field of X- and Y-type retinal ganglion cells of the cat was investigated by measuring the magnitude of responses to another spot of light (test spot, TS; 50 msec in duration) which was juxtaposed with the CS within the same receptive field's central region. Responses to the TS were suppressed steadily during the on-phase of the CS as if it were divided by a certain value. This fact indicates that the gain of the center mechanism was changed by the CS presentation. The setting of the gain to a new level was rapid (within 100 msec after the onset or the cessation of the CS), and the magnitude of a gain change was not affected by the surround antagonism. These characteristics of the gain control were common to X- and Y-cells under both mesopic and scotopic levels of light adaptation.
Collapse
|
19
|
Abstract
The brightness of a visually perceived object is mainly determined by the average local contrast around the border between object and background. This fact is demonstrated here with several examples of equiluminant objects on nonuniformly luminant backgrounds. Even in Mondrian-like patterns resembling those used by Land and McCann (1971), equiluminant objects may appear to be of unequal brightness. This result does not agree with predictions of the Retinex Theory. The importance of contrast in vision is also suggested by neurophysiological findings, both classical and recent, that reveal the dependence of visual responses on contrast over most of the visual operating range of mean illumination. The dependence on contrast appears to be the result of retinal gain control mechanisms and is not due to center-surround interaction in the receptive field. We have discovered parallel neural channels with high and low contrast gain in the monkey's visual pathway by means of single unit techniques. Visual evoked potential measurements suggest that similar visual pathways, and with high and low contrast-sensitivity, exist in man and monkey.
Collapse
|
20
|
Copenhagen DR, Green DG. The absence of spread of adaptation between rod photoreceptors in turtle retina. J Physiol 1985; 369:161-81. [PMID: 4093878 PMCID: PMC1192642 DOI: 10.1113/jphysiol.1985.sp015894] [Citation(s) in RCA: 15] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.4] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/08/2023] Open
Abstract
Adaptation by weak backgrounds and the spatial spread of desensitization between rods was studied in the snapping turtle retina, Chelydra serpentina. Intracellular membrane potentials were recorded from these photoreceptors in an eyecup preparation. The kinetics and sensitivity of rod responses were changed significantly by large, very dim backgrounds. For the twenty-five most sensitive rods where the dark-adapted flash sensitivity, SDF, was greater than 1.0 mV/Rh*, Rh* being the number of effective photo-isomerizations per rod, the background intensity required to halve the amplitude of the linear range response averaged 0.21 Rh* s-1. The time-to-peak of the test responses was reduced up to 50% by these dim backgrounds. The desensitizing effects of full field backgrounds of various intensities on the responses to large test spots were measured. The dependence of incremental flash sensitivity, SF, on background intensity, IB, followed the form (FORMULA: SEE TEXT) where I0 is the background intensity which halved SDF. The same intensity dependence held for slit-shaped background fields that desensitized responses to small test spots. The desensitizing effects of large, very dim flashed and continuous backgrounds took several seconds to appear and decay to dark levels. This in conjunction with the sparsity of photons suggests, that the desensitization from a single photoisomerization can persist for several seconds. A comparison of the desensitizing effects of spot and annular backgrounds revealed that small spot backgrounds superimposed on the centered test spots desensitized rods more effectively than annular fields. This finding held true even when annular patterns produced a greater maintained hyperpolarization in the rods. Thus, there was no unique relationship between desensitization and the steady maintained hyperpolarization evoked by a background field. The dependence of adaptation on distance from the impaled rod was determined with slit-shaped background fields placed at different positions across the rod's receptive field. The desensitizing effect of displaced slit stimuli was found to decline much more rapidly with distance than excitation. Displacing the slit by 20 micron from the centre reduced its desensitizing effect by more than 1 log unit. In contrast, excitation fell to about 80% at the same distance (lambda ranging from 50 to 70 micron). The fall off of desensitization with distance matched the calculated fall off with distance of light scatter from a slit. No difference was noted in the kinetics of test responses in the presence of equally desensitizing, superimposed and displaced slits.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 400 WORDS)
Collapse
|
21
|
Hughes HC. Effects of flash luminance and positional expectancies on visual response latency. PERCEPTION & PSYCHOPHYSICS 1984; 36:177-84. [PMID: 6514527 DOI: 10.3758/bf03202678] [Citation(s) in RCA: 65] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/20/2023]
|
22
|
|
23
|
|
24
|
Johnsen JA, Levine MW. Correlation of activity in neighbouring goldfish ganglion cells: relationship between latency and lag. J Physiol 1983; 345:439-49. [PMID: 6663507 PMCID: PMC1193806 DOI: 10.1113/jphysiol.1983.sp014987] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.6] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/21/2023] Open
Abstract
Pairs of retinal ganglion cells in the isolated goldfish retina were recorded simultaneously with a single electrode. Repeated flashes of light were delivered to evaluate the response latency of each of the units. The cross-correlation histogram for the maintained discharge of each pair of cells was examined, and its temporal relationships (lags) were compared with the differences in response latencies of the two units. There was a strong correlation between these measures; however, the differences between latencies were often at least twice as great as the lags. The differences between the times to the peaks of the responses of the two units were less reliably related to the lags of the pairs, although the correlation was positive and the differences in time-to-peak generally greater than the lags. The weaker relationship between the difference in time-to-peak and lag than between latency difference and lag is apparently a manifestation of a negative correlation between latency and rise time (from first response to peak). This indicates that cells with a longer latency compensate with a faster rise time. There was a negative correlation between the mean maintained rate of a neurone and its response latency. That is, cells with faster maintained discharge rates respond sooner than those with slower maintained rates. There was virtually no relationship between the lags or the differences in latency and the differences between the magnitudes of the responses to light. Thus, it is unlikely that differences in latency (or lags) could be attributed to unequal effectiveness of the stimuli for the two units. The relationship between differences in latency and lags did not depend on the response categorizations of the two units. Specifically, it did not matter whether the members of the pair were on centre, off centre or on-off centre; neither did it matter whether they were X-like or not-X-like neurones. Consideration of these data leads to the conclusion that there must be 'marked' pathways of differential conduction velocity through the retina.
Collapse
|
25
|
Cleland BG, Harding TH, Tulunay-Keesey U. Response to the length of moving visual stimuli of the brisk classes of ganglion cells in the cat retina. J Physiol 1983; 345:27-45. [PMID: 6663502 PMCID: PMC1193782 DOI: 10.1113/jphysiol.1983.sp014963] [Citation(s) in RCA: 9] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.2] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/21/2023] Open
Abstract
Response histograms were collected for brisk-sustained and brisk-transient ganglion cells in the cat retina as narrow bars were moved backwards and forwards across their receptive fields. When a bar of fixed length was moved across the centre of the receptive field with contrast proportional to velocity, a constant response was obtained as long as the centre of the receptive field was crossed within the summation time. However, if the length of the bar was such that it extended beyond the centre, then there was a small but steady increase in surround antagonism for an increase in velocity. The same response was produced by a brief whole-field flash as by an extended bar moving across the receptive field at high velocity if both stimulus conditions delivered the same energy uniformly across the receptive field. With brisk-sustained cells it was observed, for small bar lengths, that bar length and contrast could be exchanged to give a constant response, even when there was considerable non-linearity in the over-all stimulus-response relationship. Thus conditions that resulted in constant stimulus flux produced a constant response. This property was seen at both high and low velocities for the majority of brisk-sustained units. The stimulus-response relationship had a greater range of linearity at high velocities than at low velocities. From similar experiments with brisk-transient cells it was observed that bar length and contrast could only be exchanged to give a constant response at high velocities. At low velocities there was considerable non-linearity: there appeared to be saturation of the response from local regions and it was necessary to extend the bar outside such a region to obtain an increase in response. At lower velocities, despite the changes seen in length-response curves under different conditions of contrast and velocity, the degree of surround antagonism remained constant for a given cell. Further, both brisk-sustained and brisk-transient cells showed the same degree of surround antagonism.
Collapse
|
26
|
Cleland BG, Harding TH. Response to the velocity of moving visual stimuli of the brisk classes of ganglion cells in the cat retina. J Physiol 1983; 345:47-63. [PMID: 6663509 PMCID: PMC1193783 DOI: 10.1113/jphysiol.1983.sp014964] [Citation(s) in RCA: 13] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/21/2023] Open
Abstract
Extracellular recording of the responses of cat retinal ganglion cells to narrow moving bars revealed systematic response variations with changes in stimulus velocity. These response variations were studied by collecting peri-stimulus histograms from brisk-sustained (X) and brisk-transient (Y) ganglion cells as narrow elongated bars were moved backwards and forwards across their receptive fields. Velocity-response curves were produced from plots of the amplitude of the main peak of the histograms as a function of velocity. The shape of these curves was found to be reasonably constant for both classes of ganglion cells. For a given cell, the peak of the velocity-response curve shifted to both a higher response level and a higher velocity as the stimulus contrast was increased. Within both classes of cells there was a systematic shift in the velocity-response curve as a function of the size of the receptive field centre. For brisk-sustained cells this was seen as an increase in both the response and velocity at the peak for larger centre sizes, while for brisk-transient cells it was an increase in velocity at the peak with negligible change in response. When the velocity required to produce a small criterion response was determined, there were distinct differences between the two classes of cells. When plotted on a double-logarithmic scale as a function of centre size the brisk-sustained cells had a slope of 2.00 while brisk-transient cells had a slope of 1.20. Within the area centralis brisk-transient cells responded more readily at high velocities than brisk-sustained cells. This was not the case in the peripheral retina, where both cell classes responded about equally at high velocities.
Collapse
|
27
|
Arden GB, Hogg CR. Clinical and experimental evidence that the pattern electroretinogram (PERG) is generated in more proximal retinal layers than the focal electroretinogram (FERG). Ann N Y Acad Sci 1982; 388:580-607. [PMID: 6953889 DOI: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1982.tb50818.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 132] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/22/2023]
Abstract
A TV monitor was used to evoke either a pattern ERG to a contrast-reversing checkerboard (PERG), or a focal ERG to alternate increases and decreases of luminance of the blank screen within a bright surround (FERG). Both responses are small (approx 2 microV) and fast (approx 50 msec to peak) and are similar in several other properties. However, they differ in timing and respond differently to changes in contrast. Each frame of a TV picture evokes a "raster ERG," even though the screen is blank. The response is focal and specific to a small central strip of the screen. It is simpler to record than the FERG, where the whole screen is flashing. Because the FERG summation area is about 4 deg, small squares (checks) reversing in contrast produce little luminance response. In 5 of 7 cases where the PERG is unilaterally reduced, the FERGs or raster responses were not affected. Thus clinical evidence also suggests that the PERG may be a separate phenomenon to the FERG and produced at a different site. Toxic, traumatic, congenital, and degenerative diseases of the optic nerve reduce the PERG. The comparison is most easily made in unilateral disease. Ten weeks after an optic nerve insult, the PERG becomes reduced in the affected eye as if retrograde degeneration was occurring. In 27 amblyopes of various types, the PERG was reduced in 23 where orthoptic treatment had failed. In 4 patients responding to treatment, PERGs of the amblyopic eye were as large as, or larger than, those of the fellow eye. The loss is greater with smaller checks. Retinal changes do occur after age 4 but so slowly that responses in heavily occluded eyes are not reduced. An additional level in the visual pathway is thus accessible to evoked potential investigation.
Collapse
|
28
|
Bolz J, Rosner G, Wässle H. Response latency of brisk-sustained (X) and brisk-transient (Y) cells in the cat retina. J Physiol 1982; 328:171-90. [PMID: 7131312 PMCID: PMC1225652 DOI: 10.1113/jphysiol.1982.sp014258] [Citation(s) in RCA: 76] [Impact Index Per Article: 1.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/23/2023] Open
Abstract
1. Several methods for evaluating light-evoked response latency and its variability in brisk-sustained (X) and brisk-transient (Y) retinal ganglion cells were tested. The most accurate procedure proved to be that described by Levick (1973), in which the time of the occurrence of the fourth impulse after stimulus onset is taken as an estimate of the latency.2. The shortest response latencies are obtained when the stimuli are the same size as the receptive field centre. At medium and high response amplitudes (> 150 impulses/sec) the response of brisk-transient (Y) cells to these optimal stimuli is 10-15 msec faster than that of adjacent brisk-sustained (X) cells.3. The response latency of brisk-sustained (X) cells for stimuli larger than the receptive field centre increases, whereas that of brisk-transient (Y) cells remains constant. Brisk-sustained (X) cells respond faster than do brisk-transient (Y) cells to stimuli smaller than the receptive field centre.4. No systematic difference exists between brisk-sustained (X) and brisk-transient (Y) cells in regard to the temporal variability of the response. The standard deviation of the latency for stimuli of optimal size decreases from 2.0-8.0 msec at medium stimulus contrast to 0.6-2.0 msec at high stimulus contrast.5. The response of OFF-centre cells to the disappearance of a light spot is always slower than that of an ON-centre cell of the same class to the onset of this stimulus. However, when OFF-centre cells are stimulated with dark spots, their response latency does not differ from that of ON-centre cells of the same class.6. No simple relationship exists between the response latency and the response amplitude. At medium and high discharge rates, most brisk-transient (Y) cells respond faster than an adjacent brisk-sustained (X) cell with equal response. At the same response amplitude, the latencies become shorter as the background illumination is raised. The same discharge rate can be obtained with stimuli of sub-optimal and supra-optimal size, but the latency for the larger stimulus is shorter than that for the smaller one. Latency, therefore, is an additional parameter characterizing the light-evoked response.
Collapse
|
29
|
Bonds AB, Enroth-Cugell C. Spatial consequences of bleaching adaptation in cat retinal ganglion cells. J Physiol 1981; 318:339-54. [PMID: 7320894 PMCID: PMC1245495 DOI: 10.1113/jphysiol.1981.sp013868] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Grants] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/24/2023] Open
Abstract
1. Experiments were conducted to study the effects of localized bleaching on the centre responses of rod-driven cat retinal ganglion cells. 2. Stimulation as far as 2 degrees from the bleaching site yielded responses which were reduced nearly as much as those generated at the bleaching site. Bleaching in the receptive field middle reduced responsiveness at a site 1 degrees peripheral more than bleaching at that peripheral site itself. 3. The effectiveness of a bleach in reducing centre responsiveness is related to the sensitivity of the region in which the bleach is applied. 4. Response reduction after a 0.2 degree bleach followed the same temporal pattern for concentric test spots of from 0.2 to 1.8 degrees in diameter, implying a substantially uniform spread of adaptation within these bounds. 5. A linear trade-off between fraction of rhodopsin and area bleached over a range of 8:1 yields the same pattern of response reduction, implying that the non-linear nature of bleaching adaptation is a property of the adaptation pool rather than independent photoreceptors.
Collapse
|
30
|
Abstract
1. Action potentials were recorded from single fibres in the optic tract of anaesthetized cats. 2. A sectored disk or 'windmill', concentric with the receptive field, was rotated about its centre to cause local changes in illumination throughout the receptive field without changing the total amount of light falling on the receptive field centre or surround. 3. A cell's response to a flashing test spot centered on its receptive field was measured both while the windmill was stationary and while it rotated. While the windmill rotated, the test spot evoked a smaller average number of spikes than while the windmill was stationary. 4. The induction in response occurred in both on-centre and off-centre cells and in both X-cells and Y-cells, though the reduction in response was smaller in X-cells. 5. Surround responses, evoked by an eccentric stimulus, were also reduced by a moving peripheral pattern. 6. Suppression was graded with the contrast of the moving pattern. 7. Gratings too fine to be resolved by the receptive field centre could suppress the response of Y-cells. This suggests that the local elements responsible for the suppression are smaller than the receptive field centres of Y-cells. 8. Response suppression started within the 100 msec of the onset of pattern motion.
Collapse
|
31
|
Arden GB, Vaegan, Hogg CR. CLINICAL AND EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE THAT THE PATTERN ELECTRORETINOGRAM (PERG) IS GENERATED IN MORE PROXIMAL RETINAL LAYERS THAN THE FOCAL ELECTRORETINOGRAM (FERG). Ann N Y Acad Sci 1980. [DOI: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1980.tb19383.x] [Citation(s) in RCA: 3] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 11/27/2022]
|
32
|
Christen WG, Cohen HI, Robertson TW, Winters RW. Spatial distribution of the adaptation field of the surround response mechanisms in type X cat retinal ganglion cells. EXPERIENTIA 1979; 35:1073-4. [PMID: 477882 DOI: 10.1007/bf01949949] [Citation(s) in RCA: 0] [Impact Index Per Article: 0] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/15/2022]
Abstract
The surround response mechanism in on-center X-cells in cat retina was found to be bimodally distributed and weak or nonexistent in the receptive field middle. An on-inhibition measure was used to assess surround mechanism gain.
Collapse
|
33
|
Shapley RM, Victor JD. The effect of contrast on the transfer properties of cat retinal ganglion cells. J Physiol 1978; 285:275-98. [PMID: 745079 PMCID: PMC1281756 DOI: 10.1113/jphysiol.1978.sp012571] [Citation(s) in RCA: 391] [Impact Index Per Article: 8.3] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/24/2022] Open
Abstract
1. Variation in stimulus contrast produces a marked effect on the dynamics of the cat retina. This contrast effect was investigated by measurement of the responses of X and Y ganglion cells. The stimuli were sine gratings or rectangular spots modulated by a temporal signal which was a sum of sinusoids. Fourier analysis of the neural response to such a stimulus allowed us to calculate first order and second order frequency kernels. 2. The first order frequency kernel of both X and Y ganglion cells became more sharply tuned at higher contrasts. The peak amplitude also shifted to higher temporal frequency at higher contrasts. Responses to low frequencies of modulation (less than 1 Hz) grew less than proportionally with contrast. However, response amplitudes at higher modulation frequencies (greater than 4 Hz) scaled approximately proportionally with contrast. Also, there was a marked phase advance in these latter components as contrast increased. 3. The contrast effect was significantly larger for Y cells than for X cells. 4. The first order frequency kernel was measured with single sine waves as well as with the sum of sinusoids as a modulation signal. The transfer function measured in this way was much less affected by increases in contrast. This implied that stimulus energy at one temporal frequency could affect the response amplitude and phase shift at another temporal frequency. 5. Direct proof was found that modulation at one frequency modifies the response at other frequencies. This was demonstrated by perturbation experiments in which the modulation stimulus was the sum of one strong perturbing sinusoid and seven weak test sinusoids. 6. The shape of the graph of the amplitude of the first order frequency kernel vs. temporal frequency did not depend on the amplitudes of the first order components, but rather on local retinal contrast. This was shown in an experiment with a sine grating placed at different positions in the visual field. The shape of the first order kernel did not vary with spatial phase, while the magnitudes of the first order responses varied greatly with spatial phase. 7. Models for the contrast gain control mechanism are considered in the Discussion.
Collapse
|
34
|
Abstract
1. The discharges of ganglion cells in the cat's retina were recorded under conditions intended to isolate the cone system.2. Stiles' two-colour threshold technique permitted the photopic system to be studied when at its highest sensitivity. The absolute sensitivity of a ganglion cell, expressed in equivalent photons of lambda(max) at the cornea per impulse discharged, was about 2500 times less when driven by cones than when driven by rods. This ratio improves to around 200 when allowance is made for the much smaller fraction absorbed by cones of photons incident on the cornea.3. The number of extra impulses discharged in response to a brief flash was approximately proportional to the number of photons in the flash, up to a limit.4. There was a region in the middle of the receptive field within which the area of a test spot and its illumination for threshold varied inversely. A flash extending over the peripheral part of the receptive field raised threshold above its minimum, presumably as a result of surround antagonism. Assessed from area-threshold curves, the balance of centre-surround antagonism in the photopic receptive field did not seem to depend upon background illumination.5. The threshold for a small (0.2 degrees ) flash confined to the middle of the receptive field was independent of background illumination until the background exceeded a particular level, the ;dark light' (I(o)). In different units this ranged about a mean of 7.89 log photons (560 nm equivalent) deg(-2) sec(-1). For backgrounds that exceeded I(o), threshold followed approximately Weber's law up to the highest illuminations that could be produced.6. With test flashes that filled the centre of the receptive field, the Weber fraction (test flash illumination/background illumination) in some units fell below 1%.7. Changes in the time course and latency of response accompanied the changes in sensitivity caused by alterations in background illumination. Responses of both X- and Y-cells became more transient and faster.8. The loss of sensitivity to a test flash brought about by a steady background light depended upon the size of that light. Sensitivity varied inversely with background area within a central region that matched closely the summing area for test flashes.
Collapse
|
35
|
Jakiela HG, Enroth-Cugell C. Adaptation and dynamics in X-cells and Y-cells of the cat retina. Exp Brain Res 1976; 24:335-42. [PMID: 1261621 DOI: 10.1007/bf00235001] [Citation(s) in RCA: 39] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.8] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/26/2022]
Abstract
On the basis of the spatial summation properties of their receptive fields, cat retinal ganglion cells were classified as either X-cells (linear) or Y-cells (non-linear). Responses were then obtained to a small, centered spot, square-wave modulated in time and superimposed on various levels of diffuse, steady background illumination. When fully dark-adapted, both X-cells and Y-cells produced responses that were entirely sustained. When well light-adapted but still in the scotopic range, both cell types produced largely transient responses with only a very small sustained component. The sustained or transient nature of responses is, therefore, not an invariant characteristic of X-cells and Y-cells in the scotopic range. We also conclude that the mechanism which controls the center's sensitivity in the scotopic range is similar though not identical in the two types of cells.
Collapse
|
36
|
|
37
|
Abstract
The results from the preliminary set of experiments in which a new video sampling apparatus was used are reported. With the aid of this apparatus experiments were carried out to measure the maximum visual temporal integration time (critical duration) at various background intensities (0·034–34 cd m−2). The aim was to determine to what extent this phenomenon is attributable to either ‘central’ or ‘peripheral’ events. The extended integration period found for the number recognition task is interpreted as evidence of a ‘central’ process; to follow the argument further, an attempt was made to demonstrate information integration using a rotating form in a similar identification–discrimination situation. Monocular, binocular, and dichoptic arrangements were employed, and the amount of dichoptic summation of form information, achieved by both normal and strabismic subjects without stereoscopic depth perception, was used to test two theoretical models of binocular fusion. In addition, stereoscopic depth was generated with uncorrected sampling of the left and right images, which may be due to the action of a ‘fusion hierarchy’. Signal detection theory is suggested as a possible solution to the problem of expectation effects in identification-threshold experiments.
Collapse
|
38
|
Abstract
1. The impulse/quantum (I/Q) ratio was measured as a function of background illumination for rod-dominated, pure central, linear square-wave responses of retinal ganglion cells in the cat.2. The I/Q ratio was constant at low backgrounds (dark adapted state) and inversely proportional to the 0.9 power of the background at high backgrounds (the light adapted state). There was an abrupt transition from the dark-adapted state to the light-adapted state.3. It was possible to define the adaptation level at a particular background as the ratio (I/Q ratio at that background)/(dark adapted I/Q ratio).4. The time course of the square-wave response was correlated with the adaptation level. The response was sustained in the dark-adapted state, partially transient at the transition level, and progressively more transient the lower the impulse/quantum ratio of the ganglion cell became. This was true both for on-centre and off-centre cells.5. The frequency response of the central response mechanism at different adaptation levels was measured. It was a low-pass characteristic in the dark-adapted state and became progressively more of a bandpass characteristic as the cell became more light-adapted.6. The rapidity of onset of adaptation was measured with a time-varying adapting light. The impulse/quantum ratio is reset within 100 msec of the onset of the conditioning light, and is kept at the new value throughout the time the conditioning light is on.7. These results can be explained by a nonlinear feedback model. In the model, it is postulated that the exponential function of the horizontal cell potential controls transmission from rods to bipolars. This model has an abrupt transition from dark- to light-adapted states, and its response dynamics are correlated with adaptation level.
Collapse
|
39
|
Enroth-Cugell C, Shapley RM. Flux, not retinal illumination, is what cat retinal ganglion cells really care about. J Physiol 1973; 233:311-26. [PMID: 4747230 PMCID: PMC1350568 DOI: 10.1113/jphysiol.1973.sp010309] [Citation(s) in RCA: 111] [Impact Index Per Article: 2.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/12/2023] Open
Abstract
1. Evidence was obtained that the impulse/quantum (I/Q) ratio of the central response mechanism of retinal ganglion cells in the cat is controlled by the steady effective retinal flux of the background.2. One experiment revealed that the I/Q ratio was decreased as the area of an adapting spot, of constant illumination, was increased. The curve relating the I/Q ratio to background flux was the same regardless of the size of the adapting spot.3. The effective central summing area of many retinal ganglion cells was determined. For the same cells, the transition level (Enroth-Cugell & Shapley, 1973) of the impulse/quantum curve was also measured. Diffuse illumination at the transition level was inversely proportional to the effective summing area, when variation in dark-adapted sensitivity between cells was taken into account.4. Therefore, retinal ganglion cells with large central summing areas are more light-adapted by any given diffuse background than cells with small centres.
Collapse
|
40
|
Hickey TL, Winters RW, Pollack JG. Center-surround interactions in two types of on-center retinal ganglion cells in the cat. Vision Res 1973; 13:1511-26. [PMID: 4719083 DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(73)90010-2] [Citation(s) in RCA: 34] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.7] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/12/2023]
|
41
|
|
42
|
|
43
|
|
44
|
Van De Grind WA, Grüsser OJ, Lunkenheimer HU. Temporal Transfer Properties of the Afferent Visual System Psychophysical,Neurophysiological and Theoretical Investigations. CENTRAL PROCESSING OF VISUAL INFORMATION A: INTEGRATIVE FUNCTIONS AND COMPARATIVE DATA 1973. [DOI: 10.1007/978-3-642-65352-0_7] [Citation(s) in RCA: 24] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/02/2022]
|
45
|
Jung R. Visual Perception and Neurophysiology. CENTRAL PROCESSING OF VISUAL INFORMATION A: INTEGRATIVE FUNCTIONS AND COMPARATIVE DATA 1973. [DOI: 10.1007/978-3-642-65352-0_1] [Citation(s) in RCA: 27] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Track Full Text] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 12/20/2022]
|
46
|
Ikeda H, Wright MJ. Receptive field organization of 'sustained' and 'transient' retinal ganglion cells which subserve different function roles. J Physiol 1972; 227:769-800. [PMID: 4654257 PMCID: PMC1331286 DOI: 10.1113/jphysiol.1972.sp010058] [Citation(s) in RCA: 188] [Impact Index Per Article: 3.5] [Reference Citation Analysis] [Abstract] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/11/2023] Open
Abstract
1. Post-stimulus histograms were obtained from ;sustained' and ;transient' retinal ganglion cells for receptive field plots using a light spot with square-wave modulation of intensity, and of variable intensity and area. Fundamental differences in their receptive field organization in time and space were revealed.2. In ;sustained' cells, excitation consists of ;transient' and ;sustained' components and the ratio of transient/sustained components remains constant at a given retinal locus for a wide range of intensities. The transient component becomes proportionally larger towards the periphery of the receptive field. This rule is also applicable for the inhibitory and disinhibitory surround. In ;transient' cells, however, there is no true ;sustained' component, but some cells produce a double peaked transient post-stimulus histogram at the R.F. centre when high flux stimuli are used, while others show a single peak transient response. The magnitude and shape of transient responses changes with intensity as well as with location in the receptive field.3. The sensitivity gradients of ;sustained' and ;transient' cells show consistent differences in shape. The mean slope of the sensitivity gradients of a sample of ;sustained' cells was 10 times that of a sample of ;transient' cells. The sensitivity gradient of ;sustained' cells shows a distinct surround region where the inhibitory mechanism is more sensitive, while that of ;transient' cells usually does not, owing to an extensive ;tail' on the sensitivity gradient of the centre mechanism, which overlaps the surround.4. Ricco's Law also holds for the centre mechanism of ;transient' cells. Non-linear summation occurs at supra-threshold levels, and when the surround mechanisms are involved.5. Supra-optimal stimuli give a saturation of the response in both ;transient and ;sustained' cells. This saturation is associated with a decrease of latency in ;transient' cells, but not in ;sustained' cells.6. The latency of retinal ganglion cells is determined by both stimulus and background flux. The effect of the background is negligible except at low values of stimulus flux, where its effect may be analysed primarily in terms of its effect on the incremental threshold.7. The latency to stimulation with a standard small spot (25-27') at the receptive field centre is shorter for ;sustained' cells than for ;transient' cells; this latency difference being related to the greater sensitivity of the ;sustained' cells to stimuli of this size. Differences in conduction time along ;transient' and ;sustained' pathways to the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) and cortex were estimated, and it is concluded that despite the latency difference noted above, a response to a stimulus which is optimal for a ;transient' cell reaches the cortex faster than the response to a stimulus which is optimal for a ;sustained' cell.8. The above results together with previous evidence available suggest that for most stimuli, centre and surround mechanisms are activated simultaneously and algebraically summed by a single linear stage in ;sustained' cells. In ;transient' cells, although the centre excitation and surround inhibition pools are also spatially co-extensive, they summate and interact in time and space with a greater complexity.9. Differences in the receptive field organization of ;sustained' and ;transient' cells may reflect their different functional roles in vision: (1) analysis of spatial contrast and form recognition (;sustained' cells), and (2) fast detection of objects entering visual space to cause orientation responses (;transient' cells).
Collapse
|
47
|
Freund HJ, Wita CW, Brüstle R. Latency differences between inhibitory and excitatory responses of cat optic tract units. Exp Brain Res 1972; 16:60-74. [PMID: 4646541 DOI: 10.1007/bf00233374] [Citation(s) in RCA: 5] [Impact Index Per Article: 0.1] [Reference Citation Analysis] [MESH Headings] [Track Full Text] [Journal Information] [Subscribe] [Scholar Register] [Indexed: 01/11/2023]
|
48
|
|
49
|
Abstract
1. There is an outer disinhibitory zone surrounding the classical inhibitory surround of the retinal ganglion cell receptive field.2. The disinhibitory surround is strong and narrow in ;sustained' cells but weak and laterally spread in ;transient' cells.3. The disinhibitory surround can be demonstrated using a black spot as a probing stimulus as well as by a white spot, and is therefore not an artifact of scattered light.4. Stimulation with a light spot in the disinhibitory zone gives an increase in firing to ;stimulus on' in on-centre cells and to ;stimulus off' in off-centre cells.5. The disinhibitory surround may be revealed by plotting the latency of the first spike discharge following stimulation against position in the receptive field. The disinhibitory zone shows a decrease in latency to the centre-type stimulus.6. The disinhibitory surround may be revealed by plotting the threshold intensity of a spot against position in the receptive field. It is thus a feature of the sensitivity gradients of both ;transient' and ;sustained' cells.7. Using two spots, one at the centre of the receptive field and the other at varying distances from the receptive field centre, dynamic interactions between the centre, inhibitory and disinhibitory zones are demonstrated. A spot presented in the disinhibitory zone causes an enhancement of the centre response when flashing in phase with the centre spot, while it causes inhibition of the centre response when presented 180 degrees out of phase.8. A scheme for the anatomical basis of the disinhibitory surround is proposed, and the relation of disinhibition to the spatial transfer characteristics of the visual pathways is discussed.
Collapse
|
50
|
|